Objectives
To examine cognitive abilities of low-income preschoolers with uncorrected ametropia and effects of spectacle correction.
Methods
Baseline and 6-week data from a longitudinal controlled study were analyzed. Subjects were 70 preschoolers (mean age, 4.6 years; 60.0% were female; and 85.7% were Latino), including 35 children with previously uncorrected ametropia and 35 emmetropic control subjects. Ametropia was defined as bilateral hyperopia of 4.00 diopters (D) or more in children aged 3 to 5 years, astigmatism of 2.00 D or more in children aged 3 years and 1.50 D or more in children aged 4 and 5 years, or a combination of both. Emmetropia was defined as 2.00 sphere diopters or less and 1.00 cylinder diopter or less in both eyes. Ametropes were assessed before and 6 weeks after correction and compared with control subjects. Primary and secondary outcome measures were Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration and Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised scores, respectively.
Results
At baseline, uncorrected ametropes scored significantly lower on the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (P = .005) and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised performance scale (P = .01). After 6 weeks of correction, the ametropic group significantly improved on the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration compared with emmetropic control subjects (P = .02).
Conclusions
Preschoolers with uncorrected ametropia had significant reduction in visual-motor function. Wearing spectacles for 6 weeks improved Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration scores to emmetropic levels.
Although there have been studies examining the relationship of refractive errors and academic performance in elementary school children,1-5few have looked at ametropia and cognitive abilities in younger children. In elementary school children, one study1found no relation between refractive errors and scores on tests of academic achievement and reading, whereas others2-7found associations between lower academic achievement scores and hyperopia of 1.25 to 3.00 diopters (D) but not myopia. Younger children (aged 9 months to 5.5 years) with hyperopia of 3.50 D or greater in at least 1 axis were found to have modest but consistent reductions in scores on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children involving visual-motor function.8,9
We conducted a study to add to previous research in preschool children8,9and to determine the relevance for clinical practice in the care of preschool children by assessing the cognitive abilities of ametropic children aged 3 to 5 years with a battery of widely used tests that are standardized, age-normed, normalized developmental measures of cognitive abilities and are predictive of academic achievement.10,11Scores on tests performed by children before and 6 weeks after wearing optical correction were compared with those of control subjects assessed at comparable times.
The protocol for this longitudinal controlled study was approved by the University of California, San Diego Human Research Protections Program Institutional Review Board as well as the Human Subjects Research Committees of the San Diego Unified School District and the Neighborhood House Association Head Start. Written informed parental consent was obtained.
The study was designed to determine the cognitive abilities of ametropic children before and 6 weeks after wearing corrective lenses in comparison with those of group-matched emmetropic control subjects assessed at the comparable times. The main comparison was between the change on cognitive test scores in the ametropic group before and 6 weeks after wearing optical correction and that in the emmetropic control group.
Participants in this study were recruited from children seen sequentially on the mobile eye clinic of the University of California, San Diego that serves preschool children attending Head Start (federally funded) and the San Diego Unified School District (state funded). Children in both programs are required to be from low-income families. The curriculum is determined by the preschool administration and is similarly standardized with structured classroom activities. Both programs often use the same facilities. The 70 subjects were enrolled between January 21, 2003, and May 24, 2006. Inclusion criteria for the ametropic group were uncorrected bilateral hyperopia of 4.00 D or more in children aged 3 to 5 years, astigmatism of 2.00 D or more in children aged 3 years and 1.50 D or more in children aged 4 to 5 years, or a combination of both. Inclusion criteria for emmetropes were bilateral refractive error of 2.00 sphere diopters or less and 1.00 cylinder diopter or less in both eyes. All of the subjects also met the following criteria: (1) had no other eye abnormalities (eg, did not have strabismus, amblyopia [defined as a 2-line visual acuity difference between eyes], cataract, or glaucoma and did not previously wear glasses); (2) had no developmental problems, eg, autism, hearing loss, cerebral palsy, or mental retardation on preschool health records; (3) were aged 3, 4, or 5 years; and (4) used English or Spanish as the primary language.
Overall, there were 81 eligible children. Of these, 5 had parents who declined and 6 moved. Nonparticipants were not significantly different from participants in demographic and vision characteristics (P > .05). The sample comprised 35 subjects with previously uncorrected ametropia and 35 emmetropic subjects.
