Importance
Because exposure to violence, crime, and abuse has been shown to have serious consequences on child development, physicians and policymakers need to know the kinds of exposure that occur at various developmental stages.
Objectives
To provide updated estimates of and trends for childhood exposure to a broad range of violence, crime, and abuse victimizations.
Design
The National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence was based on a cross-sectional, US national telephone survey conducted in 2011.
Setting
Interviews by telephone.
Participants
The experiences of 4503 children and youth aged 1 month to 17 years were assessed by interviews with caregivers and with youth in the case of those aged 10 to 17 years.
Results
Two-fifths (41.2%) of children and youth experienced a physical assault in the last year, and 1 in 10 (10.1%) experienced an assault-related injury. Two percent experienced sexual assault or sexual abuse in the last year, but the rate was 10.7% for girls aged 14 to 17 years. More than 1 in 10 (13.7%) experienced maltreatment by a caregiver, including 3.7% who experienced physical abuse. Few significant changes could be detected in rates since an equivalent survey in 2008, but declines were documented in peer flashing, school bomb threats, juvenile sibling assault, and robbery and total property victimization.
Conclusions and Relevance
The variety and scope of children’s exposure to violence, crime, and abuse suggest the need for better and more comprehensive tools in clinical and research settings for identifying these experiences and their effects.
Evidence continues to accumulate that there are serious consequences to health and well-being and society from childhood exposure to violence and abuse.1,2 Child maltreatment, peer victimization, and exposure to family and community violence have been shown to be connected to developmental difficulties, problem behavior, and physical and mental health effects extending throughout the life span.3-6 However, the epidemiology of child victimization remains fragmented,7 with published studies8,9 on limited portions of the age and exposure spectrum and only occasionally with a national scope. Controversies persist about the most common forms of victimization, age of maximum exposure across type, and trends over time.10-12
In an effort to improve the epidemiology and make it more comprehensive, the US Department of Justice and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention have combined resources to support a more regular and systematic national assessment of children’s exposure to violence, crime, and abuse. In 2008, the first such assessment, the National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence I (NatSCEV I), was conducted.13 The next wave in this assessment, conducted in 2011, has been completed. This article provides updated epidemiology on the exposure of children to violence, crime, and abuse based on those data.
The NatSCEV II was designed to obtain up-to-date incidence and prevalence estimates of a wide range of childhood victimizations. It consists of a national sample of 4503 children and youth aged 1 month to 17 years in 2011. Study interviews were conducted over the telephone by the employees of an experienced survey research firm. Telephone interviewing is a cost-effective method14,15 that has been demonstrated to be comparable with in-person interviews in data quality, even for reports of victimization, psychopathology, and other sensitive topics.16-21 In fact, some evidence suggests that telephone interviews are perceived by respondents as more anonymous, less intimidating, and more private than in-person modes16,22 and, as a result, may encourage greater disclosure of victimization events.16All procedures were authorized by the institutional review board of the University of New Hampshire.
The primary foundation of the study design was a nationwide sampling frame of residential telephone numbers from which a sample of telephone households was drawn by random digit dialing. Two additional samples were obtained to represent the growing number of households that rely entirely or mostly on cell phones, including a small national sample of cellular telephone numbers drawn from the random digit dialing method (n = 31) and an address-based sample (n = 750). The address-based sample started with a national sample of addresses from the postal Delivery Sequence File. These addresses were mailed a 1-page questionnaire. The address-based sample was drawn from the pool of returned questionnaires that represented households with children and youth 17 years or younger. These households were then recontacted by interviewers and asked to participate in the survey. Approximately one-half of the eligible households obtained through the address-based sample were cell phone–only households and represented an effective way to include households without landlines in our sample.
A short interview was conducted with an adult caregiver (usually a parent) to obtain family demographic information. One child was then randomly selected from all eligible children living in a household by selecting the child with the most recent birthday. If the selected individual was aged 10 to 17 years, the main telephone interview was conducted with the child. If the selected child was younger than 10 years, the interview was conducted with the caregiver who “is most familiar with the child’s daily routine and experiences.”
Respondents were promised complete confidentiality and were paid $20 for their participation. The interviews, averaging 55 minutes in length, were conducted in English or Spanish. Respondents who disclosed a situation of serious threat or ongoing victimization were recontacted by a clinical member of the research team, trained in telephone crisis counseling, whose responsibility was to stay in contact with the respondent until the situation was appropriately addressed locally.
