[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. 
Percentages Correct Overall: Total, Amblyopic Eyes, and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyesa
Percentages Correct Overall: Total, Amblyopic Eyes, and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyesa
Table 2. 
Odds of Correct Reading in Amblyopic and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyes
Odds of Correct Reading in Amblyopic and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyes
Table 3. 
Odds of Correct Reading of Top-Quintile Letters (A, L, Z, T) and Bottom-Quintile Letters (S, F, C, B)
Odds of Correct Reading of Top-Quintile Letters (A, L, Z, T) and Bottom-Quintile Letters (S, F, C, B)
Table 4. 
Percentages Correct at Threshold: Total, Amblyopic Eyes, and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyesa
Percentages Correct at Threshold: Total, Amblyopic Eyes, and Nonamblyopic Fellow Eyesa
Table 5. 
Odds of Correct Reading of Top-Quintile Letters (L, T, Z, A) and Bottom-Quintile Letters (S, F, C, B) at Threshold
Odds of Correct Reading of Top-Quintile Letters (L, T, Z, A) and Bottom-Quintile Letters (S, F, C, B) at Threshold
Table 6. 
Odds of Correct Reading of Letters in Positions 1, 3, and 5 at Thresholda
Odds of Correct Reading of Letters in Positions 1, 3, and 5 at Thresholda
Table 7. 
Odds of Correct Reading of Letters at the Beginning and End of a Threshold Line
Odds of Correct Reading of Letters at the Beginning and End of a Threshold Line
1.
American Academy of Ophthalmology, Basic and Clinical Science Course VI. San Francisco, CA Pediatric Ophthalmology2006;67- 75
2.
American Academy of Ophthalmology, Basic and Clinical Science Course VI. San Francisco, CA Pediatric Ophthalmology2006;77- 95
3.
Stuart  JABurian  HM A study of separation difficulty: its relationship to visual acuity in normal and amblyopic eyes.  Am J Ophthalmol 1962;53471- 477PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Holmes  JMBeck  RWRepka  MX  et al. Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, The amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol.  Arch Ophthalmol 2001;119 (9) 1345- 1353PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Bailey  ILLovie  JE New design principles for visual acuity letter charts.  Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1976;53 (11) 740- 745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Ferris  FL  IIIKassoff  ABresnick  GHBailey  I New visual acuity charts for clinical research.  Am J Ophthalmol 1982;94 (1) 91- 96PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study Group, Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study design and baseline patient characteristics: ETDRS report number 7.  Ophthalmology 1991;98 (5) ((suppl)) 741- 756PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Beck  RWMoke  PSTurpin  AH  et al.  A computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol 2003;135 (2) 194- 205PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Sloan  LRowland  WMAltman  A Comparison of three types of test target for the measurement of visual acuity.  Q Rev Ophthalmol 1952;84- 16Google Scholar
10.
Ferris  FL  IIIFreidlin  VKassoff  AGreen  SBMilton  RC Relative letter and position difficulty on visual acuity charts from the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study.  Am J Ophthalmol 1993;116 (6) 735- 740PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Hedin  AOlsson  K Letter legibility and the construction of a new visual acuity chart.  Ophthalmologica 1984;189 (3) 147- 156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Bennett  AG Ophthalmic test types: a review of previous work and discussions on some controversial questions.  Br J Physiol Opt 1965;22 (4) 238- 271PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
McMonnies  CWHo  A Letter legibility and chart equivalence.  Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2000;20 (2) 142- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McMonnies  CW Chart construction and letter legibility/readability.  Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1999;19 (6) 498- 506PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Chung  STLi  RWLevi  DM Crowding between first- and second-order letters in amblyopia.  Vision Res 2008;48 (6) 788- 798PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Clinical Sciences
February 14, 2011

Varying Difficulty of Snellen Letters and Common Errors in Amblyopic and Fellow Eyes

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Ophthalmology/Lions Eye Institute (Dr Simon), Albany Medical College (Mss Mathew and Shah), Albany, New York.