Refraction, Prescription of Glasses, and Compliance
Eye examinations were performed by optometrists in the department's mobile eye clinic. Cycloplegia was induced with a combination of phenylephrine hydrochloride (1.6%), tropicamide (0.167%), and cyclopentolate hydrochloride (1.3%).12After waiting a minimum of 30 minutes, all of the children received retinoscopy under cycloplegia and most had autorefraction (Nikon Retinomax K-plus 2; Nikon, Melville, New York) and manifest refraction. The lowest value of refractive error on any test was used to meet study criteria. Visual acuity was assessed before correction prior to cycloplegia and after correction under cycloplegia at near using the Allen Preschool Vision Test and at far using B-VAT PC version 2.3 software (Medtronic Solan, Jacksonville, Florida).
When glasses were prescribed, full astigmatic errors were corrected and hyperopic refractive errors were undercorrected by 1.50 D to 2.50 D or by 3.00 D if the hyperopic component was 7.00 D or more.13,14Children's compliance to wearing glasses was monitored 3 weeks after receiving glasses using a questionnaire completed by parents. Compliance was assessed on the basis of answers to the question “Does your child wear his/her glasses?” on a scale of always, most of the time, sometimes, and never. A child reported as wearing glasses at least most of the time was considered compliant.
Tests of Cognitive Abilities and Behavior
Bilingual English- and Spanish-speaking psychometrists performed developmental assessments at 2 points at the child's preschool or home in accordance with standard highly structured methods.10,11,15Baseline assessments were conducted an average of 2 weeks after the eye examination. At the end of developmental testing, the prescribed glasses were then given to the child. The second assessment was performed 6 weeks after wearing corrective lenses for ametropic children and at a comparable interval for control subjects.
The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration10(VMI) is a nonverbal, standardized, age-normed test of visual-motor integration widely used in assessment of visual perception and eye-hand coordination in children aged 3 to 7 years. The child is required to copy 18 large geometric figures in a sequence of increasing difficulty (eg, from drawing lines, to closed figures [eg, circles and squares], to embedded figures [eg, 3 overlapping circles], to joined figures [eg, a square touching a circle]). The child must satisfy all of the criteria exactly as specified in the manual to receive a score of 1 point. Possible scores range from 45 to 155, with higher scores indicating higher performance. The VMI has been found to be free of cultural biases and to be predictive of school achievement, especially in children from low socioeconomic groups.10
The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised11(WPPSI-R) is a widely used standardized developmental test of cognitive abilities in children aged 3 to 7 years. It is age normed, ie, raw scores are transformed into equivalent scaled scores on the basis of norms defined for each age range. This allows for comparison of children's scores at different ages and times. Possible scores range from 41 to 160, with higher scores indicating higher cognitive abilities (mean [SD] standardized score, 100 [15]). Credit is given for each item only when the child's answer matches the correct answers listed in the WPPSI-R manual. The WPPSI-R full scale comprises verbal and performance scales (Table 1). The verbal scale contains information, comprehension, arithmetic, vocabulary, similarities, and sentences subtests that are free from sensorimotor demand. Performance scale subtests are object assembly, geometric design, block design, mazes, picture completion, and animal pegs. Some degree of eye-hand coordination is required for performance scale subtests except for picture completion, a 28-item subtest for which only vision is required to identify the missing component in a picture. Each WPPSI-R subtest has a mean (SD) scaled score of 10 (3).11Performance on the WPPSI-R is strongly correlated with intellectual abilities as measured by other tests and academic achievement, especially reading abilities.11The WPPSI-R has been successfully translated into Spanish for research in investigations in Latin America.15For this study, the WPPSI-R was translated by a Spanish-speaking clinical psychologist (Brian Lee Ford, PhD) experienced in working with the study population.
Both the VMI and the WPPSI-R use materials (ie, geometric figures, images, and objects) that are large, high contrast, and/or brightly colored.
The Child Behavior Checklist 1.5-5 English/Spanish16(CBCL) is completed by parents and is widely used in clinical research to assess behavioral problems. Referral to a mental health professional is recommended at a standard score of 65.
Demographic and family health information came from standard medical questionnaires completed by parents.