Averaged across collection modalities, the cooperation rate was 60%, and the response rate was 40.4%. These are good rates by current survey research standards23-25 given the steady declines in response rates that have occurred during the past 3 decades26 and the particular marked drop in recent years.24,27,28 Although the potential for response bias remains an important consideration, several recent studies29-32 have shown no meaningful association between response rates and response bias.
This survey used an enhanced version of the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire. This instrument obtains an inventory of childhood victimization.33-35
The enhanced version of the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire obtained reports on 54 forms of offenses against youth that cover the following 6 general areas of concern: sexual assault, child maltreatment, conventional crime, Internet victimization, peer and sibling victimization, and witnessing and indirect victimization. Follow-up questions for each screening item gathered additional information, including the use of a weapon and perpetrator characteristics, as well as whether injury resulted and whether the event occurred in conjunction with another screening event. Because different kinds of victimizations can occur together and can overlap by definition (eg, physical abuse by a caretaker can also be an assault with or without injury), rates reported for victimizations in this article reflect considerable rescoring of these data provided by the screening items and follow-up questions. Specific screening items reflecting the 54 types of events are given in eAppendix 1, and definitions of the rescored victimizations and aggregates are given in eAppendix 2 (Supplement).
The survey instrument used in the NatSCEV II included several new screening items that were not included in the NatSCEV I in 2008. Rates shown for the NatSCEV II reflect the incorporation of the new screening information in rates; however, comparisons of rates with 2008 were based only on data from screening items that were used in both surveys.
The weighting plan for the survey was a multistage sequential process of weighting the sample to correct for study design and demographic variations in nonresponse. Specifically, weights were applied to adjust for (1) differing probabilities of household selection based on sampling frames, (2) variations in within-household selection resulting from different numbers of eligible children across households, and (3) differences in sample proportions according to sex, age, income, census region, race/ethnicity, number of adults and children in the household, and telephone status (cell only, mostly cell, or other) relative to the 2010 American Community Survey Public Use Microdata Sample.
Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 give the exposure rates for 5 major domains, including assaults and bullying, sexual victimization, maltreatment by a caregiver, property victimization, and witnessing victimization. Each table summarizes rates of exposure for the last year in total and broken down by sex and age. They also give rates of lifetime exposure in total, by sex, and for those aged 14 to 17 years. Finally, they list changes in current rates compared with the NatSCEV I (2008) rates.
Two of 5 children (41.2%) were physically assaulted during the last year (Table 1). One in 10 children (10.1%) was injured. Siblings and nonsibling peers were common perpetrators. Also common were physical intimidation (13.7%) and relational aggression (36.5%), terminology we use instead of the more common terms of physical and emotional bullying, which in their technical definition require a “power imbalance” in the relationship between victim and perpetrator. Some specific kinds of assaults occurred in smaller groups of youth, including bias attack to 1.8%, assault by gang or group to 1.7%, attempted or completed kidnapping to 0.6%, and dating violence to 3.2% of youth older than 12 years. Boys experienced more assaults overall (45.2% vs 37.1% for girls). Compared with girls, boys had particularly disproportionate levels of assault with injury (13.0% vs 7.1%), assault by gang or group (2.5% vs 0.9%), and nonsexual assault to the genitals (9.3% vs 1.0%). Compared with boys, girls were targets of more dating violence (4.7% vs 1.9%). Assault with injury, dating violence, and nonsexual assault to the genitals were higher among the oldest youth (those aged 14-17 years). Assault by peer tended to be most common during middle childhood.
Bullying-type victimizations are summarized in Table 1. Relational aggression and Internet or cell phone harassment were higher for girls. Physical intimidation was highest for children younger than 10 years, and relational aggression was highest for those aged 6 to 9 years. Internet or cell phone harassment was highest for those aged 14 to 17 years.
The overall estimate for assault in 2011 was down 2.2 percentage points compared with 2008, and most specific forms of assault also showed declines. However, except for the decline in lifetime exposure to sibling assault, none of the changes in assault from 2008 to 2011 were statistically significant.
