Objectives
To test whether increased television viewing is associated with increased total energy intake and with increased consumption of foods commonly advertised on television, and to test whether increased consumption of these foods mediates the relationship between television viewing and total energy intake.
Design
Prospective observational study with baseline (fall 1995) and follow-up (spring 1997) measures of youth diet, physical activity, and television viewing. We used food advertising data to identify 6 food groups for study (sweet baked snacks, candy, fried potatoes, main courses commonly served as fast food, salty snacks, and sugar-sweetened beverages).
Setting and Participants
Five public schools in 4 communities near Boston. The sample included 548 students (mean age at baseline, 11.70 years; 48.4% female; and 63.5% white).
Main Outcome Measures
Change in total energy intake and intake of foods commonly advertised on television from baseline to follow-up.
Results
After adjusting for baseline covariates, each hour increase in television viewing was associated with an additional 167 kcal/d (95% confidence interval, 136-198 kcal/d; P<.001) and with increases in the consumption of foods commonly advertised on television. Including changes in intakes of these foods in regression models provided evidence of their mediating role, diminishing or rendering nonsignificant the associations between change in television viewing and change in total energy intake.
Conclusions
Increases in television viewing are associated with increased calorie intake among youth. This association is mediated by increasing consumption of calorie-dense low-nutrient foods frequently advertised on television.
Overweight prevalence has increased sharply among all sex and racial/ethnic groups in the United States. Among 12- to 19-year old individuals, overweight (body mass index [BMI] [calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters] ≥95th age- and sex-specific percentile) prevalence increased from 4.6% in the mid 1960s to 15.5% in 2000.1 In many cross-sectional2-4 and longitudinal5-10 studies, a positive association exists between hours of television viewing and measures of obesity and overweight in youth. Television viewing time also predicts obesity, development of type 2 diabetes mellitus,11,12 and poor lipid profiles13,14 in adults. In randomized controlled trials10,15-17 among youth, changes in television viewing have been instrumental in reducing overweight.
While published studies are consistent with a causal relationship between television viewing and overweight, the mechanisms driving this are not well understood. Television viewing could influence energy balance by reducing energy expenditure relative to other uses of leisure time, by increasing energy intake (eg, through snacking and exposure to food marketing), or by a combination of these. Although television viewing itself is a sedentary activity, contributing little to nonbasal energy expenditure, studies18-20 show only a weak or modest negative correlation between time spent on television viewing and time spent in moderate and vigorous physical activity. A randomized controlled trial21 found that increasing screen time resulted in reduced energy expenditure and increased energy intake.
Television's potential role in increasing energy intake must be considered in the context of the extraordinary amount of television-based food advertising that occurs annually. Television advertisements account for roughly 75% of food manufacturers' and 95% of fast food restaurants' advertising expenditures.22 In 1997, when data collection for this study ended, food industries spent $11 billion on advertising.22 Food manufacturers, responsible for most ($7 billion) of this, spent 25% of their advertising dollars promoting candy, cookies, salty snacks, and soft drinks—and 5% promoting fruits, vegetables, grains, beans, meat, poultry, and fish.22 Fast food restaurants spent more than $1 billion on advertising in 1999.23
Studies22,24-28 suggest these dollars are spent effectively—food advertising works. For example, several studies show associations between exposure to advertisements and children's requests for specific foods, food purchasing, and food consumption. Television viewing, a major venue for advertising exposure, is directly associated with calorie intake among adults11 and youth29,30 in cross-sectional studies, and with intake of foods commonly advertised on television (FCAT)—soft drinks, fried foods, and snacks—among youth.3 These types of foods pose risks for overeating and weight gain. Fast food restaurant use, which is heavily promoted on television, is positively associated with calorie intake in youth31 and with BMI among adults.32 In a recent study,30 on days when children ate fast food, they consumed more calories, fat, added sugars, and sugar-sweetened beverages and fewer servings of milk, fruits, and nonstarchy vegetables than on days when they did not eat fast food. Prospective studies33 among children have indicated a positive relationship between intake of sugar-sweetened soft drinks and obesity incidence.