Statistical analyses were conducted using Statistica for Windows version 6.1 statistical software (Statsoft, Tulsa, Oklahoma). Demographic and health characteristics listed in Table 2were considered as potential confounding variables and were assessed as possible covariates. Continuous variables were compared using 2-sample ttests or using Wilcoxon rank sum tests if normality and homogeneity of variance assumptions were not met. Discrete variables were compared using χ2analysis and Fisher exact test. If differences between groups were detected and factors were found to be associated with the outcome variables, confounders were included as covariates in an analysis of covariance model.
The VMI score was the primary outcome measure. Secondary outcome measures were the WPPSI-R and CBCL scores. A general linear model was applied on each outcome measure to allow for inclusion of covariates in the analysis. If no significant covariate was found, repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed on overall standardized test scores of outcome measures with 1 within-subject factor, time (2 levels: baseline and reassessment), and 1 between-subject factor, group (2 levels: ametropia with correction vs emmetropia). The time × group interaction was used to compare slopes or change in scores between ametropic and emmetropic groups. Group × subtests multivariate ANOVAs on subtests of the WPPSI-R performance and verbal scales were performed. If assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were not met for each variable, a nonparametric test, ie, Mann-Whitney rank sum test, was performed on the difference scores. Comparisons with normed values were performed with 1-sample ttests.
Associations between ophthalmic measures and developmental outcome variables, ie, VMI, WPPSI-R, and CBCL scores, were examined using Pearson ror using Spearman ρ if assumption of normality was not met. Analyses were reported only on visual acuity before correction prior to cycloplegia at far distance because only this measure detected a difference between ametropes and emmetropes. Refractive errors were transformed based on a Fourier analysis into a power vector representation of spherocylinders to provide 1 value for statistical analyses that captures interactions between the 3 components of refractive error.17-20The statistical significance level was set at P = .05. With the sample size of 70 subjects, the power was 0.61 to detect the observed effect size of 0.55, a standardized difference of 6 points on the primary outcome measure, ie, the VMI score.
The sample comprised 70 (35 ametropic and 35 emmetropic) low-income children enrolled in Head Start and public preschool (Table 2). The mean age was 4.6 years; 60.0% were female; and 85.7% were Latino. There were no differences between the ametropic and emmetropic groups in demographic or medical characteristics (Fisher exact test, 2-tailed P > .05). No demographic or medical characteristics were associated with outcome variables (P > .05). Therefore, no covariate was included in the ANOVA model. Other factors not reported in Table 2, ie, prematurity, marital status of parents, place of testing, family mental health history, and interval were balanced between the 2 groups and not associated with test scores (P ≥ .63).
Comparisons between the clinical characteristics of the ametropic and emmetopic groups and among the subtypes of refractive errors in the ametropic group revealed differences between groups and among subtypes as shown in Table 3and Table 4.
Values from cycloplegic retinoscopy and autorefraction were highly correlated (Spearman ρ ≥ 0.71; P < .001). Correlations were highly significant (P ≤ .001) between the amount of refractive error expressed in sphere and cylinder diopters, the power vector representation of spherocylinders, and the visual acuity at far distance before correction prior to cycloplegia expressed in logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution in the best eye (ie, the eye with the lowest amount of refractive error).
At baseline and 6-week reassessment, there were significant correlations between scores on the VMI and WPPSI-R full scale as well as the WPPSI-R performance scale (r70 ≥ 0.51; P < .001 for the entire sample). Scores on the VMI were moderately correlated with results on WPPSI-R verbal scale at baseline (r70 = 0.28; P = .02) but not at 6-week reassessment (r70 = 0.19; P = .11).
The 35 ametropic children had worn their glasses always or most of the time for the 6-week period. Five children broke or lost their glasses and subsequently had them replaced within 1 week.
At baseline before optical correction, children with ametropia scored significantly lower on the VMI (P = .005; mean difference score, 8.6; 95% confidence interval, 6.2-11.0) (Table 5). The mean score of the ametropic group on the VMI was significantly lower than the norm value (P = .009), whereas the mean score of the emmetropic group was not (P = .19). There was no correlation between scores on the VMI and visual acuity tested at near and far prior to cycloplegia and correction (P ≥ .44).