Almost 6% (5.6%) of the total sample experienced a sexual victimization in the last year, and 2.2% experienced a sexual assault (Table 2). (Sexual assault excludes sexual harassment and includes attempted and completed rape, plus contact sex offenses by adults and peers. It is equivalent to contact sexual abuse.) Rates were considerably higher for girls aged 14 to 17 years (the highest-risk group), 22.8% of whom experienced a sexual victimization and 10.7% of whom experienced a sexual assault in the last year. Among this group, 8.1% had reported an attempted or completed rape, 13.6% experienced sexual harassment, and 12.9% were exposed to an unwanted Internet sexual solicitation in the last year.
There is considerable focus in the literature on the lifetime risk of sexual assault and victimization. The NatSCEV II lifetime estimates for youth aged 14 to 17 years (who have almost completed childhood) by sex are given in Table 2: 17.4% of the older girls and 4.2% of the older boys said they had experienced a sexual assault during childhood. Completed rape occurred in 3.6% of girls and 0.4% of boys. Sexual assault by a known adult occurred in 5.9% of girls and 0.3% of boys. Sexual assault by an unknown adult occurred in 3.8% of girls and 0.1% of boys. One category of sexual victimization, peer flashing, saw a significant decline since 2008.
Any child maltreatment (summarized in Table 3) included neglect, physical abuse, emotional abuse, custodial interference, and sexual abuse by a known adult; overall, 13.8% of the sample experienced such maltreatment in the last year. The lifetime rate of child maltreatment for the oldest subgroup was 41.2%. Emotional abuse by a caregiver was the most frequent, with the last-year rate being 8.0% for the total sample and the lifetime rate being 25.7% for those aged 14 to 17 years. Physical abuse by a caregiver occurred in 3.7% of the full sample in the last year and in 18.2% of those aged 14 to 17 years during their lifetimes. Neglect occurred in 6.5% during the last year in the full sample and in 22.3% over the lifetime for those aged 14 to 17 years. Sex differences were evident only for physical abuse, with boys experiencing somewhat higher rates (4.5% vs 2.9% for girls). Rates of physical abuse and emotional abuse were significantly higher for older children. There were no significant trends in child maltreatment from 2008 to 2011.
Property victimization (Table 4), consisting of robbery, vandalism, and theft, occurred in 24.1% of children and youth during the last year. Vandalism was more common for boys (8.7% vs 4.8% for girls). Theft was more common among older youth. Property victimization as a whole and robbery specifically declined significantly since 2008.
Almost one-quarter of the sample (22.4%) had witnessed violence in the last year in the family or in the community (Table 5). In total, 8.2% had witnessed a family assault, and 6.1% had witnessed a parent assault another parent (or parental partner) in the last year. The lifetime rate of witnessing any family assault among the oldest youth was 34.5%, and 28.3% had witnessed an interparental assault in their lifetimes. There were few significant sex or age differences in the witnessing of family assaults.
In the case of witnessing a community assault (Table 5), rates for all children and youth were 16.9% in the last year and 58.9% over the lifetime of the oldest youth. Lifetime exposure to shooting was 16.8% for this oldest group of youth, but exposure to warfare was only 2.0%. In total, 7.9% of all children and youth had been exposed to household theft in the last year, and 3.7% had experienced a bomb threat in their school. Last-year and lifetime exposure to bomb threats significantly declined since 2008.
Overall, 57.7% of the children and youth had experienced or witnessed at least 1 of 5 aggregate exposures (assaults and bullying, sexual victimization, maltreatment by a caregiver, property victimization, or witnessing victimization) in the year before this survey. It was also common to have had multiple exposures. In total, 48.4% had more than 1 of 50 possible specific victimization types involving direct or witnessed victimization; 15.1% had 6 or more, and 4.9% had 10 or more.
Exposure to one type increased the likelihood that a child or youth had exposures to other types as well. As summarized in Table 6, in most cases risk for an additional type of exposure was increased by a factor of 2 or 3 for a last-year exposure and somewhat more for a lifetime exposure. For example, having a last-year assault was associated with a 2.7 times higher likelihood of sexual victimization and a 2.9 times higher likelihood of caregiver maltreatment. There were no combinations for which this risk amplification did not occur.