The present study examines whether increases in adolescents' television viewing time are associated with increases in total energy intake (TEI), and whether viewing increases are also associated with increases in intake of FCAT (candy, fast foods, fried potatoes, sweet baked snacks, salty snacks, and sugar-sweetened beverages). We also examine whether changes in intakes of these foods mediate the relationship between change in viewing time and change in TEI.
In light of the literature, we hypothesize that increases in television viewing time will result in increases in TEI. One causal pathway will consist of increased consumption of FCAT. To test this hypothesis, we examine a longitudinal observational cohort in which we collected measures of television viewing and food consumption at 2 time points. Statistical models in which change in an independent variable predicts change in a dependent variable may provide stronger evidence for causality than predictions involving the independent variable measured at just 1 point in time (eg, baseline).34 Attenuation of the association between an independent and a dependent variable by the addition of a second independent variable in the causal pathway can provide evidence for mediation in the context of a plausible theoretical framework.35 In addition, a mediator variable is also independently associated with exposure and outcome. Therefore, in our analyses of the relationship of change in television viewing time to change in TEI, we evaluate evidence for the mediating effects of FCAT by examining the associations between television and FCAT; FCAT and TEI; and television, FCAT, and TEI. In our models, we control for other characteristics of respondents that could potentially influence energy intake at follow-up, including baseline BMI, age, sex, indicator variables for race/ethnicity, baseline physical activity of 3.5 metabolic equivalents (METs) or more, change in physical activity of 3.5 METs or more, and school.
Data were collected as part of the Planet Health intervention and evaluation, a group randomized controlled trial that took place in 10 schools in 4 communities in the Boston metropolitan area between fall 1995 and spring 1997. Schools were matched and then randomized to the control and intervention conditions, and no differences were observed between these groups on baseline measures, including age, BMI, TEI, television viewing, or physical activity levels. The 548 students composing the analytic sample attended the 5 randomly assigned control schools that did not take part in the intervention designed to reduce obesity prevalence.16 The median annual household income of ZIP code areas where the control schools were located averaged $34 200, according to 1990 census data, lower than that for all households in Massachusetts in 1990 ($41 000), but similar to the US figure ($33 952).36 A total of 780 subjects (64.5% of those eligible) completed the baseline evaluation in fall 1995 at the control schools after excluding individuals who transferred schools at baseline, were in special education classes, were in grades other than sixth or seventh, or did not complete the English-language version of the questionnaire. Follow-up data were collected in spring 1997 for 655 subjects (84.0% of the baseline group), reflecting loss to follow-up of 18.0% for girls and 14.0% for boys. The main reason for lack of follow-up data was school transfer (half of those not followed up) and school absence (one quarter). Of the 571 subjects with complete data on all variables, 548 were included in these analyses after excluding 23 with implausible daily energy intakes (≤500 or ≥7000 kcal). Further details on sampling and exclusions are available elsewhere.16 The study was approved by the Committee on Human Subjects at the Harvard School of Public Health.
The 548 subjects were a mean ± SD age of 11.70 ± 0.75 years at baseline, and 48.4% were female. Of the subjects, 63.5% were white, 15.3% were Hispanic, 13.9% were African American, 7.7% were Asian, 0.9% were American Indian, and 7.7% were another ethnicity (this totals >100% because respondents could select multiple descriptors). Anthropometric data and student surveys were collected in fall 1995, when subjects were in either grade 6 or grade 7, and follow-up measurements were collected approximately 19 months later, in spring 1997 (when subjects were in grade 7 or 8).
For BMI, height without shoes was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a stadiometer (Irwin Shorr, Olney, Md) and weight in light clothes was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg on a portable electronic scale (model 770; Seca, Hanover, Md), calibrated using a standard weights “step-up” test (Seca).
Assessment of diet, physical activity level, and television viewing
Measures of dietary intake, physical activity level, and television viewing were obtained via an optically scannable student food and activity survey, used with permission of the Brigham and Women's Hospital, Boston. Students completed the food and activity survey independently in class under the supervision of teachers who participated in a 1-hour training session before administration.