After wearing glasses for 6 weeks, there was a significant interaction on the VMI (P = .02), showing that the ametropic group improved by a mean of 6.0 points (95% confidence interval, 4.2-7.8) from baseline to 6-week reassessment compared with 0.0 points for the emmetropic group and reached the level of performance of the emmetropic control group (Table 5). There was a weak correlation between change scores on the VMI and the amount of refractive error expressed in sphere diopters and spherocylindrical power vectors of the best eye (Spearman ρ70 = 0.24, P = .06; and Spearman ρ70 = 0.27, P = .04, respectively). There was no correlation with visual acuity at near or far distance (P ≥ .33).
At baseline, children with ametropia also scored significantly lower on the WPPSI-R performance scale (P = .01; mean difference score, 7.0; 95% confidence interval, 4.9-9.1). A group × subtests multivariate ANOVA comparing the scores of the ametropic group with those of emmetropic control subjects on the WPPSI-R performance scale subtests as the repeated-measure variable yielded a main effect of status (F1,68 = 10.3, P = .002), indicating that the ametropic group scored significantly lower on the subtests than the emmetropic control subjects (mean [SD] scores, 9.8 [2.8] vs 11.0 [2.3], respectively). The mean subtest scores of the ametropic group were below norm values, whereas those of the control subjects were not (Table 5).
A main effect of subtest (F5,330 = 18.7; P < .001) showed that both the ametropic and emmetropic groups had similar scores on the 1 subtest in which only vision but not eye-hand coordination was involved, ie, picture completion. Scores on this subtest for both groups were above the norm value (Table 5).
There was no difference between the ametropic and control groups on the WPPSI-R verbal scale (P = .56) or its subtests (P = .39) (Table 5).
After wearing optical correction for 6 weeks, the ametropic group improved on the WPPSI-R performance scale, bringing their scores to the baseline level of the control group (Table 5), but the amount of change between groups did not reach statistical significance (P = .17). Across performance scale subtests, a multivariate ANOVA revealed a trending difference (P = .10), indicating that the ametropic group improved more than the emmetropic group (mean [SD] scores, 6.5 [5.9] vs 4.3 [5.3], respectively). Repeated-measures ANOVAs on the WPPSI-R verbal scale and multivariate ANOVAs on its subtests revealed no significant differences in change scores between the ametropic and emmetropic groups (P ≥ .95) (Table 5).
Correlational analyses revealed no association between change on the VMI score and change on the WPPSI-R performance and verbal scale scores in the ametropic or emmetropic groups (P ≥ .11). However, for children who performed under the norm value (ie, VMI score < 100) at baseline, there was a correlation between change on the VMI and verbal scale scores for the corrected ametropic group (r22 = 0.42; P = .05) but not for the control group (r14 = 0.009; P = .98). This suggests that improvement in visual-motor coordination scores after wearing corrective lenses may be associated with some improvement in scores on verbal abilities in children who performed below the norm value on the VMI.
There were no differences at baseline and at 6 weeks after wearing corrective lenses between the ametropic and emmetropic groups on the CBCL (P = .96). Average standard scores were in the normal range for both groups at both times16(Table 5).
The baseline results showed that low-income preschool children with uncorrected ametropia as defined in this study had significantly reduced scores on standardized tests involving visual-motor integration skills when compared with group-matched emmetropic control subjects.Specifically, these alterations were found on the VMI and most of the WPPSI-R performance subtests requiring eye-hand coordination. The reduction of scores on the WPPSI-R performance scale in the ametropic group accounted for their lower scores on the full scale of the WPPSI-R.
Because our subjects were tested using the VMI and the WPPSI-R, which are widely used, normalized, standardized measures with strong psychometric properties, it is possible to compare cognitive alterations of the children with ametropia with other conditions that affect young children's cognitive abilities. Reduced test scores in the ametropic group are comparable to those found in studies of preschool-aged children affected by nutritional deficiencies,15high blood lead concentrations,21,22and low birth weight and prematurity.23-25Furthermore, because reduced scores on the VMI and the WPPSI-R found in this study are at levels that have been shown to predict lower levels of academic achievement,10,11,15,26-28ametropia may be a risk factor for academic difficulties.
Since this study was limited to low-income children in standardized structured preschool programs, economic status and type of preschool instruction did not confound the findings. It is not known whether these findings would apply to all populations of preschool children affected by ametropia.