Rates reported herein from 2011 have been compared with rates from 2008, the year of the first NatSCEV, which was also based on a nationally representative sample of children and youth aged 1 month to 17 years.13 Comparisons are shown in Tables 1 through 5. The comparisons for 5 aggregate types of exposure (assaults and bullying, sexual victimization, maltreatment by a caregiver, property victimization, and witnessing victimization) suggest more stability than change. The percentage experiencing any of the 5 direct and witnessed aggregate types in the last year fell by 2.3 percentage points, but the change was not significant. Among the specific types of exposures, declines somewhat outnumbered increases, but there were only 6 types for which decreases reached significance during the last year or the lifetime. Assault by juvenile siblings (lifetime) declined, school bomb threats (last year and lifetime) declined, property victimization (last year) and robbery (last year) declined, and flashing by a peer and statutory sex offense (last year and lifetime) declined.
This study reinforces numerous previous studies37-39 showing that children and youth are frequently exposed to violence, crime, and abuse on an annual basis and over the course of their childhoods. However, what is unique about the NatSCEV is its ability to provide more precise epidemiology for this exposure, breaking it down by various distinct and sometimes overlapping types, as well as by age and by last-year and lifetime rates, to suit various needs. Researchers and policymakers who want to focus on the most serious exposures can do so, but so can those who want a more comprehensive picture that aggregates across types.
Therefore, some may be most interested in the 10.1% of children and youth who are injured by violence in the course of a year or in those who were sexually assaulted by a known adult (0.5% of children and youth for the last year and 3.0% for the lifetime as reported by those aged 14 to 17 years). Others may prefer a rate for a sample that includes all physical assaults (41.2% of children and youth for the last year) or all sexual assaults (2.2% of children and youth for the last year and 10.6% for the lifetime of the older youth).
The comprehensiveness also allows an unusual perspective on the degree to which some children and youth experience multiple exposures and that seem to be highly correlated. Eleven percent of youth in the sample had 6 or more direct victimizations (excluding witnessing) in a single year, a highly vulnerable segment of youth we have labeled as “polyvictims,” who appear prone to distress, many adversities, and other problems.40,41
The NatSCEV model also allows researchers and policymakers to track trends over time and to monitor the possible effects of social changes and public policies. Overall, more rate changes from 2008 and 2011 moved down than up, but few of the changes were large enough to be detectable as significant with the sample sizes available. There were significant downward trends in lifetime exposure to last-year peer flashing, juvenile sibling assault, last-year statutory sex crimes, last-year and lifetime school bomb threats, and last-year robbery and total property victimization. According to Federal Bureau of Investigation statistics,42-44 the overall trends in crime as tracked by reports to the police were down 4%, 6%, and 5% for each successive year from 2008 to 2011. In general, the findings reported herein seem consistent with these statistics, although the size of our survey and the precision of estimates are not great enough to reliably detect small changes. Perhaps most noteworthy was that, in the context of continuing widespread economic distress and high unemployment, there were no signs of increased abuse, conflict, and aggression.
Readers should also keep in mind several limitations of the study. Various factors may have prevented us from capturing the full extent of exposure. The families that could not be contacted at home or who refused cooperation for themselves or their children may be the families whose children have discrepant levels of exposure compared with the cooperating families. Children may for many reasons fail to disclose all their exposures, and parents in particular may have incentive to cover up their children’s exposures. The screening questions for exposures needed to be brief and may not have included enough examples and details to trigger the memory of qualifying experiences. Some exposures, especially over a long time span, may be forgotten or may have occurred before the memory capacity of some victims was well formed. Some important types of victimization, like witnessing parental homicide, occur too infrequently in the population to be adequately counted in a survey method of this sort. Despite these limitations, the approach taken by the NatSCEV is more detailed and comprehensive than that used in previous violence exposure studies.
The high rate of exposure in children and youth and the complexity and interrelationships among the types of exposure are arguments for much more systematic, frequent, and intensive efforts to monitor the epidemiology of these problems. Despite the intense public attention focused on phenomena, such as clergy abuse (as illustrated by the experience in the Catholic church), coach abuse (as illustrated in the Penn State case, University Park, Pennsylvania), and bullying (as illustrated by the suicide of Massachusetts high school student, Phoebe Prince), it is remarkable that there have been no reliable, regular, validated data sources for tracking these problems. No annual surveys assess the frequency of bullying or dating violence, no annual estimates are acquired on the total number of child molestations, and no annual data are obtained on the number of teachers or coaches who are investigated for sex offenses against children. The NatSCEV points to the feasibility of obtaining more coordinated and frequent epidemiological data about these exposures.