The FCAT were identified from the data of Gallo.22 Comparable foods were identified from the youth food frequency questionnaire component of the food and activity survey, an instrument adapted and validated for use in ethnically and socioeconomically diverse populations.37-39 Six FCAT groups were created to best match the available youth food frequency questionnaire data with information on food advertising22,23: sugar-sweetened beverages, salty snacks, fried potatoes, sweet baked snacks, candy, and main courses commonly served at fast food restaurants.
The youth food frequency questionnaire asked how often in the past 30 days food items were consumed. There were 4 to 6 response categories typically offered. For example, response categories for soda (1 of 3 items composing sugar-sweetened beverages) were never or less than 1 can per month, 1 to 3 cans per month, 1 can per week, 2 to 6 cans per week, 1 can per day, or 2 cans or more per day. For each item, the uppermost category was truncated at the highest value (eg, ≥2 glasses per day was assigned a value of 2) and the lowest category was coded as 0.5 per month. If a food item was left blank, it was recoded as a 0. Table 1 describes the food groups and the response categories for each item.
Total energy intake was estimated from all foods on the youth food frequency questionnaire by the Channing Laboratory, Boston, using calorie values assigned to standard portion sizes from releases 10 and 11 of the US Department of Agriculture Nutrient Database for Standard Reference. In a separate study,37 the Pearson product moment correlation coefficient for TEI estimated by comparing 2 food frequency questionnaires with three 24-hour recalls was 0.49.
Physical activity level was assessed with the youth activity questionnaire component of the food and activity survey. The youth activity questionnaire included 16 items that estimate hours per day spent in moderate and vigorous activities (≥3.5 METs)40 over the past month. Walking was excluded because of low agreement among measures in other studies.36 The youth activity questionnaire is based on a 14-item physical activity questionnaire demonstrated to have good reproducibility and validity in adults41,42 and high school youth.36 In a validation study among 53 participants in Planet Health, using repeat 24-hour physical activity recalls 1 month apart, the 16 youth activity questionnaire items for activity of 3.5 METs or more had a deattenuated correlation43 with the 24-hour recall of r = 0.80, with equivalent means.16
Time spent watching television was measured with the 11-item television and video measure.16 Seven of these questions asked about hours of television typically viewed during each day of the week. The midpoint of each response category was used to estimate total viewing, and the uppermost category (eg, “5 or more hours per day”) was assigned a value of 5. Items were appropriately weighted and summed to obtain a television viewing hour-per-day estimate. Other items in the television and video measure assessed video and computer game use and were not included in this analysis. In the validation sample (n = 53), we found a deattenuated43 correlation of television viewing via the television and video measure and the 24-hour recall of r = 0.54, with equivalent means.16 Television data were imputed among 7 students missing 1 or more days of television data in 1995 and among 3 students in 1997. When weekdays were missing, the imputed value was equal to the mean of the other weekdays for that student. When weekend days were missing, the imputed value was equal to the value for the available weekend day or, for 4 students missing both weekend days in 1995, the mean value for the sample was used. Sample means for television viewing were unchanged by imputation.
Age was calculated from birth date and date of anthropometric examinations. Sex was classified at the time of examination by measurers or from school list data. Racial/ethnic categories were derived from the question, “How do you describe yourself?,” to which students could select multiple responses from the following list: white, black, Hispanic, Asian or Pacific Islander, American Indian or Alaskan Native, or other. Participants indicating black were classified as African American. Indicator variables were used to classify the 5 schools, with the largest school omitted as the referent. The distribution of subjects from each of the 5 schools was as follows: 261 (47.6%), 122 (22.3%), 79 (14.4%), 48 (8.8%), and 38 (6.9%).
SAS statistical software, version 8.2 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC), was used for all analyses. For adjusted analyses, we used PROC SURVEYREG to estimate regressions because these take into account clustering at the school level. PROC SURVEYREG uses an implicit Taylor linearization estimation method. Change variables were calculated as the 1997 value minus the 1995 value to obtain continuous measures.
