Objectives
To compare 2 clinical tests of vestibular function, the head autorotation test (HART) and the head thrust test (HTT), and to determine why they give disparate results in patients with known unilateral vestibular deficiency (UVD) due to labyrinthectomy.
Methods
We used scleral coils to measure the horizontal (yaw) vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) in 5 healthy human subjects and in 11 patients who underwent labyrinthectomy. We used 2 paradigms. Using HART, subjects visually fixated a target during self-generated, swept-frequency, sinusoidal, horizontal head rotations. Using HTT, patients fixated the target during horizontal head thrusts delivered randomly in direction and time.
Results
In subjects without UVD, eye movements were almost perfectly compensatory for both paradigms. In subjects with UVD, VOR gain for ipsilesional head thrusts was low for both paradigms, but significantly (P<.001) higher (less abnormal) for HART (0.60 ± 0.13) than for HTT (0.14 ± 0.13). Contralesional gain was reduced for both, to 0.64 ± 0.20 for HART and to 0.57 ± 0.17 for HTT. Because ipsilesional and contralesional gains were not statistically different for HART (P = .69), comparison of VOR gains for half-cycle responses to the HART stimulus could not reliably identify the side of the known lesion. In contrast, HTT consistently identified the side of the lesion for all subjects with UVD. To investigate whether preprogramming contributes to the boost in VOR as measured by HART, we compared the gain and response delay of eye movements during actively self-generated and passively received head thrusts. For subjects without UVD, response delays were shorter for active (6 ± 1 milliseconds) than for passive (12 ± 1 milliseconds) HTT. For ipsilesional rotations of subjects with UVD, active HTT yielded a significantly higher gain (0.44 ± 0.20) (P<.001) and a shorter delay (15 ± 6 milliseconds) (P<.001) than did passive HTT (0.14 ± 0.13 and 37 ± 15 milliseconds, respectively). Contralesional test results revealed a similar performance boost for active head movements. Data are given as mean ± SD.
Conclusion
When comparison of half-cycle gains is used to identify the lesion side, self-generated predictable head movement paradigms, such as HART and active HTT, are less accurate than passive HTT in the characterization of UVD, in part because preprogramming can augment the VOR during voluntary head movements.
DIZZINESS IS the ninth most common reason adults visit a primary care physician, and it affects approximately 90 million Americans.1-4 Identification of the side, affected semicircular canals, and degree of unilateral vestibular deficiency (UVD) is an important goal in the examination of patients experiencing dizziness.
Two high-frequency rotational stimulus paradigms—the head thrust test (HTT) and the head autorotation test (HART)—are in wide use for assessing angular vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) function, yet have not been directly compared.
Researchers5-8 have advocated HTT, in which high-acceleration impulsive head rotations are delivered in the plane of one pair of semicircular canals while a subject attempts to maintain visual fixation on a distant target. Implied by the second law of Ewald,9 such head rotations elicit an asymmetric response in patients with UVD by maximally stimulating the neural pathway arising from one semicircular canal crista while maximally inhibiting or silencing the pathway from the coplanar canal. The approach has proved sensitive for the detection of vestibular dysfunction in subjects with known UVD.6,10
A second paradigm, HART, uses self-generated, swept-frequency, sinusoidal head rotations performed while the subject visually fixates a distant earth-fixed target. Fourier analysis of system gain and phase, along with formulaic measures of response asymmetry, are applied to the measured head and eye movements. Researchers11-16 have reported extensively on this approach, and several commercially available vestibular testing systems are based on this paradigm.
We sought to compare the ability of HTT with that of HART to identify dysfunction in subjects with known UVD after surgical labyrinthectomy. There are 3 differences between these 2 stimulus paradigms that could contribute to a difference in measured VOR for a given patient. First, HTT stimuli are passive (ie, delivered by the examiner), while during HART, the head movement is active (ie, self-generated). Second, HTT stimuli are presented unpredictably in time and direction, while the sinusoidal frequency–sweep head movement during HART is predictable cycle by cycle. Third, HTT stimuli are often of higher acceleration and wider spectral bandwidth than are HART stimuli, because of some subjects' inability to make rapid, self-generated, sinusoidal head movements.