This dearth of epidemiology stands in contrast to the situation in public health, in which accurate annual estimates are provided for more than 60 diseases, some quite obscure.45 Population surveys track other health characteristics such as insurance coverage and the prevalence of asthma and obesity.46 Crimes, such as auto theft and purse snatching, are tracked by the National Crime Victimization Survey.47 Large gaps exist in the coverage of children’s exposure to violence and abuse. Given its importance, priority should be given to filling these gaps.
Corresponding Author: David Finkelhor, PhD, Crimes Against Children Research Center, University of New Hampshire, 126 Horton Social Science Center, 20 Academic Way, Durham, NH 03824 (david.finkelhor@unh.edu).
Accepted for Publication: November 21, 2012.
Published Online: May 13, 2013. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.42.
Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Finkelhor, Turner, Hamby.
Acquisition of data: Finkelhor, Turner.
Analysis and interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Finkelhor, Shattuck.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Finkelhor, Turner, Hamby.
Statistical analysis: Shattuck.
Obtained funding: Finkelhor, Turner.
Administrative, technical, and material support: Finkelhor, Turner.
Study supervision: Finkelhor, Turner.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This project was supported by grants 2006-JW-BX-0003 and 2009-JW-BX-0018 from the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Justice Programs, US Department of Justice. The total amount of federal funding involved is $2 848 809. For the purposes of compliance with §507 of Pub L No. 104-208 (the Stevens Amendment), readers are advised that 100% of the funds for this program are derived from federal sources.
Disclaimer: Viewpoints or opinions in this document are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position or policies of the US Department of Justice.
Correction: This article was corrected for an error in Table 1 on January 28, 2014.
1.Shonkoff
JP, Boyce
WT, McEwen
BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention.
JAMA. 2009;301(21):2252-2259.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Fang
X, Brown
DS, Florence
CS, Mercy
JA. The economic burden of child maltreatment in the United States and implications for prevention.
Child Abuse Negl. 2012;36(2):156-165.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Danese
A, Moffitt
TE, Harrington
H,
et al. Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(12):1135-1143.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Sachs-Ericsson
N, Blazer
D, Plant
EA, Arnow
B. Childhood sexual and physical abuse and the 1-year prevalence of medical problems in the National Comorbidity Survey.
Health Psychol. 2005;24(1):32-40.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Widom
CS, DuMont
KA, Czaja
SJ. A prospective investigation of major depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(1):49-56.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Bensley
L, Van Eenwyk
J, Wynkoop Simmons
K. Childhood family violence history and women’s risk for intimate partner violence and poor health.
Am J Prev Med. 2003;25(1):38-44.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Finkelhor
D, Wells
M. Improving national data systems about juvenile victimization.
Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(1):77-102.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Finkelhor
D. Childhood Victimization: Violence, Crime, and Abuse in the Lives of Young People. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2008.
9.Nansel
TR, Overpeck
MD, Haynie
DL, Ruan
WJ, Scheidt
PC. Relationships between bullying and violence among US youth.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157(4):348-353.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Almeida
J, Cohen
AP, Subramanian
SV, Molnar
BE. Are increased worker caseloads in state child protective service agencies a potential explanation for the decline in child sexual abuse? a multilevel analysis.
Child Abuse Negl. 2008;32(3):367-375.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Mulford
C, Giordano
PC. Teen Dating Violence: A Closer Look at Adolescent Romantic Relationships. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice; 2008.
12.Pepler
DJ, Jiang
D, Craig
W, Connolly
J. Developmental trajectories of bullying and associated factors.
Child Dev. 2008;79(2):325-338.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Finkelhor
D, Turner
H, Ormrod
R, Hamby
SL. Violence, abuse, and crime exposure in a national sample of children and youth.
Pediatrics. 2009;124(5):1411-1423.
Google ScholarCrossref 14.McAuliffe
WE, Geller
S, LaBrie
R, Paletz
S, Fournier
E. Are telephone surveys suitable for studying substance abuse? cost, administration, coverage and response rate issues.
J Drug Issues. 1998;28(2):455-481.
Google Scholar 15.Weeks
MF, Kulka
RA, Lessler
JT, Whitmore
RW. Personal versus telephone surveys for collecting household health data at the local level.
Am J Public Health. 1983;73(12):1389-1394.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Acierno
R, Resnick
H, Kilpatrick
DG, Stark-Riemer
W. Assessing elder victimization: demonstration of a methodology.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2003;38(11):644-653.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Pruchno
RA, Hayden
JM. Interview modality: effects on costs and data quality in a sample of older women.