We estimated adjusted associations of change in television viewing with change in TEI in a series of regression models. Predictor variables in each model included baseline television viewing and change in viewing, baseline BMI, TEI, age, sex, indicator variables for race/ethnicity, baseline physical activity of 3.5 METs or more, change in physical activity of 3.5 METs or more, and indicator variables for schools.
In the first set of models, the outcome was change in TEI. We evaluated the mediating role of intake of FCAT by adding baseline and change variables for the 6 food groups individually and then simultaneously to create 7 regressions in addition to the basic model for TEI. Foods were considered potential mediating, and not confounding, variables because in our theoretical model, we posit that increases in FCAT intake follow increases in television viewing in a temporal manner and, therefore, are part of the causal pathway. Empirically, this suggests that changes in television viewing predict changes in FCAT intake, and that FCAT intake predicts TEI. Two analyses were undertaken to examine evidence for the hypothesized mediation. First, we assessed evidence for independent associations between changes in intakes of the food groups and change in TEI (all models were significant at P<.05; data not shown). Second, we estimated an additional set of regression models (shown later) to assess whether changes in television viewing predicted change in number of servings per day of each food group.
Dietary and activity data are summarized in Table 2. The mean ± SD BMI at baseline was 20.73 ± 3.99. The TEI increased from baseline to follow-up. Although the average hours of television viewing changed little, 152 subjects (27.7%) increased television viewing by 1 h/d or less and 83 (15.1%) increased viewing by more than 1 h/d (data not shown). All mean FCAT intakes increased during the study period. The highest daily number of servings at follow-up were reported for baked sweet snacks, sugar-sweetened beverages, and fast food–type main courses, and the combined category indicated roughly 5 servings of FCAT per day, or 35 per week.
In adjusted analyses, baseline viewing and change in television viewing predicted change in daily TEI (Table 3). With no foods in the model, each hour of increase in television viewing was associated with a 167-kcal increment. Baseline television viewing time was also independently associated with change in TEI. The FCAT mediated these associations; coefficients for television viewing time (baseline and change) were greatly reduced, and some were rendered insignificant when foods were added into the models. For example, adjusting for sugar-sweetened beverages reduced the kilocalories associated with a 1-hour increase in television viewing from 167 to 123, a 26% reduction; and adjusting for fast food–type main courses or all FCAT combined rendered the association between television increasing and TEI nonsignificant. Baseline television viewing predicted change in calorie intake when no foods were in the model, but was no longer a predictor in models controlling for change in baked sweet snacks, candy, fast food–type main courses, sugar-sweetened beverages, or all foods combined.
In support of the hypothesis of mediation, Table 4 shows that increased television viewing time was also associated with increased intake of FCAT. These relationships are necessary to demonstrate that intakes of these foods can mediate television's effect on TEI. The table shows the coefficients for change in servings per day of these foods associated with baseline and change in television viewing. With the exception of fried potatoes, higher intakes of all foods at follow-up were significantly positively associated with baseline television viewing level. Moreover, change in television viewing was significantly associated with change in consumption of each food group. With each 1-hour increment in television viewing, increases in servings per day ranged from 0.03 (fried potatoes) to 0.21 (baked sweet snacks) across food groups, or 0.2 to 1.4 servings per week.
Identifying the mechanisms by which television increases risk of obesity through its influence on energy balance is important in providing evidence for causality for this well-documented observation. We show that, among youth, increases in television viewing predict increases in TEI, and that increasing intakes of FCAT mediate this relationship.