Arch Ophthalmol. 2011;129(2):184-187. doi:10.1001/archophthalmol.2010.369
Abstract

Objective  To investigate the varying difficulty of Snellen letters in children with amblyopia.

Methods  We tabulated the letter-by-letter responses of amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes on random, computer-generated Snellen lines. Participants were 60 children, aged 5 to 13 years, with a history of amblyopia. Main outcome measures were relative difficulties of Snellen letters and common misidentifications.

Results  Errors were 7.5 times more common with certain letters (B, C, F, S) than with others (A, L, Z, T), this difference increasing to 17.6-fold at threshold. Similar relative letter difficulty was demonstrated at lines above and at visual acuity thresholds, and both difficult and easy letters were the same for amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes. Specific misidentification errors were often repeated and were often reciprocal (eg, B for E and E for B).

Conclusion  Since therapeutic decisions in amblyopia management are often based on small differences in visual acuities, the relative difficulties of letters used in their measurement should be considered. The Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study system should be considered for use in this clinical setting.

Amblyopia represents the most common cause of decreased visual acuity in children and young adults.1 Fortunately, its treatment is generally effective, especially in younger children. Since visual acuity is both the primary end point of treatment and the parameter most useful in making therapeutic decisions, its measurement is of critical importance in amblyopic children. Changes in visual acuity measured before and after treatment intervals and differences between fellow eyes may be ascribed clinical significance, even if measured differences are small.

Unfortunately, visual acuity measurement can be problematic for a variety of reasons.2 Some are related to the nature of amblyopia: lines of optotypes are more difficult than single optotypes and letters are more difficult than figures.3,4 Others are related to the nature of children: they may not remember the names of some letters and they may become confused or fatigued when presented an entire line of letters.

Although Snellen letters are often used to measure visual acuity in children who know the letters reliably, there are inherent difficulties in the Snellen chart itself. Some have been well described.2,5,6 Children are adept at memorizing the letters, and there are fewer letters represented nearer the top of traditional wall or projected Snellen charts. Many of these difficulties have been addressed by the visual acuity charts originally developed for the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS), and some of their features have been incorporated in commonly used, computer-based visual acuity systems.7,8

On the other hand, a common observation among clinicians experienced with visual acuity testing is that some letters are more difficult than others. Part of this problem may relate to the position of letters in a given line. The crowding phenomenon, typical in amblyopia, results in the common finding that single letters or letters at either end of a line may be more easily identified than those in the middle of the line. But our experience has been that some letters are particularly easy (eg, L, T, A) and some particularly difficult (eg, B, S, C) for both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes, regardless of where in a line they are presented.

It has long been recognized that letters vary in difficulty.9-12 Indeed, the Snellen chart excludes I, J, M, Q, W, X, and Y for this reason. The ETDRS charts are limited to only 10 Sloan letters (S, K, H, N, O, C, D, V, R, Z), which were initially considered to be of similar difficulty.9 Even within this group, however, some letters were proved more difficult to identify than others (eg, C was 8.4 times as likely to be misidentified than Z).10

To our knowledge, the relative difficulty of the full range of Snellen letters has not been systematically investigated, children have not been the focus of previous studies, and the impact of amblyopia has not been described in this context. We were interested in addressing these issues and in defining any specific misidentifications that tended to recur.

Methods

This prospective study was approved by the Albany Medical College institutional review board and was found to comply with the requirements of the US Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act. Parental consents for tabulations of visual acuity measurements were executed prior to determination of best-corrected Snellen visual acuities of 60 consecutive children, ranging in age from 5 to 13 years. Children with other ophthalmologic conditions and those with developmental delays sufficient to preclude visual acuity measurements were excluded. An occluder or opaque tape was used for monocular testing.

All children had strabismic or anisometropic amblyopia, or a combination of the two, with visual acuities ranging from 20/20 to 20/100 (mean, 20/38.9; median, 20/40). Fellow eyes ranged from 20/20 to 20/100 (mean, 20/23.2; median, 20/25). Excluding 1 outlier, whose fellow eye was 20/100, the range was 20/20 to 20/40 in fellow eyes.