J Aging Health. 2000;12(1):3-24.
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Bajos
N, Spira
A, Ducot
B, Messiah
A; ASCF Principal Investigators and Their Associates. Analysis of sexual behavior in France (ACSF): a comparison between two modes of investigation: telephone survey and face-to-face survey.
AIDS. 1992;6(3):315-323.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Bermack
E. Effects of telephone & face-to-face communication on rated extent of self-disclosure by female college students.
Psychol Rep. 1989;65(1):259-267.
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Marin
G, Marin
B. A comparison of three interviewing approaches for studying sensitive topics with Hispanics.
Hisp J Behav Sci. 1989;11(4):330-340.
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Taylor
A. I’ll call you back on my mobile: a critique of the telephone interview with adolescent boys.
Westminster Stud Educ. 2002;25(1):19-34.
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Kohut
A, Keeter
S, Doherty
C, Dimock
M, Christian
L. Assessing the Representativeness of Public Opinion Surveys. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center; 2012.
24.Keeter
S, Kennedy
C, Dimock
M, Best
J, Craighill
P. Gauging the impact of growing nonresponse on estimates from a national RDD telephone survey.
Public Opin Q. 2006;70:759-779.
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Babbie
E. The Practice of Social Research.11th ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth; 2007.
26.Atrostic
BK, Bates
N, Burt
G, Silberstein
A. Nonresponse in U.S. government household surveys: consistent measures, recent trends, and new insights.
J Official Stat.2001;17(2):209-226.
Google Scholar 27.Curtin
R, Presser
S, Singer
E. Changes in telephone survey nonresponse over the past quarter century.
Public Opin Q. 2005;69:87-98.
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Curtin
R, Presser
S, Singer
E. The effects of response rate changes on the index of consumer sentiment.
Public Opin Q. 2000;64(4):413-428.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Keeter
S, Miller
C, Kohut
A, Groves
RM, Presser
S. Consequences of reducing nonresponse in a national telephone survey.
Public Opin Q. 2000;64(2):125-148.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Groves
RM. Nonresponse rates and nonresponse bias in household surveys.
Public Opin Q. 2006;70:646-675.
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Merkle
D, Edelman
M. Nonresponse in exit polls: a comprehensive analysis. In: Groves
RM, Dillman
DA, Eltinge
JL, Little
RJA, eds. Survey Nonresponse. New York, NY: Wiley; 2002:343-358.
33.Finkelhor
D, Hamby
SL, Ormrod
RK, Turner
HA. The JVQ: reliability, validity, and national norms.
Child Abuse Negl. 2005;29(4):383-412.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Finkelhor
D, Ormrod
RK, Turner
HA, Hamby
SL. Measuring poly-victimization using the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire.
Child Abuse Negl. 2005;29(11):1297-1312.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Hamby
SL, Finkelhor
D, Ormrod
RK, Turner
HA. The Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (JVQ): Administration and Scoring Manual. Durham, NH: Crimes Against Children Research Center; 2004.
36.Zhang
J, Yu
KF. What’s the relative risk? a method of correcting the odds ratio in cohort studies of common outcomes.
JAMA.1998;280(19):1690-1691.
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Baum K. Juvenile Victimization and Offending, 1993-2003. Washington, DC: Office of Justice Programs, US Dept of Justice; 2005. NCJ 209468.
38.Kilpatrick DG, Saunders BE, Smith DW. Youth Victimization: Prevalence & Implications. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, US Dept of Justice; 2003. NCJ 194972.
39.Straus
MA, Hamby
SL, Finkelhor
D, Moore
DW, Runyan
DK. Identification of child maltreatment with the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales: development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents [published correction appears in
Child Abuse Negl. 1998;22(11):1177].
Child Abuse Negl. 1998;22(4):249-270.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Finkelhor
D, Ormrod
RK, Turner
HA. Poly-victimization: a neglected component in child victimization.
Child Abuse Negl. 2007;31(1):7-26.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Turner
HA, Finkelhor
D, Ormrod
R. Poly-victimization in a national sample of children and youth.
Am J Prev Med. 2010;38(3):323-330.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Case Definitions: National Notifiable Conditions Infectious and Non-Infectious Case. Atlanta, GA: CDC; 2012.
47.Truman JL. Criminal Victimization, 2010. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics, US Dept of Justice; 2011. NCJ 235508.