These results build on other recent research. Epstein et al21 found that in experimental manipulations, increasing screen time by 50% was associated with a 250-kcal/d increase in energy intake among youth. Increasing consumption of non–nutrient-dense foods high in added sugars and fats, a pattern typical of fast food meals that include a burger, French fries, and soda, may compromise diet quality by increasing energy intake,29-32 displacing more healthful food options,30 and promoting weight gain.33 The growth in fast food consumption in the US population is, therefore, of great concern. While fast food meals accounted for 3% of eating occasions and 3% of daily calorie intake in 1977, in 1995 they accounted for 9% of eating occasions and 12% of calories.44 Concurrent evidence45,46 suggests that total average daily energy intakes among youth have increased, reflecting in part increased intake of added sugars.47 Among adolescents, about 40% of added sugars derive from soda,48 consumption of which increased by 131% between the late 1970s and the mid 1990s.49 Our analyses support a link between television viewing and these unhealthy dietary changes, suggesting that television advertising for food has a powerful influence on diets. Indeed, studies show that television viewing is inversely associated with intake of fruits and vegetables,31,50 which receive little air time22 despite their potential to promote health in various ways and protect against weight gain.51
Several limitations of the analysis relate to measurement error and its attendant effect on regression analyses. The validity of the television measure is limited by recall bias and instrument design, although prior studies indicate modest validity. Dietary intake is typically measured with a large component of random measurement error. In linear regression models with random error in the dependent variable (such as total energy or food intake), regression coefficients will be unbiased, although the R2 value for the model will be reduced; conversely, random measurement errors in independent variables will attenuate regression coefficients and reduce their ability to function effectively as mediating or control variables.52 In using dietary intake, television viewing time, and physical activity measures as independent variables, random error due to questionnaire design, recall error, and within-person variability not captured by the instrument may, therefore, attenuate regression coefficients. When food servings are converted to energy intake, additional errors are introduced because of assumptions about serving size and food composition. Another potential source of error is in the use of television viewing time as a proxy for exposure to advertising. Although we assumed that, on average, youth who watched more television were exposed to more advertisements, we did not assess what programs or channels subjects were exposed to, both of which could affect the total “dose” of advertising. We also had limited ability to control for other potential confounding variables, such as baseline levels of moderate and vigorous physical activity and body composition, which may also have contributed to bias in estimates of association.
Causality is suggested but not proved by our findings, which are observational and not experimental. Generalizability may be limited because of the nature of our sample. Further research could address these limitations through a randomized trial to reduce television viewing in a broader population of youth using survey instruments that collected data on sources of food.
In conclusion, although children and youth are encouraged to watch what they eat, many youth seem to eat what they watch, and in the process increase their risk for increasing their energy intake. In the absence of regulations restricting food advertising aimed at children, reduction in television viewing is a promising approach to reducing excess energy intake. Although a threshold of safe exposure to television and food marketing has not been empirically demonstrated, the American Academy of Pediatrics has long advocated limiting children to no more than 2 hours of television per day to decrease sedentary time and to decrease exposure to content that may encourage a range of negative behaviors.53 This study adds more evidence in support of this recommendation.
Correspondence: Jean L. Wiecha, PhD, Harvard School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Ave, Seventh Floor, Boston, MA 02115 (jwiecha@hsph.harvard.edu).
Accepted for Publication: July 25, 2005.
Author Contributions: Dr Wiecha had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant HD-30780 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Rockville, Md; Prevention Research Centers grants U48/CCU115807 and U48/DP00064-00S1 from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Ga; grant R01DK59240 from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, Bethesda, Md; and the Charles H. Hood Foundation, Boston.
Role of the Sponsor: The funding bodies had no role in data extraction and analyses, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Disclaimer: This work is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent official views of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention or other granting institutions.
Acknowledgment: We thank Alice Colby for her assistance with manuscript preparation.
1.Ogden
CLFlegal
KMCarroll
MDJohnson
CL Prevalence and trends in overweight among US children and adolescents, 1999-2000
JAMA 2002;2881728- 1732
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Obarzanek
ESchreiber
GBCrawford
PB
et al. Energy intake and physical activity in relation to indexes of body fat: the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Growth and Health Study
Am J Clin Nutr 1994;6015- 22
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Utter
JNeumark-Sztainer
DJeffery
RStory
M Couch potatoes or French fries: are sedentary behaviors associated with body mass index, physical activity, and dietary behaviors among adolescents?