Lines of random Snellen letters, each 5 letters in length, were presented at distance using the M and S Technologies Smart System II PC-Plus (M and S Technologies, Skokie, Illinois). Best-corrected visual acuity end points were determined as the smallest line with fewer than half of the letters missed, assuming at least equivalent performance on larger lines. Data collected for each amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eye included which letters were presented, which were identified correctly and which were missed, and what specific errors were made in each presentation.

Results

The number of presentations and the proportions correctly identified for each of the 19 letters used by the system are given in Table 1 for both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes in order of increasing difficulty. As indicated in Table 2, amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes were statistically similar in their overall proportion of correct answers: the 95% confidence interval around the odds ratio of 0.83 included 1.0. The most difficult letters (Table 1) were S, F, C, and B. The easiest letters were A, L, Z, and T. For both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes, the easiest and the most difficult quintiles were nearly identical and, as indicated in Table 3, the easiest 4 letters were identified correctly 7.5 times as frequently as the most difficult 4 letters.

A separate analysis of threshold data demonstrated relative letter difficulties similar to the full data set (Table 4). At threshold, however, the 4 easiest letters were identified correctly 17.6 times more frequently than the 4 most difficult (Table 5). The greatest separation between amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes was noted in R, O, N, and K. These letters were correctly identified 2.5 times more frequently at threshold by nonamblyopic fellow eyes than by amblyopic eyes (95% confidence interval, 1.1136-5.4334).

Both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes correctly identified letters at either end of each line 2.7 times as often as letters in the third position, or middle, of the line (Table 6). As seen in Table 7, no statistical difference was found between letters at the very beginning and very end of the line (the confidence interval of the odds ratio included 1.0).

Interestingly, the specific errors were similar for both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes. Several errors were reciprocal: B was misidentified as E in 58% and E as B in 70% of errors involving these letters. F was misidentified as P in 74% and P as F in 41%. H was misidentified as N in 76% and N as H in 55%. U was misidentified as V in 53% and V as U in 50%. C was misidentified as O in 75%, D as O in 70%, and G as O in 59%. K was misidentified as R in 45%, P as R in 41%, R as A in 67%, and Z as Y in 80%.

Comment

Our experience with random presentations of Snellen letters confirms the common impression, formerly reported, that some are more difficult than others.9-13 Indeed, some letters are so difficult as to be excluded in both Snellen and ETDRS charts. The most difficult Snellen letters were S, F, C, and B ; the easiest were A, L, T, and Z. We were particularly interested to find that both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes had difficulty with similar letters and that difficult letters were relatively difficult and easy letters relatively easy both at threshold and at lines above threshold.

Although some letters of moderate difficulty (R, O, N, K) seemed relatively better able to distinguish amblyopic from nonamblyopic fellow eyes, the strength of this difference was not compelling. Further, we do not have the capability to limit the letters presented to only these 4.

Although they have not been extensively studied, the specific errors we identified are familiar to those who regularly measure visual acuities.14 Again, errors were similar for amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes: B and E, F and P, H and N, and V and U were common confusions. Several letters of similar contour were confused with O: C, G, and D.

We recognize limitations inherent in this small, single-center study using 1 computerized visual acuity system. The ETDRS studies, by contrast, included more patients but reported only the errors made on the threshold line.10 Our data set included 1712 letter presentations in 120 eyes of 60 children. We found many of the same Snellen letters as the ETDRS letters to be among the most difficult, especially letters with contours, as opposed to straight lines. On the other hand, some differences may have resulted from the selection of the letters and the protocol used: the ETDRS chart used only 10 Sloan letters presented in 14 consistent sequences, while our Snellen-based system used 19 letters presented entirely at random.

As has been previously reported, the relative position of letters in a given line influences their difficulty: earlier letters are easier than those in the middle of the line, perhaps because the middle letters appear “crowded,” especially to the amblyopic eye.15 Letters at the beginning of the line may be easier, not only because there is no crowding to the left but also because more effort may be expended before fatigue sets in toward the middle and end of the line.10 Our data confirm that letters at the beginning and end of the line were easier than letters in the middle. But we found that letters at the end of the line were as easy as those at the beginning of the line, suggesting that crowding within a given line may have been a more important factor than fatigue, even for nonamblyopic fellow eyes.

In conclusion, our data indicate that both amblyopic and nonamblyopic fellow eyes have difficulty identifying the same letters and make similar specific errors, both at threshold and on larger lines, and that position within the line influences difficulty. Children with amblyopia appear similar in all regards to the adults previously reported. Some letters are as much as 7.5 times as difficult as others, this difference increasing to 17.6-fold at threshold. In determining appropriate levels of occlusion or penalization, clinicians should therefore consider the relative difficulty of letters used when deciding whether small sequential or interocular differences in visual acuity are significant. The easiest and most difficult letters might best be excluded. The ETDRS lines, although comprising letters of varying difficulty, have the advantage of balancing easy and difficult letters within each. We recommend that the ETDRS system be considered for use in amblyopic subjects.

Correspondence: John W. Simon, MD, Department of Ophthalmology/Lions Eye Institute, Albany Medical College, 1220 New Scotland Rd, Ste 202, Slingerlands, NY 12159 (simonj@mail.amc.edu).

Submitted for Publication: March 3, 2010; final revision received May 3, 2010; accepted May 4, 2010.

Author Contributions: All authors had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Additional Contributions: Catherine Simon, MA, assisted with tabulation and analysis of data.

References
1.
American Academy of Ophthalmology, Basic and Clinical Science Course VI. San Francisco, CA Pediatric Ophthalmology2006;67- 75
2.
American Academy of Ophthalmology, Basic and Clinical Science Course VI. San Francisco, CA Pediatric Ophthalmology2006;77- 95
3.
Stuart  JABurian  HM A study of separation difficulty: its relationship to visual acuity in normal and amblyopic eyes.  Am J Ophthalmol 1962;53471- 477PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Holmes  JMBeck  RWRepka  MX  et al. Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group, The amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol.  Arch Ophthalmol 2001;119 (9) 1345- 1353PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Bailey  ILLovie  JE New design principles for visual acuity letter charts.  Am J Optom Physiol Opt 1976;53 (11) 740- 745PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Ferris  FL  IIIKassoff  ABresnick  GHBailey  I New visual acuity charts for clinical research.  Am J Ophthalmol 1982;94 (1) 91- 96PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study Group, Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study design and baseline patient characteristics: ETDRS report number 7.  Ophthalmology 1991;98 (5) ((suppl)) 741- 756PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Beck  RWMoke  PSTurpin  AH  et al.  A computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study testing protocol.  Am J Ophthalmol 2003;135 (2) 194- 205PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Sloan  LRowland  WMAltman  A Comparison of three types of test target for the measurement of visual acuity.  Q Rev Ophthalmol 1952;84- 16Google Scholar
10.
Ferris  FL  IIIFreidlin  VKassoff  AGreen  SBMilton  RC Relative letter and position difficulty on visual acuity charts from the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study.  Am J Ophthalmol 1993;116 (6) 735- 740PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Hedin  AOlsson  K Letter legibility and the construction of a new visual acuity chart.  Ophthalmologica 1984;189 (3) 147- 156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Bennett  AG Ophthalmic test types: a review of previous work and discussions on some controversial questions.  Br J Physiol Opt 1965;22 (4) 238- 271PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
McMonnies  CWHo  A Letter legibility and chart equivalence.  Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2000;20 (2) 142- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McMonnies  CW Chart construction and letter legibility/readability.  Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1999;19 (6) 498- 506PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Chung  STLi  RWLevi  DM Crowding between first- and second-order letters in amblyopia.  Vision Res 2008;48 (6) 788- 798PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×