We hypothesized that during active predictable head movements, subjects with UVD can augment apparent VOR performance by using information about the intended/expected head movement to complement deficient vestibular function and guide compensatory eye movements. Such an effect would be expected to reduce the sensitivity of tests like HART that rely on self-generated head movement stimuli for detecting vestibular hypofunction. We tested these predictions by comparing VOR gains measured for subjects with and without UVD during HART and HTT, and by comparing subjects' responses to active and passive impulsive head rotations.
We studied 5 human subjects without UVD and 11 with UVD due to labyrinthectomy. Subjects without UVD ranged in age from 32 to 55 years and were free of vestibular and ocular disease, except for wearing corrective lenses. Subjects with UVD ranged in age from 48 to 72 years, and the time from labyrinthectomy ranged from 3 to 120 months. All had undergone vestibular rehabilitation therapy postoperatively. The indication for labyrinthectomy was vestibular schwannoma or meningioma in 6 subjects and unilateral Meniere disease in 5. The side of labyrinthectomy was the left for 6 subjects with UVD and the right for 5. All subjects gave written informed consent, and the experimental procedures were approved by the Joint Committee on Clinical Investigation, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Md.
Head and eye movements were recorded in 3 dimensions using magnetic search coils embedded in contact lenses and in a bite block. The instrumentation and technique have been described in detail elsewhere.17 Eye and bite block angular positions were sampled at 500 Hz. The resulting signals were low-pass filtered with a single-pole analog filter with a 3-dB bandwidth of 100 Hz. Each subject was tested while seated upright and centered within a uniform magnetic field, with the interpupillary line parallel to the horizon and the Frankfort line (from the top of the tragus to the infraorbital foramen) in the plane of head rotation. All rotational stimuli were in the horizontal (yaw) plane. During each trial, the room was completely dark except for a target light-emitting diode positioned 1.24 m anterior to the center of the head, at the same elevation as the pupils. All subjects were tested more than 20 minutes after removal of eyeglasses.
Each experiment began with the subject's head held rigidly at the starting position via connection of the bite block to a bar, while the subject performed calibration tasks. The bar was then removed, and all subsequent trials were performed with the head free to move during a stimulus, then returning to the starting position.
For HART, subjects attempted to visually fixate the target while sinusoidally shaking their heads horizontally (ie, rotating about an earth-vertical axis through the center of the cervical spine) at maximum tolerated velocity in time with a metronome swept logarithmically in frequency from 1 to 6 Hz for 20 seconds. Results from 3 trials were averaged.
For HTT, subjects fixated the same target during brief (100-200–millisecond) rotations in the earth-horizontal plane, at high acceleration (3000°-5000°/s2) starting from a complete stop. Head velocity reached 25° to 75°/s at 40 milliseconds into the movement and 150° to 300°/s peak velocity by 100 to 120 milliseconds. The final position was 10° to 15° from center. For passive HTT (pHTT), stimuli were delivered manually at an unpredictable onset time and in a randomly varied direction, starting from center. The subject received no cues about the direction or time of an impending passive head thrust. For active HTT (aHTT), the subject generated the head thrust voluntarily. Only head movements reaching greater than 100°/s within the first 60 milliseconds were included in the analysis. A typical trial lasted about 3 minutes and included about 20 to 40 head thrusts to each side.
To facilitate comparison between eye and head angular velocities, both were analyzed in 3 dimensions in the head frame of reference. Horizontal rotational components were extracted and compared.18 All results reported are for the horizontal components of rotation only. Angular velocity and acceleration were computed from angular position signals using a 30-Hz, band-limited, 10-element, linear phase differentiating digital filter designed using computer software (MATLAB; The Mathworks, Inc, Cambridge, Mass) and applied in zero-delay fashion.
We quantified the eye movement responses in terms of velocity gain and response delay. Gain was defined as the angular velocity of the eye divided by the angular velocity of the head at the instant head velocity crossed to above 120°/s, with the eye velocity inverted so that a gain of +1.0 denotes an ideal compensatory response. Using the method of Tabak et al,7 we defined response delay as the time between the onsets of head and eye movements, where onset was defined as the time at which velocity crossed a threshold set at 2°/s plus 8 times the root-mean-square velocity in the 100 milliseconds preceding the stimulus.8 We used 1- or 2-tailed t tests to compare gain and response delay distributions between experimental groups, with P<.05 considered significant. Data are given as mean ± SD unless otherwise indicated.