J Am Diet Assoc 2003;1031298- 1305
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Andersen
RECrespo
CJBartlett
SJCheskin
LJPratt
M Relationship of physical activity and television watching with body weight and level of fatness among children: results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
JAMA 1998;279938- 942
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Dietz
WH
JrGortmaker
SL Do we fatten our children at the television set? obesity and television viewing in children and adolescents
Pediatrics 1985;75807- 812
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Proctor
MHMoore
LLGao
D
et al. Television viewing and change in body fat from preschool to early adolescence: the Framingham Children's Study
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2003;27827- 833
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Gortmaker
SLMust
ASobol
AMPeterson
KColditz
GADietz
WH Television viewing as a cause of increasing obesity among children in the United States, 1986-1990
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1996;150356- 362
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Hu
FBLeitzmann
MFStampfer
MJColditz
GAWillett
WCRimm
EB Physical activity and television watching in relation to risk for type 2 diabetes mellitus in men
Arch Intern Med 2001;1611542- 1548
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Hu
FBLi
TYColditz
GAWillett
WCManson
JE Television watching and other sedentary behaviors in relation to risk of obesity and type 2 diabetes mellitus in women
JAMA 2003;2891785- 1791
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Fung
TTHu
FBYu
J
et al. Leisure-time physical activity, television watching, and plasma biomarkers of obesity and cardiovascular disease risk
Am J Epidemiol 2000;1521171- 1178
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Fung
TTRimm
EBSpiegelman
D
et al. Association between dietary patterns and plasma biomarkers of obesity and cardiovascular disease risk
Am J Clin Nutr 2001;7361- 67
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Epstein
LHValoski
AMVara
LS
et al. Effects of decreasing sedentary behavior and increasing activity on weight change in obese children
Health Psychol 1995;14109- 115
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Gortmaker
SLPeterson
KWiecha
J
et al. Reducing obesity via a school-based interdisciplinary intervention among youth: Planet Health
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1999;153409- 418
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Robinson
TN Reducing children's television viewing to prevent obesity: a randomized controlled trial
JAMA 1999;2821561- 1567
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Pate
RRHeath
GWDowda
MTrost
SG Associations between physical activity and other health behaviors in a representative sample of US adolescents
Am J Public Health 1996;861577- 1581
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Lowry
RWechsler
HGaluska
DAFulton
JEKann
L Television viewing and its associations with overweight, sedentary lifestyle, and insufficient consumption of fruits and vegetables among US high school students: differences by race, ethnicity, and gender
J Sch Health 2002;72413- 421
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Marshall
SJBiddle
SJGorely
TCameron
NMurdey
I Relationships between media use, body fatness and physical activity in children and youth: a meta-analysis
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2004;281238- 1246
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Epstein
LHPaluch
RAConsalvi
ARiordan
KScholl
T Effects of manipulating sedentary behavior on physical activity and food intake
J Pediatr 2002;140334- 339
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Gallo
AFrazao
Eed Food advertising in the United States
America's Eating Habits: Changes and Consequences. Washington, DC Economic Research Service, US Dept of Agriculture1999;173- 180.Agriculture Information Bulletin 750
Google Scholar 23.Nestle
M Food Politics: How the Food Industry Influences Nutrition and Health. Berkeley University of California Press2002;
24.Taras
HLSallis
JFPatterson
TLNader
PRNelson
JA Television's influence on children's diet and physical activity
J Dev Behav Pediatr 1989;10176- 180
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Borzekowski
DLRobinson
TN The 30-second effect: an experiment revealing the impact of television commercials on food preferences of preschoolers
J Am Diet Assoc 2001;10142- 46
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Galst
JWhite
M The unhealthy persuader: the reinforcing value of television and children's purchase influence attempts at the supermarket
Child Dev 1976;471089- 1096
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Goldberg
MGorn
GGibson
W TV messages for snacks and breakfast foods: do they influence children's preferences?
J Consum Res 1978;573- 81
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Gorn
GJGoldberg
ME Behavioral evidence of the effects of televised food messages on children
J Consum Res 1982;9200- 205
Google ScholarCrossref 29.McNutt
SWHu
YSchreiber
GBCrawford
PBObarzanek
EMellin
L A longitudinal study of the dietary practices of black and white girls 9 and 10 years old at enrollment: the NHLBI Growth and Health Study
J Adolesc Health 1997;2027- 37
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Bowman
SAGortmaker
SLEbbeling
CBPereira
MALudwig
DS Effects of fast-food consumption on energy intake and diet quality among children in a national household survey
Pediatrics 2004;113(pt 1)112- 118
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.French
SAStory
MNeumark-Sztainer
DFulkerson
JAHannan
P Fast food restaurant use among adolescents: associations with nutrient intake, food choices and behavioral and psychosocial variables
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2001;251823- 1833
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Binkley
JKEales
JJekanowski
M The relation between dietary change and rising US obesity
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000;241032- 1039
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Ludwig
DSPeterson
KEGortmaker
SL Relation between consumption of sugar-sweetened drinks and childhood obesity: a prospective, observational analysis
Lancet 2001;357505- 508
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Shadish
WRCook
TDCampbell
DT Experimental and Quasi-experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Boston, Mass Houghton Mifflin Co2002;
35.Baron
RMKenny
DA The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations
J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;511173- 1182
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Peterson
KEField
AEFox
MK Validation of YRBSS Questions on Dietary Behaviors and Physical Activity Among Adolescents in Grades 9-12. Atlanta, Ga Division of Adolescent and School Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention1996;
37.Rockett
HRBreitenbach
MFrazier
AL
et al. Validation of a youth/adolescent food frequency questionnaire
Prev Med 1997;26808- 816
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Rockett
HRWolf
AMColditz
GA Development and reproducibility of a food frequency questionnaire to assess diets of older children and adolescents
J Am Diet Assoc 1995;95336- 340
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Field
AEPeterson
KEGortmaker
SL
et al. Reproducibility and validity of a food frequency questionnaire among fourth to seventh grade inner-city school children: implications of age and day-to-day variation in dietary intake
Public Health Nutr 1999;2293- 300
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Ainsworth
BEHaskell
WLLeon
AS
et al. Compendium of physical activities: classification of energy costs of human physical activities
Med Sci Sports Exerc 1993;2571- 80
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Wolf
AMHunter
DJColditz
GA
et al. Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire
Int J Epidemiol 1994;23991- 999
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Chasan-Taber
SRimm
EBStampfer
MJ
et al. Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire for male health professionals
Epidemiology 1996;781- 86
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Rosner
BWillett
WC Interval estimates for correlation coefficients corrected for within-person variation: implications for study design and hypothesis testing
Am J Epidemiol 1988;127377- 386
PubMedGoogle Scholar 44.Lin
BGuthrie
JFrazao
EFrazao
Eed Nutrient contribution of food away from home
America's Eating Habits: Changes and Consequences. Washington, DC Economic Research Service, US Dept of Agriculture1999;213- 239.Agriculture Information Bulletin 750
Google Scholar 45.Dixon
LBErnst
ND Choose a diet that is low in saturated fat and cholesterol and moderate in total fat: subtle changes to a familiar message
J Nutr 2001;131(suppl)510S- 526S
Google Scholar 47.Krebs-Smith
SM Choose beverages and foods to moderate your intake of sugars: measurement requires quantification
J Nutr 2001;131(suppl)527S- 535S
Google Scholar 48.Guthrie
JFMorton
JF Food sources of added sweeteners in the diets of Americans
J Am Diet Assoc 2000;10043- 51
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Tippett
KCleveland
LFrazao
Eed How current diets stack up: comparison with dietary guidelines
America's Eating Habits: Changes and Consequences Washington, DC Economic Research Service, US Dept of Agriculture1999;51- 70Agriculture Information Bulletin 750
Google Scholar 50.Boynton-Jarrett
RThomas
TNPeterson
KEWiecha
JSobol
AMGortmaker
SL Impact of television viewing patterns on fruit and vegetable consumption among adolescents
Pediatrics 2003;1121321- 1326
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Ludwig
DSPereira
MAKroenke
CH
et al. Dietary fiber, weight gain, and cardiovascular disease risk factors in young adults
JAMA 1999;2821539- 1546
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Fox
J Applied Regression Analysis, Linear Models and Related Methods. Thousand Oaks, Calif Sage Publications1997;
53.Committee on Communications, American Academy of Pediatrics, Children, adolescents, and television
Pediatrics 1995;96786- 787
PubMedGoogle Scholar