There are at least 3 possible explanations for reduced baseline performance in visual-motor integration skills of the preschoolers with ametropia. The first is that without spectacle correction, the children may not see testing materials. This possibility is unlikely because the tests use large, high-contrast materials. Moreover, the ametropic group did as well as the control group on the subtest that required only vision and no eye-hand coordination, ie, picture completion. A second possible explanation is that ametropia may be a risk factor for a neural alteration not yet understood.8,9Finally, ametropia may make near tasks uncomfortable, interfering with development of visual-motor coordination skills in these preschoolers.29
Our study also showed that after wearing glasses for 6 weeks, the ametropic group improved on the VMI to the level of the emmetropic control subjects, with the differences in scores between the ametropic and emmetropic groups no longer being statistically significant.Although the sample size was small, the magnitude of this change demonstrated that wearing glasses for 6 weeks improved the ametropic group to the VMI norm level. The ametropic group also improved on the WPPSI-R performance scale, but the amount of change did not reach statistical significance. Whereas VMI assesses only visual-motor skills, the WPPSI-R performance scale comprises measures that include not only visual-motor coordination but also spatial analysis and planning skills. Consequently, the WPPSI-R performance scale may not be as sensitive as the VMI to changes in visual-motor integration.
Improvement on visual-motor scores in the corrected ametropic group disagrees with the findings by Atkinson et al,8,9who reported that children with glasses30did not show significantly better scores on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children at ages 3.5 or 5.5 years. However, their study was not designed to investigate change after refractive correction in young children. Instead, they compared, post hoc, children with glasses with children with uncorrected refractive errors. Furthermore, their measures did not include the VMI, which is widely used in clinical and research settings to assess alterations in visual-motor function.
The improvement on the VMI in preschool children with ametropia following refractive correction for 6 weeks is similar to the time observed for improvement in anisometropic amblyopia with refractive correction in children aged 4 to 7 years.31,32Further research, now under way with the study population used here, also may show continued improvement over a longer period.
Our study also suggested a possibility that poorer visual-motor integration scores may be associated with lower verbal scores, which may be improved by wearing glasses. This could be clarified with a larger sample and/or a longer period of observation.
These results from this relatively small sample of low-income preschoolers with ametropia suggested that early identification and correction should optimize cognitive development and learning, at least in the studied sample.
Correspondence:Stuart I. Brown, MD, Department of Ophthalmology, University of California, San Diego, 9415 Campus Point Dr, La Jolla, CA 92093-0946 (sbrown@eyecenter.ucsd.edu).
Submitted for Publication:September 21, 2006; final revision received June 21, 2007; accepted June 25, 2007.
Financial Disclosure:None reported.
Funding/Support:This work was supported in part by the Foster Fellowship in Vision and Development and Research to Prevent Blindness.
Previous Presentation:This paper was presented in part as a poster at the 2005 Annual Meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology; May 3, 2005; Fort Lauderdale, Florida.
Additional Contributions:Lara Hustana, OD, and Louis Katz, OD, assessed the refractive conditions of the children and reviewed the manuscript; Idalid Franco, BA, performed psychometric testing; and Alfonso Rodriguez, PhD, Neighborhood House Association Head Start, and Jack Campana, EdD, San Diego City Schools, provided partnership and mentorship in conducting the study. We are indebted to the children and their families for their participation.
1.Helveston
EMWeber
JCMiller
K
et al. Visual function and academic performance.
Am J Ophthalmol 1985;99
(3)
346- 355
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.Rosner
JRosner
J The relationship between moderate hyperopia and academic achievement: how much plus is enough?
J Am Optom Assoc 1997;68
(10)
648- 650
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Rosner
JRosner
J Comparison of visual characteristics in children with and without learning difficulties.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1987;64
(7)
531- 533
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Stewart-Brown
SHaslum
MNButler
N Educational attainment of 10-year-old children with treated and untreated visual defects.
Dev Med Child Neurol 1985;27
(4)
504- 513
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Williams
WRLatif
AHAHannington
LWatkins
DR Hyperopia and educational attainment in a primary school cohort.
Arch Dis Child 2005;90
(2)
150- 153
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Simons
HDGassler
PA Vision anomalies and reading skill: a meta-analysis of the literature.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1988;65
(11)
893- 904
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Eames
TH The influence of hypermetropia and myopia on reading achievement.
Am J Ophthalmol 1955;39
(3)
375- 377
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Atkinson
JAnker
ANardini
M
et al. Infant vision screening predicts failures on motor and cognitive tests up to school age.