Head autorotation testing
Figure 1 shows HART results for a healthy 33-year-old man without UVD. In the trial shown, head velocity ranged from 40°/s at 1 Hz to approximately 180°/s at 6 to 8 Hz (Figure 1A). Eye velocity traces (inverted for comparison with head velocity) are nearly identical to head velocity traces, to within less than the 4°/s of retinal image slip for which visual acuity begins to degrade (Figure 1B).19 When plotted as eye velocity vs head velocity, these data lie almost exactly along a y = −x line, consistent with a nearly perfect VOR (Figure 1C). This subject without UVD has rightward and leftward VOR velocity gains of 1.0 ± 0.05 and 1.0 ± 0.02, respectively.
Figure 2 shows HART results for a symptomatically well-compensated 63-year-old man who had undergone right translabyrinthine vestibular schwannoma resection and postoperative vestibular rehabilitation 5 months before testing. For this subject, the response during head turns toward the contralesional left side (Figure 2A and B) is somewhat degraded, but not significantly (P = .07) different from normal (gain, 0.87 ± 0.13). During rightward head turns, there is a significantly (P<.001) abnormal response, with a velocity gain of 0.43 ± 0.19 and retinal slip errors of up to 100°/s. Retinal slip not only degrades foveal visual acuity but also accrues to cause the subject to lose target fixation, eliciting large saccadelike corrective eye movements to reacquire the visual target. For this subject, HART results clearly reveal an asymmetry.
Such asymmetries between responses to ipsilesional and contralesional head rotations were not always apparent. Figure 3 presents the HART responses from a 68-year-old man tested 38 months after undergoing surgery resulting in left UVD. At maximum effort, this patient achieved slower peak head velocity than the subject shown in Figure 2. The slow-phase components of the response to ipsilesional rotations were not markedly different from those observed in the contralesional half cycles. However, a gaze-correcting saccadic eye movement was often observed in the ipsilesional responses.
Figure 4A and B shows a summary of VOR gains measured using HART in 5 healthy subjects and in 11 subjects with UVD, respectively. The VOR gains for subjects without UVD are segregated into rightward and leftward head movements and illustrated separately as a check of test reliability. The VOR gains for subjects without UVD are 0.95 ± 0.13 and 1.03 ± 0.11 for rightward and leftward head rotations, respectively. There was no significant difference between right and left VOR gains (P = .13), and the combined data set was insignificantly different from 1.0 (P = .64) (95% confidence interval, 0.93-1.05). The HART gains for subjects with UVD were segregated into ipsilesional and contralesional directions. Four findings were notable. First, the ipsilesional gain of 0.60 ± 0.13 was significantly lower than normal (P<.001). Second, there was a wide distribution of ipsilesional gains for this sample population, ranging from 0.2 to 1.0. Third, the contralesional gain of 0.64 ± 0.20 was also significantly decreased from normal (P<.001). Finally, HART gains were not significantly different for ipsilesional and contralesional rotations in these subjects with UVD and, therefore, could not reliably indicate the side of the known lesion (P = .69).
Passive HTT was performed on 4 of the subjects without UVD and on all 11 subjects with UVD who had been studied using HART. Figure 5A shows head velocity and concurrent eye velocity traces for 2 representative head thrusts from the same subject without UVD who was described in Figure 1. The high-acceleration head rotation transients reach a peak velocity of greater than 200°/s within 80 to 100 milliseconds and are tracked accurately by compensatory eye movements, with a slight overshoot following the peak of head velocity. Figure 5B shows multiple head thrust stimuli and the corresponding eye movement responses for the first 80 milliseconds of each. Peak head accelerations ranged from approximately 3000° to 5000°/s2. Although there is a slight early mismatch of eye and head traces because of VOR latency, retinal slip is kept below 5°/s throughout most of each trace. In Figure 5C, a plot of eye velocity vs head velocity reveals that all traces lie close to a y = −x line, corresponding to nearly perfect performance. Rightward and leftward gains for this subject were 1.00 ± 0.04 and 0.99 ± 0.06, respectively. Subjects without UVD never complained of losing sight of the target during testing.
In contrast, Figure 6 shows pHTT responses for the 68-year-old man with left UVD for whom HART data were described in Figure 3. In Figure 6A, a rightward head thrust is tracked fairly well by the eye, although the VOR gain is less than normal and a small catch-up saccadelike movement is required to correct the final eye position after the head movement ends. For a leftward head thrust, the VOR response decrease is more pronounced, as are the prominent catch-up eye movements. The second and third catch-up movements occur after the head stops moving; these are the corrective eye movements an observer can detect when using HTT as part of the physical examination. Figure 6B shows the first 80 milliseconds of multiple stimuli for this subject. For rightward head rotations, the eye tracks the head fairly well; however, the initial VOR-mediated eye movement response for leftward head thrusts is of low amplitude. Figure 6C reveals an asymmetry in the eye movement responses to contralesional and ipsilesional head thrusts, corresponding to the measured VOR gains of 0.50 ± 0.15 and 0.06 ± 0.09, respectively.
Figure 4C and D shows a summary of pHTT VOR gains for 4 subjects without UVD and 11 subjects with UVD, respectively. (The same subjects were used for pHTT and HART, except for 1 subject without UVD who was tested only with HART.) Right and left pHTT responses of subjects without UVD are shown separately in Figure 4C. Rightward (0.94 ± 0.04) and leftward (0.90 ± 0.05) pHTT gains were not significantly different (P = .27) for subjects without UVD. For subjects with UVD (Figure 4D), gains were decreased for ipsilesional and contralesional head rotations. In contrast to the HART VOR gains for the same population of subjects, there was a marked asymmetry between ipsilesional (0.14 ± 0.13) and contralesional (0.58 ± 0.17) gains. For every patient with UVD tested, ipsilateral gains were significantly lower than contralesional gains (P<.001). Ipsilesional and contralesional gains were each significantly different from normal (P<.001).
Comparison of HART and pHTT results reveals that although the ipsilateral VOR gains are reduced for the HART paradigm applied to subjects with UVD, a similar reduction in contralesional gains was such that HART could not reliably distinguish the side of the known lesion. In contrast, there was a marked and consistent asymmetry in the VOR gains of subjects with UVD measured using the pHTT paradigm, with the ipsilateral gains close to 0 and the contralesional gains close to gains measured using HART.
To better identify why the VOR is apparently enhanced when measured by the HART paradigm, we compared VOR gains measured in subjects with and without UVD using head thrusts that were either passively and unpredictably received by the subjects as in the usual application of the test (pHTT) or actively generated by the subjects (aHTT). Analysis was limited to active and passive head movements that were similar in mean peak velocity, peak acceleration, and spectral content.
Figure 7 shows the first 80 milliseconds of head movements and corresponding eye movement responses to aHTT for the subject with left UVD shown in Figure 3 and Figure 6. Whereas the ipsilesional pHTT response was essentially nonexistent for the first 80 milliseconds after stimulus onset (Figure 6B), the aHTT response VOR gain is enhanced, to 0.34 ± 0.06, from 0.06 ± 0.09 (Figure 7A). The contralesional VOR gain is also increased, to 0.74 ± 0.05, from 0.50 ± 0.15. Similarly, the time from stimulus onset to threshold response is shorter for aHTT than for pHTT.
Figure 8 shows pHTT and aHTT VOR gains for 3 subjects without UVD and 10 subjects with UVD (the same subjects used for HART except for those who did not generate aHTT head movements similar to those of pHTT). For subjects without UVD (Figure 8A), responses to aHTT and pHTT were not significantly different from each other or from 1.00 (P>.05 for all). For subjects with UVD, there was a significant (P<.001) increase in apparent ipsilesional VOR gain when measured using the aHTT paradigm, to 0.44 ± 0.20 (from 0.14 ± 0.13 for pHTT) (Figure 8B). The contralesional VOR gain also increased significantly (P = .004) under aHTT conditions, to 0.81 ± 0.14 (from 0.58 ± 0.17 for pHTT) (Figure 8C). This level was not significantly different from the VOR gains measured in subjects without UVD using pHTT (P = .08).
To obtain a measure of response timing, we used the method of Tabak et al8 to define stimulus and response onset and to compute a response delay. Figure 9 shows pHTT and aHTT VOR response delays for 4 subjects without UVD and 11 subjects with UVD. For subjects without UVD (Figure 9A), the response delay was 11.0 ± 3.3 milliseconds for pHTT and slightly, but significantly, lower at 5.0 ± 1.8 milliseconds for aHTT (P = .006). For subjects with UVD, the response delay for ipsilesional head thrusts was significantly shorter for aHTT (19 ± 16 milliseconds) than for pHTT (42 ± 17 milliseconds) (P = .006) (Figure 9B). The response delay in subjects with UVD for contralesional head thrusts was also shorter for aHTT (11.0 ± 5.1 milliseconds) than for pHTT (15.0 ± 4.5 milliseconds), but the difference was not significant (P = .07) (Figure 9C).
Clinical tests for identification of uvd
Traditional caloric and rotary chair tests for the identification of UVD fail to test the vestibular system using physiologically appropriate stimuli over the range of frequencies and accelerations for which the system apparently evolved. Caloric testing has the advantage of being strictly unilateral, but uses an unnatural stimulus (a thermal gradient) that, as measured by the eye movements it elicits, is the equivalent of a low frequency (0.025 Hz) and low amplitude (approximately 50°/s) head rotation. The caloric response can be altered by anatomic changes in the external ear canal and by individual variation in temporal bone anatomic features, and it only evaluates 1 of the 5 vestibular receptors (the lateral semicircular canal). Rotary chair vestibular testing is more physiologic; however, at the lower frequencies and velocities typically used in clinical vestibular laboratories, rotary chair tests are insensitive in the identification of chronic total unilateral vestibular hypofunction.20,21 The sensitivity of rotary chair testing can be improved by comparison of responses to steps of acceleration with the head up and head down.22
In contrast, high-frequency and high-acceleration rotational stimuli unmask the inherent asymmetry in the vestibular system, as described by the second law of Ewald9—that the excitatory responses of vestibular pathway neurons encode motion over a larger dynamic range than do inhibitory responses. The eye movements elicited by such stimuli are principally due to excitation of the vestibular pathway arising from the canal ipsilateral to the direction of head acceleration and are, therefore, sensitive to a unilateral peripheral vestibular loss in that canal.
There are 2 high-frequency rotational stimuli in clinical use, pHTT and HART. The main objective of this study was to compare these 2 stimuli for the identification of unilateral vestibular hypofunction. The pHTT is a single, passive (operator delivered), unpredictable, high-acceleration (2000°-4000°/s2), low amplitude (15°-30°) head rotation in the direction of a single semicircular canal. The HART is a continuous, active (self-generated), sinusoidal head rotation that begins slowly and becomes faster, covering a frequency range of about 1 to 6 Hz. During each test, subjects are required to fixate a visual target in front of them, and their ability to maintain visual fixation is accepted as a measure of VOR function. However, because HART is actively generated, non-VOR processes, such as predictive eye movements driven by an "efference copy" neural representation of the intended head movement, could contribute to maintaining gaze stability. As a result, VOR function could seem artificially enhanced, making HART a less sensitive and less accurate test of UVD.
We tested 11 patients who had undergone surgical labyrinthectomy and compared their responses with those of 5 subjects without UVD. The VOR gains for subjects without UVD using pHTT were insignificantly different from an ideal gain of 1.0, consistent with findings from previous studies.23 For subjects with UVD, the VOR gains of 0.14 ± 0.13 for ipsilesional head thrusts and 0.58 ± 0.17 for contralesional head thrusts were significantly different, and the side with the lesion was clearly identified by pHTT in each patient, consistent with previous studies.6,24 For HART, the VOR gain in subjects without UVD was essentially perfect, as one would expect. Surprisingly, however, although some subjects with UVD showed obvious asymmetry of VOR half-cycle gains on HART, there was no significant difference between the ipsilesional and contralesional VOR gains over the population with UVD (P = .69).
Other features of the response to the actively generated HART stimulus did provide an indication of the side of unilateral vestibular hypofunction. The occurrence of rapid, gaze-correcting, saccadic eye movements during head movements toward the side of the lesion, when they occur, is a reliable indication of the side of UVD. A bias velocity in the eye movement response during the HART stimulus (a direct current shift in the eye velocity trace toward the side of the lesion) provides another such indication. Head autorotation is, therefore, a useful paradigm, but comparison of VOR gains from half-cycle analysis of HART responses is not reliable for the identification of the side of UVD.
Mechanisms for improved responses to active head rotations
There are 2 main differences between HART and pHTT stimuli that might explain the improvement in VOR gain of patients with UVD for ipsilesional head movements during HART. First, the peak velocities and accelerations generated by the examiner in pHTT are greater than those that some patients are able to generate during HART. This constraint on stimulus intensity may limit HART's ability to discern unilateral weakness in such patients. In contrast, the stimulus used for pHTT is generated by the examiner and reaches a peak acceleration of 3000°/s2 and a peak velocity of 250°/s. Although these head movements have low amplitude in displacement (10°-15°) and are well tolerated by patients, they are sufficient to elicit excitation-inhibition asymmetry and, thus, selectively probe the function of the excited canal.
A second difference is that the pHTT is passive, transient, and unpredictable, whereas the HART is a self-generated, sinusoidal, predictable stimulus. Self-generated, sinusoidal, steady-state rotations could allow for nonvestibular eye movement systems (eg, predictive eye movements, efference copy, and visual-following mechanisms) to contribute to the response. The existence of predictive mechanisms in augmenting vestibular responses is suggested by measures of visual acuity during head movement. Visual acuity is improved during active compared with passive head movements.25,26 To further investigate these potential effects of self-generated stimuli, we asked patients to perform the HTT themselves, aiming to mimic the speed and amplitude of the operator-delivered head thrusts. Although patients had some difficulty reaching the top speed of operator-delivered stimuli (about 300°/s), they did reach speeds of approximately 200°/s, and we compared responses for active and passive head thrusts of similar peak velocity and acceleration. In subjects without UVD, the VOR gain was already insignificantly different from 1.00 for pHTT, so no significant change was observed for aHTT. For subjects with UVD, however, there was a marked improvement in VOR gain during active head movements, and the boost in VOR gain during active thrusts occurred from the onset of the head thrust, during the initial 20 to 40 milliseconds. The only 2 oculomotor systems with a latency of less than 40 milliseconds are the direct VOR and predictive eye movements. Saccades, the cervico-ocular reflex, and visual-following mechanisms, such as smooth pursuit, all have latencies that are 70 to 150 milliseconds.27 From previous studies,6,10 it has been shown that the 3-neuron VOR arc generates the response to pHTT, so the boost in gain during aHTT is, therefore, likely to be a result of predictive eye movements.
To study the effect of prediction, we measured the time between the onset of head rotation and the onset of eye rotation for active and passive head thrusts. We used manually applied passive head thrusts to approximate the usual clinical application of pHTT and the movements our subjects made during aHTT. Manual thrusts do not have an onset sharp enough or an acceleration constant enough to precisely measure VOR latency (estimated at 8.6 milliseconds in subjects without UVD by Collewijn and co-workers28,29 using a torque-applying helmet in an HTT-type paradigm). To obtain a measure of response timing, we used the method of Tabak et al8 to define stimulus and response onset times (at which head and eye velocities cross set thresholds) and compute a response delay. This response delay probably does not precisely equal the true synaptic and axonal conduction delay of the VOR, because the finite rate of change of acceleration for manually applied stimuli and responses reduces the precision with which onset times for head and eye movements can be measured. However, this response delay provides a useful measure for comparison of responses to passive and active head thrusts.
For subjects without UVD, the response delay was shorter for active than for passive head thrusts. For subjects with UVD, the response delay during ipsilesional head thrusts was significantly longer for passive thrusts than for active thrusts. A similarly prolonged response delay for passive ipsilesional head rotations has been reported by Tabak et al.8 The apparent reduction in the response delay to aHTT might be further evidence of preprogrammed eye rotation. Alternatively, some of the apparent difference in response delay may be due to a difference in VOR gain for the first 40 milliseconds of the response. The initial low velocity eye rotation makes it difficult to precisely define the onset of the eye rotation, and could give the appearance of a delayed onset.
Timing of corrective eye movements
Further evidence for preprogramming of eye rotations during active head thrusts was the reduced latency of rapid corrective eye movements during active compared with passive head thrusts. To compensate for a deficient VOR, patients with UVD used rapid eye movements to correct accrued gaze error and reacquire visual fixation of the target. During active head thrusts, these corrective movements occurred as early as 60 milliseconds after the onset of the head rotation, significantly earlier than during passive head thrusts (Figure 10). Under normal circumstances, the latency of voluntary true saccadic eye movements is 200 milliseconds.25 This latency can be reduced to 100 milliseconds if the subject is trained in a specific task (so-called express saccades30). Tian et al25 have shown that under pHTT conditions, these rapid eye movements have latencies shorter than those of express saccades. Our data indicate that these latencies can be reduced even further under conditions of active head movement.
Gain asymmetries on the pHTT reliably detected the presence of known unilateral vestibular lesions in all patients with UVD tested, whereas the half-cycle gains measured from HART did not. Relative to pHTT, HART gains overestimated ipsilesional vestibular function and were, therefore, a less accurate indicator of unilateral hypofunction.
For subjects with UVD who are unable to generate high-acceleration sinusoidal head movements, HART may underestimate hypofunction simply because it relies on stimulus accelerations inadequate to silence inhibitory pathways arising in the intact ear. In contrast, the high-acceleration movements of pHTT reliably identify response asymmetry by more selectively probing the function of the excited canal. However, even for head movements of similar acceleration and time course, the measured VOR during self-generated head rotations (aHTT) has significantly higher gain and shorter response delay than does the VOR to passive unpredictable stimuli (pHTT).
Subjects with UVD may use a variety of strategies for improving visual fixation, including preprogrammed eye movements designed to augment the deficient VOR and to compensate for a planned or anticipated head movement. Tests using passive high-acceleration head thrusts delivered unpredictably in time and direction should, therefore, be more sensitive for discerning VOR hypofunction than tests using active and/or predictable stimuli.
Accepted for publication February 18, 2002.
This study was supported by the Garnett Passe and Rodney Williams Memorial Foundation, Parkville, Australia (Dr Cremer); the Royal Australasian College of Physicians, Sydney, New South Wales (Dr Cremer); grants K23 DC00196 (Dr Carey) and R01 DC02390 (Dr Minor) from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md; and a Johns Hopkins Clinician-Scientist Award (Dr Carey).
We thank Grace C. Y. Peng, PhD, David S. Zee, MD, Adrian G. Lasker, MS, and Dale C. Roberts, MS, for their assistance with data acquisition; and John K. Niparko, MD, and Howard W. Francis, MD, for their referral of subjects.
Corresponding author and reprints: Lloyd B. Minor, MD, Department of Otolaryngology–Head & Neck Surgery, The Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, 601 N Caroline St, Baltimore, MD 21287-0910 (e-mail: lminor@bme.jhu.edu).
1.Hoffman
RMEinstadter
DKroenke
K Evaluating dizziness.
Am J Med.1999;107:468-478.
Google Scholar 2.Kroenke
KArrington
MEManglesdorff
AD The prevalence of symptoms in medical outpatients and the adequacy of therapy.
Arch Intern Med.1990;150:1685-1689.
Google Scholar 3.Cypress
BK Patterns of ambulatory care in internal medicine: the National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey.
Vital Health Stat 13.1984;(80):1-69.
Google Scholar 4.Kroenke
KHoffman
REinstadter
D How common are various causes of dizziness? a critical review.
South Med J.2000;93:160-167.
Google Scholar 5.Halmagyi
GMCurthoys
ISCremer
PD
et al The human horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration stimulation before and after unilateral vestibular neurectomy.
Exp Brain Res.1990;81:479-490.
Google Scholar 6.Cremer
PDHalmagyi
GMAw
ST
et al Semicircular canal plane head impulses detect absent function of individual semicircular canals.
Brain.1998;121:699-716.
Google Scholar 7.Tabak
SCollewijn
HBoumans
LJvan der Steen
J Gain and delay of human vestibulo-ocular reflexes to oscillation and steps of the head by a reactive torque helmet, I: normal subjects.
Acta Otolaryngol.1997;117:785-795.
Google Scholar 8.Tabak
SCollewijn
HBoumans
LJvan der Steen
J Gain and delay of human vestibulo-ocular reflexes to oscillation and steps of the head by a reactive torque helmet, II: vestibular-deficient subjects.
Acta Otolaryngol.1997;117:796-809.
Google Scholar 9.Ewald
JR Physiologische Untersuchungen uber das Endorgan des Nervus Octavus. Wiesbaden, Germany: Bergmann; 1892.
10.Aw
STHalmagyi
GMCurthoys
ISTodd
MJYavor
RA Unilateral vestibular deafferentation causes permanent impairment of the human vertical vestibulo-ocular reflex in the pitch plane.
Exp Brain Res.1994;102:121-130.
Google Scholar 11.Fineberg
RO'Leary
DPDavis
LL Use of active head movements for computerized vestibular testing.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg.1987;113:1063-1065.
Google Scholar 12.O'Leary
DPDavis
LLMaceri
DR Vestibular autorotation test asymmetry analysis of acoustic neuroma.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg.1991;104:103-109.
Google Scholar 13.Hoffman
DLO'Leary
DPMunjack
DJ Autorotation test abnormalities of the horizontal and vertical vestibulo-ocular reflexes in panic disorder.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg.1994;110:259-269.
Google Scholar 14.Ng
MDavis
LLO'Leary
DP Autorotation test of the horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex in Meniere's disease.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg.1993;109:399-412.
Google Scholar 15.Meulenbroeks
AAKingma
HVan Twisk
JJVermeulen
MP Quantitative evaluation of the Vestibular Autorotation Test (VAT) in normal subjects.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl.1995;520(pt 2):327-333.
Google Scholar 16.Hirvonen
TPyykko
IJuhola
MAalto
H High frequency deficit of the vestibular system after acoustic neurinoma surgery.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl.1995;520(pt 1):16-18.
Google Scholar 17.Straumann
DZee
DSSolomon
DLasker
AGRoberts
DC Transient torsion during and after saccades.
Vision Res.1995;35:3321-3334.
Google Scholar 18.Haslwanter
T Mathematics of three-dimensional eye rotations.
Vision Res.1995;35:1727-1739.
Google Scholar 19.Demer
JLHonrubia
VBaloh
RW Dynamic visual activity: a test for oscillopsia and vestibulo-ocular reflex function.
Am J Otol.1994;15:340-347.
Google Scholar 20.Jenkins
HAHonrubia
VBaloh
RH Evaluation of multiple-frequency rotatory testing in patients with peripheral labyrinthine weakness.
Am J Otolaryngol.1982;3:182-188.
Google Scholar 21.Paige
GD Nonlinearity and asymmetry in the human vestibulo-ocular reflex.
Acta Otolaryngol.1989;108:1-8.
Google Scholar 22.Tusa
RJGrant
MPBuettner
UWHerdman
SJZee
DS The contribution of the vertical semicircular canals to high-velocity horizontal vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) in normal subjects and patients with unilateral vestibular nerve section.
Acta Otolaryngol.1996;116:507-512.
Google Scholar 23.Aw
STHaslwanter
THalmagyi
ISCurthoys
RAYavor
RATodd
MJ Three-dimensional vector analysis of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration head rotations, I: responses in normal subjects.
J Neurophysiol.1996;76:4009-4020.
Google Scholar 24.Aw
STHalmagyi
GMHaslwanter
TCurthoys
ISYavor
RATodd
MJ Three-dimensional vector analysis of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex in response to high-acceleration head rotations, II: responses in subjects with unilateral vestibular loss and selective semicircular canal occlusion.
J Neurophysiol.1996;76:4021-4030.
Google Scholar 25.Tian
JCrane
BTDemer
JL Vestibular catch-up saccades in labyrinthine deficiency.
Exp Brain Res.2000;131:448-457.
Google Scholar 26.Herdman
SJSchubert
MCTusa
RJ Role of central preprogramming in dynamic visual acuity with vestibular loss.
Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg.2001;127:1205-1210.
Google Scholar 27.Leigh
RJZee
DS The Neurology of Eye Movements. New York, NY: Oxford University Press Inc; 1999.
28.Tabak
SCollewijn
H Human vestibulo-ocular responses to rapid, helmet-driven head movements.
Exp Brain Res.1994;102:367-378.
Google Scholar 29.Collewijn
HSmeets
JB Early components of the human vestibulo-ocular response to head rotation: latency and gain.
J Neurophysiol.2000;84:376-389.
Google Scholar 30.Fischer
BRamsperger
E Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements.
Exp Brain Res.1984;57:191-195.
Google Scholar