[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Mean Hebrew Infant-Toddler Meaningful Integration scale of the connexin (Cx) and the non-Cx groups before implantation and 6 months after implantation. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Mean Hebrew Infant-Toddler Meaningful Integration scale of the connexin (Cx) and the non-Cx groups before implantation and 6 months after implantation. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Figure 2. 
Mean Hebrew Picture Speech Pattern Contrast scores for vowel height, vowel place, and initial manner for the connexin (Cx) and non-Cx groups with duration of implant use between 2 and 3 years. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Mean Hebrew Picture Speech Pattern Contrast scores for vowel height, vowel place, and initial manner for the connexin (Cx) and non-Cx groups with duration of implant use between 2 and 3 years. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Table 1. Description of Groups
Description of Groups
Table 2. Mean Speech Perception Open-Set Results
Mean Speech Perception Open-Set Results
1.
Estivill  XFortina  PSurrey  S  et al.  Connexin-26 mutations in sporadic and inherited sensorineural deafness.  Lancet 1998;351394- 398PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Van Camp  GWillems  PJSmith  RJ Nonsyndromic hearing impairment: unparalleled heterogeneity.  Am J Hum Genet 1997;60758- 764PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Van Camp  GSmith  RJH Hereditary Hearing Loss Web site.  Available at: http://webhost.ua.ac.be/hhh. Accessed 2005
4.
Kelsell  DPDunlop  JStevens  HP  et al.  Connexin 26 mutations in hereditary non-syndromic sensorineural deafness.  Nature 1997;38780- 83PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Kelley  PMAbe  SAskew  JWSmith  SDUsami  SKimberling  WJ Human connexin 30 (GJB6), a candidate gene for nonsyndromic hearing loss: molecular cloning, tissue-specific expression, and assignment to chromosome 13q12.  Genomics 1999;62172- 176PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Forge  ABecker  DCasalotti  S  et al.  Gap junctions and connexin expression in the inner ear.  Novartis Found Symp 1999;219134- 156PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Denoyelle  FMarlin  SWeil  D  et al.  Clinical features of the prevalent form of childhood deafness, DFNB1, due to a connexin-26 gene defect: implications for genetic counselling.  Lancet 1999;3531298- 1303PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Sobe  TVreugde  SShahin  H  et al.  The prevalence and expression of inherited connexin 26 mutations associated with nonsyndromic hearing loss in the Israeli population.  Hum Genet 2000;10650- 57PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Lerer  ISagi  MMalamud  ELevi  HRaas-Rothschild  AAbeliovich  D Contribution of connexin 26 mutations to nonsyndromic deafness in Ashkenazi patients and the variable phenotypic effect of the mutation 167delT.  Am J Med Genet 2000;9553- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Del Castillo  I A deletion involving the connexin 30 gene in nonsyndromic hearing impairment.  N Engl J Med 2002;346243- 249PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Lerer  ISagi  MBen-Neriah  ZWang  TLevi  HAbeliovich  D A deletion mutation in GJB6 cooperating with a GJB2 mutation in trans in non-syndromic deafness: a novel founder mutation in Ashkenazi Jews.  Hum Mutat 2001;18460- 469PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Del Castillo  IMoreno-Pelayo  MADel Castillo  FJ  et al.  Prevalence and evolutionary origins of the del(GJB6-D13S1830) mutation in the DFNB1 locus in hearing-impaired subjects: a multicenter study.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;731452- 1458PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Clark  GM Cochlear Implants.  Bologna, Italy: Monduzzi Editore; 1997
14.
Pyman  BBlamey  PLacy  PClark  GDowell  R The development of speech perception in children using cochlear implants: effects of etiologic factors and delayed milestones.  Am J Otol 2000;2157- 61PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Francis  HWPulsifer  MBChinnici  J  et al.  Effects of central nervous system residua on cochlear implant results in children deafened by meningitis.  Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130604- 611PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Matsushiro  NDoi  KFuse  Y  et al.  Successful cochlear implantation in prelingual profound deafness resulting from the common 233delC mutation of the GJB2 gene in the Japanese.  Laryngoscope 2002;112255- 261PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fukushima  KSugata  KKasai  N  et al.  Better speech performance in cochlear implant patients with GJB2-related deafness.  Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2002;62151- 157PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sinnathuray  ARToner  JGClarke-Lyttle  JGeddis  APatterson  CCHughes  AE Connexin 26 (GJB2) gene-related deafness and speech intelligibility after cochlear implantation.  Otol Neurotol 2004;25935- 942PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Sinnathuray  ARToner  JGGeddis  AClarke-Lyttle  JPatterson  CCHughes  AE Auditory perception and speech discrimination after cochlear implantation in patients with connexin 26 (GJB2) gene-related deafness.  Otol Neurotol 2004;25930- 934PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Dahl  HHWake  MSarant  JPoulakis  ZSiemering  KBlamey  P Language and speech perception outcomes in hearing-impaired children with and without connexin 26 mutations.  Audiol Neurootol 2003;8263- 268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Bauer  PWGeers  AEBrenner  CMoog  JSSmith  RJ The effect of GJB2 allele variants on performance after cochlear implantation.  Laryngoscope 2003;1132135- 2140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Green  GEScott  DAMcDonald  JM  et al.  Performance of cochlear implant recipients with GJB2-related deafness.  Am J Med Genet 2002;109167- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
McConkey Robbins  AKoch  DBOsberger  MJZimmerman-Phillips  SKishon-Rabin  L Effect of age at cochlear implantation on auditory skill development in infants and toddlers.  Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130570- 574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Kishon-Rabin  LTaitelbaum  RMuchnik  CGehtler  IKronenberg  JHildesheimer  M Development of speech perception and production in children with cochlear implants.  Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl 2002;18985- 90PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Cullen  RDBuchman  CABrown  CJ  et al.  Cochlear implantation for children with GJB2-related deafness.  Laryngoscope 2004;1141415- 1419PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
May 2006

Connexin-Associated Deafness and Speech Perception Outcome of Cochlear Implantation

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Departments of Communication Disorders (Ms Taitelbaum-Swead and Drs Muchnik, Kishon-Rabin, and Hildesheimer) and Human Molecular Genetics and Biochemistry (Drs Brownstein, Frydman, and Avraham), Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel; Speech and Hearing Center, Tel-Hashomer, Israel (Ms Taitelbaum-Swead and Drs Muchnik and Hildesheimer); Department of Otorhinolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery (Drs Kronenberg and Megirov) and The Danek Gertner Institute of Human Genetics (Dr Frydman), Chaim Sheba Medical Center, Tel-Hashomer.

Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2006;132(5):495-500. doi:10.1001/archotol.132.5.495
Abstract

Objective  To compare performance after cochlear implantation in children with mutations in connexin (Cx) 26 (GJB2) or Cx30 (GJB6) and children with deafness of unknown etiology.

Design  Genetic analysis and speech perception evaluation was performed in the children with and without Cx mutations who had undergone cochlear implantation. Speech perception performance was retrospectively analyzed 6, 12, 24, 36, and 48 months after implantation. Test material was selected according to the child's age and cognitive and language abilities.

Setting  The study took place at speech and hearing and genetic centers of a hospital in the central part of Israel and the genetics departments of 3 additional centrally located hospitals.

Patients  A total of 30 children who had undergone cochlear implantation were selected for the study, with control patients matched according to age at implantation, duration of implant use, and mode of communication. There was no evidence for additional disabilities or handicaps in either group.

Main Outcome Measures  Speech perception measurements included a questionnaire, as well as closed and open-set tests.

Results  Overall, the 2 groups showed significant improvement in speech perception results after implantation. Four years after implantation, both groups achieved mean open-set speech perception scores of approximately 60%, 75%, and 90% for monosyllabic, 2 syllables, and words in sentences tests, respectively.

Conclusions  There were no apparent differences in speech perception performance after implantation between the children with Cx mutations and children with deafness of unknown etiology. These data have important implications as a prognostic indicator when counseling candidates for cochlear implantation.

Hearing impairment affects approximately 1 in 1000 newborns and 4% of individuals younger than 45 years.1 Inherited hearing loss (HL) accounts for at least 60% of patients with deafness, of whom HL is syndromic in 30% and nonsyndromic in 70%. The most common form of nonsyndromic HL is autosomal recessive, which accounts for about 80% of cases.2 More than 100 genes are thought to be involved in HL. Thus far, 96 loci have been mapped and 43 cloned.3 The genes known to date to be involved in human hereditary nonsyndromic HL encode a large variety of proteins, including transcription factors, ion channels, molecular motors, gap junctions, and proteins that form the extracellular matrix of the inner ear.

The first discovery of an autosomal recessive gene, GJB2, was reported in 1997.4GJB2 encodes the gap-junction protein connexin (Cx) 26, which belongs to a family of more than 20 members who share a common structure of 4 transmembrane segments. Most cell types express more than 1 Cx species, which can form homomeric or heteromeric connexons. Intercellular channels in the auditory system are formed predominantly by Cx26 but also by Cx30 and Cx31.5 Connexin 26 appears to play a role in maintaining a high extracellular electrical potential in the cochlea by facilitating the circulation of K+ ions.6 A surprising finding was that despite the extreme genetic heterogeneity of deafness, this gene is responsible for a high proportion of nonsyndromic HL cases. Mutations in GJB2 are responsible for up to 50% of cases of severe to profound prelingual recessive deafness in several worldwide populations.7 In Israel they account for 38.7% of such cases in the general deaf population8 and for 70.4% of cases among Ashkenazi Jews.9 In the latter population, a mutation has also been described in the GJB6 gene encoding the protein Cx30, which is coexpressed with Cx26 in the inner ear. GJB6 is located on chromosome 13, within 50 kb of GJB2. A mutation encountered in the Israeli population, del(GJB6-D13S1830), leaves the GJB2 coding region intact but deletes a large region close to GJB2 and truncates GJB6. This deletion is frequently found in double heterozygosity with a GJB2 mutation, and the associated HL is assumed to be caused either by the deletion of a putative GJB2 regulatory element or by digenic inheritance.10 Pure digenic inheritance seems unlikely, however, because double heterozygosity with a GJB2 mutation has not been detected with other GJB6 mutations. Double heterozygotes for Cx26 and Cx30 mutations are associated with profound HL and manifest the same phenotypes as homozygotes for Cx26 and for Cx30.10-12

Cochlear implantation is a common rehabilitation option for the population with severe to profound HL. Performance with cochlear implants is highly variable and depends on many factors, such as age of implantation, amount of residual hearing, and mode of communication. The contribution of these factors to speech perception abilities has been documented and was found to explain less than 50% of the variance in the results.13 The remaining variability, therefore, is clearly attributable to additional factors.

One factor likely to affect the results of cochlear implantation is the underlying cause of deafness. It is generally assumed that speech perception performance after cochlear implantation might be poorer in underlying causes known to include neural and/or central damage to the auditory system (eg, cytomegalovirus, meningitis, and/or auditory neuropathy)14,15 than in those primarily affecting the hair cells (eg, hereditary nonsyndromic deafness). One such cause, which is known to affect the cochlea, is genetic mutations in GJB2 or GJB6 genes, leading to nonsyndromic HL.

In several studies the postimplantation speech perception in children with GJB2 mutations has been compared with that of a control group, but the overall results have been inconclusive. In a study using the Infant-Toddler–Meaningful Auditory Integration Scale (IT-MAIS) questionnaire, 6 months after implantation the results in the Cx26 group were significantly better compared with those of children without Cx mutations.16 In another study there was a tendency toward better results in the Cx26 group than in the control group, but this was not significant.17 Sinnathury et al recently reported better speech intelligibility18 and better auditory perception results19 in the Cx26 group, whereas other authors have obtained similar speech perception results in the 2 groups.20,21 Two studies found higher reading comprehension scores in the Cx26 group compared with the non-Cx26 group.21,22 The large variability in the collected data can be attributed to several factors, including the small number of participants, nonidentical criteria for selection of the control group, and other confounding factors such as matching of age at implantation and the duration of deafness.

The purpose of this study was to compare the postimplantation performance of a relatively large group of children with GJB2 or GJB6 mutations with those of a control group with deafness of unknown etiology. The control group was carefully matched with the experimental group with respect to the factors known to confound the results of previous investigations. Our results demonstrate that there is no inherent advantage in harboring Cx mutations on speech perception outcome.

Methods
Subjects

A total of 250 (between 1993-2004) children underwent cochlear implantation at the Sheba Medical Center, Tel-Hashomer, Israel. Of these, 49 children with congenital HL and cochlear implants underwent genetic analysis in one of the centers: the Genetic Counseling Center of the Chaim Sheba Medical Center (Tel-Hashomer) and the Genetic Departments of the Rabin Medical Center (Petach Tikva), Sourasky Medical Center (Tel Aviv), and the Wolfson Medical Center (Holon) in Israel. A complete clinical history of each affected individual was collected to ensure that no environmental factors were involved. From the 49 children, only those whose native language was Hebrew and had no suspicion of additional problems were included in the present study. After this screening, 30 subjects participated in the study. Of these subjects, 17 had GJB2 and/or GJB6 mutations and 13 were in the non-Cx group. Of this control group, 7 cases were sporadic and 6 were familial (more than 1 subject in the family with HL). The children in the 2 groups were carefully matched according to age at implantation, duration of implant use, mode of communication, and hearing thresholds preimplantation. Table 1 illustrates the background data of the 2 groups (Cx group and the control group).

The project was approved by the institutional review board committees at Tel Aviv University, Chaim Sheba Medical Center, Rabin Medical Center, Sourasky Medical Center, and the Wolfson Medical Center. Blood samples were obtained following informed consent from the parents of each individual, since they are all younger than 18 years.

MUTATION ANALYSIS OF Cx26 (GJB2) AND Cx30 (GJB6) GENES

The open reading frame of GJB2 was examined for mutations as described previously.8 Briefly, primers GJB2-1F, 5′-TCT TTT CCA GAG CAA ACC GC-3′, and GJB2-2R, 5′-GGG CAA TGC GTT AAA CTG GC-3′, amplified a 722–base pair (bp) fragment that was sequenced and checked for mutations. In addition, the noncoding exon 1 of GJB2 was screened for the splice site mutation IVS1 + 1(G→A). Primers Cx26-Exon1F 5′-GGCGACACCACAAACCTC-3′ and Cx26-Exon1R 5′- CCTCCGTAACTTTCCCAGTC-3′ amplified a 540-bp fragment. The polymerase chain reaction product was digested with BspMI (37°C for 10 hours) and separated by electrophoresis on a 2% gel. The BspMI restriction enzyme cuts the wild-type fragment into 2 fragments of 309 bp and 231 bp. A mutation eliminates the BspMI site, and thus the expected band size is 540 bp for the mutant allele.

For GJB6, the deletion Δ(GJB6-D13S1830) identified in Jewish Ashkenazi and Spanish hearing-impaired individuals10,11 was investigated in our DNA samples. Primers GJB6-1R, 5′-TTT AGG GCA TGA TTG GGG TGA TTT-3′, designed 244 bp upstream of the proximal breakpoint of the deletion and GJB6-BKR-1, 5′-CAC CAT GCG TAG CCT TAA CCA TTT T-3′, localized 216 bp downstream of the distal breakpoint of the deletion, amplified a 460-bp fragment encompassing the breakpoint deletion if it was present. To obtain a fragment in the event no deletion occurred, we designed a third primer localized 681 bp downstream of the GJB6-1R primer, to be included in the same reaction, GJB6-RVS2, 5′-TCA TCG GGG GTG TCA ACA AAC A-3′. Wild-type DNA yielded a 681-bp band.

Speech perception evaluation

The tests administered to each child were determined based on age and cognitive and language abilities. We used a questionnaire for the infants as well as closed- and open-set tests for the older children. The questionnaire included the Hebrew Infant Toddler Meaningful Auditory Integration Scale (HIT-MAIS).23 The Phonological Contrasts test included the Hebrew Picture Speech Pattern Contrast test (HePiSPAC).24 Three subtests included vowel height and place and initial manner. All stimuli are consonant-vocal-consonant (CVC) meaningful words. The open-set tests included 1-syllable CVC isophonemic meaningful word list tests, which are scored by words and phonemes. Open-set 2-syllable word test included lists of 2 syllable isophonemic meaningful word lists. Open-set words in the sentences test included lists of common sentences.

Procedure

The results of the speech perception tests were collected retrospectively. Speech perception abilities were measured at 6, 12, 24, 36, and 48 months after implantation. Note that not all subjects were available at all times. Infants younger than 2.5 years were usually evaluated with the HIT-MAIS. Children 2.5 years or older, who were able to respond via the auditory channel alone, were tested with the open-set tests. Children 2.5 years or older were also evaluated by the HePiSPAC closed-set test. All speech perception tests were presented at 70-dB sound pressure level via live voice, with a distance of 1 meter between the loudspeaker and listener.

Results
Genetic results

Biallelic Cx mutations were detected in the GJB2 and/or the GJB6 genes in 17 children. Five children were homozygous for the 167delT mutation, which is common among the Ashkenazi population in Israel,9 2 were homozygous for the 35delG mutation, 7 had compound 35delG/167delT mutations, and 3 were double heterozygous for both GJB2 and GJB6 35delG/del(GJB6-D13S1830) mutations.

Speech perception results

Mean HIT-MAIS results of the 2 groups before implantation and 6 months after implantation are illustrated in Figure 1. Both groups showed improvement in the scores of the questionnaire 6 months after implantation. The 2 groups showed similar mean scores of approximately 80%. Statistical analysis using the nonparametric Mann-Whitney test revealed no significant differences (P>.05) between the groups. Only small groups (n = 7) of children who had undergone cochlear implantation were evaluated by the HIT-MAIS questionnaire. Infants who cannot be evaluated by other direct speech perception tests are tested with this questionnaire.

Mean HePiSPAC scores of 3 phonological contrasts (vowel height, vowel place, and initial manner) in experienced children with cochlear implants (implant use of 2-3 years) from the 2 groups are shown in Figure 2. Statistical analysis using the nonparametric Mann-Whitney test revealed no significant differences (P>.05) between the groups in initial manner and vowel height. Vowel place scores were higher in the non-Cx group. These results were with P values close to statistical significance (P = .06). Furthermore, the same hierarchy in speech contrasts can be observed for the 2 groups. Vowel place was the easiest contrast to perceive, while initial manner was the most difficult from the 3 contrasts we evaluated.

Mean open-set speech perception results and standard deviations of the 2 groups 1, 2, 3, and 4 years after implantation are summarized in Table 2. Two analyses of variance, one by group (Cx and non-Cx) and one by duration of implant use (1, 2, 3, and 4 years), were conducted with a element model. There were no statistical differences between groups in either of the tests, as shown in Table 2. In most of the speech perception tests, there was longitudinal improvement in the results after implantation. The statistical analyses revealed a near-significant effect of duration of implant use in Hebrew AB phonemes (F3,15 = 2.92, P = .06) and a significant effect of time since implantation in word scores (F3,15 = 3.08, P = .05) and words in sentences scores (F2,6 = 6.66, P = .03).

Comment

The main purpose of the present study was to retrospectively compare speech perception results of children with and without Cx mutations who had undergone cochlear implantation. The results indicate that when these 2 groups of children are carefully matched by many variables, there are no apparent differences in speech perception scores between the 2 groups. Both groups show improvement in speech perception abilities after implantation. This was found for all speech perception measures, for the questionnaire in the very young children, as well as for the closed-set and the open-set tests in the older children. Our results in the present study support the findings obtained in earlier Cx studies.20,25 However, these results differ from those obtained in other studies,16-19 which found better results in the Cx group. These differences can be attributed to confounding factors such as the small number of subjects and criteria for selecting the control group.

Specifically, results of the HIT-MAIS questionnaire showed that infants in both groups reached an average score of approximately 80% within 6 months after implantation. On the other hand, the study by Matsushiro et al16 obtained mean results of approximately 70% in the Cx group 6 months after implantation when using the IT-MAIS but found significantly lower results (mean of 44%) in the non-Cx group. These differences in results can be partly attributed to the older age at implantation in the study by Matsushiro et al,16 particularly in the non-Cx group. Most of our subjects in the subgroups who were evaluated by the IT-MAIS underwent implantation at an age younger than 2 years, compared with a mean age of 45.3 months in the study by Matsushiro et al16; this may explain our better results at 6 months after implantation. Our findings are consistent with the published data of McConkey Robbins et al,23 who found, using the IT-MAIS questionnaire, that infants and toddlers who had undergone cochlear implantation showed rapid improvement in auditory skills during the first year of device use and that younger infants achieve higher scores.

The perception of phonological contrasts of the 2 groups was approximately 100%, 85%, and 75% for vowel place, vowel height, and initial manner, respectively. The highest score for vowel place is similar to the results of Kishon-Rabin et al,24 who found that vowel place is the best contrast to produce and perceive by Hebrew cochlear implants users.

The results from the open-set battery of tests for the older children who had undergone cochlear implantation showed improvement in the 2 groups after implantation. Our mean results in the Cx group 4 years after implantation was 66%, 83%, and 85% for monosyllabic word score, phoneme score, and words in sentences, respectively. These results are similar to the results obtained in the literature of children with Cx mutations who had undergone implantation. In the study by Fukushima et al,17 the mean monosyllabic words score in the Cx26 group (3 subjects) was approximately 66%. The mean result in the non-Cx26 group (4 subjects) was lower. This can be attributed to the results of 2 of 4 patients who received low scores, but their cause of deafness was low birth weight, which may be associated with other deficits.

These differences in the results strengthened our conviction that when such studies are designed, the control group (the group with non-Cx mutations) should be as similar as possible to the group with Cx mutations. Otherwise, the results can be slanting by confounding factors. In the Chaim Sheba Medical Center, there are more than 250 children who had undergone cochlear implantation, but only a part of them have a “pure” HL without any other difficulties, including learning disabilities, dyspraxia, and motor delay. Thus, the findings of the present study provide evidence that when we carefully select “clean” groups that differ only in the cause of deafness, the performance with the implant is similar. In our control group, although the cause of deafness is unknown, it is likely that many of these cases have a genetic cause, particularly in those cases in which more than 1 individual in the family has HL. At this time, the cost-benefit of screening for mutations in genes other than GJB2 and GJB6 is low owing to the large size of most known genes and the lack of known frequencies in a given population for most of the known genes. Most important, the responsible gene for almost 50 mapped loci has yet to be identified, and there are undoubtedly more loci to be discovered. Furthermore, we would expect cochlear implantation outcome to be similar for deafness due to a nongenetic cause that involves sensory HL but not central auditory pathways.

These results have implications and prognostic value regarding counseling for cochlear implant candidates with Cx mutations, as well as for other nonsyndromic HL cases with no additional complications. Overall, the results were the same in both our study groups and were found to be above average in performance.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Karen B. Avraham, PhD, Department of Human Molecular Genetics and Biochemistry, Sackler School of Medicine, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel (karena@post.tau.ac.il).

Submitted for Publication: July 17, 2005; final revision received October 5, 2005; accepted November 2, 2005.

Author Contributions: Ms Taitelbaum-Swead and Dr Brownstein contributed equally to this study.

Financial Disclosure: None.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant RO1 DC005641 from the National Institutes of Health, grant LSHG-CT-20054-512063 from the European Commission FP6 Integrated Project EUROHEAR, and a gift from B. and A. Hirschfield.

Additional Information: This work was performed by Dr Brownstein in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a PhD degree, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Tel Aviv University.

Acknowledgment: We thank Tamar Kaufman, BA, and Hen Haiman, BA, for collecting the data; Orna Segal, MA, Shira Cohen, MA, and Yifaat Yaar-Sofer, MA, from the cochlear implant team at the Chaim Sheba Medical Center, for their contribution in evaluating the children; Eti Shabtai, MSc, for the statistical analyses; and Mordechai Shohat, MD, Dorit Lev, MD, Avi Orr-Utreger, MD, PhD, for patient ascertainment.

References
1.
Estivill  XFortina  PSurrey  S  et al.  Connexin-26 mutations in sporadic and inherited sensorineural deafness.  Lancet 1998;351394- 398PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Van Camp  GWillems  PJSmith  RJ Nonsyndromic hearing impairment: unparalleled heterogeneity.  Am J Hum Genet 1997;60758- 764PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Van Camp  GSmith  RJH Hereditary Hearing Loss Web site.  Available at: http://webhost.ua.ac.be/hhh. Accessed 2005
4.
Kelsell  DPDunlop  JStevens  HP  et al.  Connexin 26 mutations in hereditary non-syndromic sensorineural deafness.  Nature 1997;38780- 83PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Kelley  PMAbe  SAskew  JWSmith  SDUsami  SKimberling  WJ Human connexin 30 (GJB6), a candidate gene for nonsyndromic hearing loss: molecular cloning, tissue-specific expression, and assignment to chromosome 13q12.  Genomics 1999;62172- 176PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Forge  ABecker  DCasalotti  S  et al.  Gap junctions and connexin expression in the inner ear.  Novartis Found Symp 1999;219134- 156PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Denoyelle  FMarlin  SWeil  D  et al.  Clinical features of the prevalent form of childhood deafness, DFNB1, due to a connexin-26 gene defect: implications for genetic counselling.  Lancet 1999;3531298- 1303PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Sobe  TVreugde  SShahin  H  et al.  The prevalence and expression of inherited connexin 26 mutations associated with nonsyndromic hearing loss in the Israeli population.  Hum Genet 2000;10650- 57PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Lerer  ISagi  MMalamud  ELevi  HRaas-Rothschild  AAbeliovich  D Contribution of connexin 26 mutations to nonsyndromic deafness in Ashkenazi patients and the variable phenotypic effect of the mutation 167delT.  Am J Med Genet 2000;9553- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Del Castillo  I A deletion involving the connexin 30 gene in nonsyndromic hearing impairment.  N Engl J Med 2002;346243- 249PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Lerer  ISagi  MBen-Neriah  ZWang  TLevi  HAbeliovich  D A deletion mutation in GJB6 cooperating with a GJB2 mutation in trans in non-syndromic deafness: a novel founder mutation in Ashkenazi Jews.  Hum Mutat 2001;18460- 469PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Del Castillo  IMoreno-Pelayo  MADel Castillo  FJ  et al.  Prevalence and evolutionary origins of the del(GJB6-D13S1830) mutation in the DFNB1 locus in hearing-impaired subjects: a multicenter study.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;731452- 1458PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Clark  GM Cochlear Implants.  Bologna, Italy: Monduzzi Editore; 1997
14.
Pyman  BBlamey  PLacy  PClark  GDowell  R The development of speech perception in children using cochlear implants: effects of etiologic factors and delayed milestones.  Am J Otol 2000;2157- 61PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Francis  HWPulsifer  MBChinnici  J  et al.  Effects of central nervous system residua on cochlear implant results in children deafened by meningitis.  Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130604- 611PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Matsushiro  NDoi  KFuse  Y  et al.  Successful cochlear implantation in prelingual profound deafness resulting from the common 233delC mutation of the GJB2 gene in the Japanese.  Laryngoscope 2002;112255- 261PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fukushima  KSugata  KKasai  N  et al.  Better speech performance in cochlear implant patients with GJB2-related deafness.  Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2002;62151- 157PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sinnathuray  ARToner  JGClarke-Lyttle  JGeddis  APatterson  CCHughes  AE Connexin 26 (GJB2) gene-related deafness and speech intelligibility after cochlear implantation.  Otol Neurotol 2004;25935- 942PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Sinnathuray  ARToner  JGGeddis  AClarke-Lyttle  JPatterson  CCHughes  AE Auditory perception and speech discrimination after cochlear implantation in patients with connexin 26 (GJB2) gene-related deafness.  Otol Neurotol 2004;25930- 934PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Dahl  HHWake  MSarant  JPoulakis  ZSiemering  KBlamey  P Language and speech perception outcomes in hearing-impaired children with and without connexin 26 mutations.  Audiol Neurootol 2003;8263- 268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Bauer  PWGeers  AEBrenner  CMoog  JSSmith  RJ The effect of GJB2 allele variants on performance after cochlear implantation.  Laryngoscope 2003;1132135- 2140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Green  GEScott  DAMcDonald  JM  et al.  Performance of cochlear implant recipients with GJB2-related deafness.  Am J Med Genet 2002;109167- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
McConkey Robbins  AKoch  DBOsberger  MJZimmerman-Phillips  SKishon-Rabin  L Effect of age at cochlear implantation on auditory skill development in infants and toddlers.  Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2004;130570- 574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Kishon-Rabin  LTaitelbaum  RMuchnik  CGehtler  IKronenberg  JHildesheimer  M Development of speech perception and production in children with cochlear implants.  Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl 2002;18985- 90PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Cullen  RDBuchman  CABrown  CJ  et al.  Cochlear implantation for children with GJB2-related deafness.  Laryngoscope 2004;1141415- 1419PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×