Strabismus 2002;10
(3)
187- 198
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Atkinson
JNardini
MAnker
ABraddick
OHughes
CRae
S Refractive errors in infancy predict reduced performance on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children at 3½ and 5½ years.
Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47
(4)
243- 251
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Beery
KE The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration: VMI With Supplemental Developmental Tests of Visual Perception and Motor Coordination: Administration, Scoring and Teaching Manual. 4th rev ed. Parsippany, NJ Modern Curriculum Press1997;
11.Wechsler
D Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Revised Manual. San Antonio, TX Harcourt Brace & Co1989;
12.Caputo
ARLingua
RW The problem of cycloplegia in the pediatric age group: a combination formula for refraction.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 1980;17
(2)
119- 128
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.American Academy of Ophthalmology Pediatric Ophthalmology Panel, Preferred Practice Pattern: Pediatric Eye Evaluations. San Francisco, CA American Academy of Ophthalmology2002;
14.American Optometric Association Consensus Panel on Pediatric Eye and Vision Examination, Pediatric Eye and Vision Examination. St Louis, MO American Optometric Association2002;
15.Lozoff
BJimenez
EWolf
AW Long-term developmental outcome of infants with iron deficiency.
N Engl J Med 1991;325
(10)
687- 694
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Achenbach
TMRescorla
LA Manual for the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment: Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1½ to 5. Burlington University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families2000;
17.Thibos
LNWheeler
WHormer
D Power vectors: an application of Fourier analysis to the description and statistical analysis of refractive error.
Optom Vis Sci 1997;74
(6)
367- 375
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Thibos
LNHormer
D Power vector analysis of the optical outcome of refractive surgery.
J Cataract Refract Surg 2001;27
(1)
80- 85
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Kleinstein
RNJones
LAHullett
S
et al. Collaborative Longitudinal Evaluation of Ethnicity and Refractive Error Study Group, Refractive error and ethnicity in children.
Arch Ophthalmol 2003;121
(8)
1141- 1147
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Gwiazda
JHyman
LHussein
M
et al. A randomized clinical trial of progressive addition lenses vs single vision lenses on the progression of myopia in children.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2003;44
(4)
1492- 1500
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.al-Saleh
INester
MDeVol
EShinwari
NMunchari
Lal-Shahria
S Relationships between blood lead concentrations, intelligence, and academic achievement of Saudi Arabian schoolgirls.
Int J Hyg Environ Health 2001;204
(2-3)
165- 174
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Baghurst
PAMcMichael
AJWigg
NR
et al. Environmental exposure to lead and children's intelligence at the age of seven years: the Port Pirie cohort study.
N Engl J Med 1992;327
(18)
1279- 1284
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Brooks-Gunn
JMcCarton
CCasey
PH
et al. Early intervention in low-birth-weight premature infants: results through age 5 from the infant health and development program.
JAMA 1994;272
(16)
1257- 1262
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Ment
LRVohr
BAllan
W
et al. Change in cognitive function over time in very low-birth-weight infants.
JAMA 2003;289
(6)
705- 711
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.McCormick
MCBrooks-Gunn
JBuka
SL
et al. Early intervention in low birth weight premature infants: results at 18 years of age for the Infant Health and Development Program.
Pediatrics 2006;117
(3)
771- 780
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Solan
HAMozlin
R The correlations of perceptual-motor maturation to readiness and reading in kindergarten and the primary grades.
J Am Optom Assoc 1986;57
(1)
28- 35
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Sortor
JMKulp
MT Are the results of the Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration and its subtests related to achievement test scores?
Optom Vis Sci 2003;80
(11)
758- 763
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Maples
WC Visual factors that significantly impact academic performance.
Optometry 2003;74
(1)
35- 49
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Simons
K Hyperopia, accommodative dysfunction and reading.
Binocul Vis Strabismus Q 2004;19
(2)
69- 70
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Anker
SAtkinson
JBraddick
ONardini
MEhrlich
D Non-cycloplegic refractive screening can identify infants whose visual outcome at 4 years is improved by spectacle correction.
Strabismus 2004;12
(4)
227- 245
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, A randomized trial to evaluate 2 hours of daily patching for strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia in children.
Ophthalmology 2006;113
(6)
904- 912
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, Treatment of anisometropic amblyopia in children with refractive correction.
Ophthalmology 2006;113
(6)
895- 903
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref