Objective
To test the effects of media violence exposure on blood pressure, negative affect, hostile social information processing, uncooperative behavior, and attitudes toward health risk behaviors among young men varying in lifetime violence exposure within the home and community.
Design
Experimental laboratory study.
Setting
University campus situated within an urban environment.
Participants
One hundred male undergraduates aged 18 to 21 years.
Intervention
Men who had previously reported differing amounts of lifetime home and community violence were randomly assigned to play The Simpsons: Hit and Run (low-violence condition) or Grand Theft Auto III (high-violence condition).
Main Outcome Measures
Systolic and diastolic blood pressure; negative affect; hostile social information processing; uncooperative behavior; and permissive attitudes toward violence, alcohol use, marijuana use, and sexual activity without condom use.
Results
Men randomly assigned to play Grand Theft Auto III exhibited greater increases in diastolic blood pressure from a baseline rest period to game play, greater negative affect, more permissive attitudes toward using alcohol and marijuana, and more uncooperative behavior in comparison with men randomly assigned to play The Simpsons. Only among participants with greater exposure to home and community violence, play of Grand Theft Auto III led to elevated systolic blood pressure in comparison with play of The Simpsons (mean, 13 vs 5 mm Hg).
Conclusions
Media violence exposure may play a role in the development of negative attitudes and behaviors related to health. Although youth growing up in violent homes and communities may become more physiologically aroused by media violence exposure, all youth appear to be at risk for potentially negative outcomes.
Experimental studies consistently show that media violence increases blood pressure, negative emotions, and aggressive behavior in the immediate aftermath of exposure, including physical assault (hitting, kicking, choking, wrestling), in samples of children and younger adolescents and willingness to inflict electric shock or loud aversive noises on a peer in samples of older adolescents and young adults.1 Results from the few existing longitudinal survey studies suggest that greater exposure to television and media violence in childhood increases the occurrence of meaningful aggression-related outcomes and criminality well into young adulthood,2-5 even after statistically controlling for potential confounds, such as early aggressive behavior, family socioeconomic status, parenting characteristics, and community safety.4,5
Anderson and Bushman6 have integrated previous influential theories of aggression7-9 into a comprehensive framework for understanding human aggression. Their General Aggression Model posits that characteristics of the situation (eg, media violence exposure) influence the internal state of an individual, including cognitions (eg, proviolent attitudes), affect (eg, anger), and arousal (eg, increased blood pressure). Internal states in turn influence appraisals (eg, interpreting others' actions as hostile) and decision processes (eg, choosing to aggress). Repeated violence exposure may lead to the development of a hostile, aggressive personality.
One less understood issue is whether media violence exposure influences risky health behaviors and social interactions more broadly than aggression-related outcomes. In addition to modeling aggression, violence models general risk taking and disregard for the well-being of others. The General Aggression Model suggests that media violence exposure may increase the likelihood that individuals engage in impulsive rather than thoughtful courses of action.6 Although a consistent body of research links real-world violence exposure to adolescent substance use and sexual risk taking,10-16 to our knowledge, only 1 study has examined the association between media violence exposure and such behaviors, and this study yielded mixed results. Male youth aged 11 to 25 years who watched television programs involving contact sports and realistic crime (eg, COPS) reported higher levels of risk behaviors (eg, alcohol and other drug use), while those who watched violent drama (eg, NYPD Blue) reported lower levels.17 Real-world violence exposure18,19 and media violence exposure1,20,21 are linked with hostile appraisals and aggressive behavior, but research has not addressed whether violence exposure influences social behaviors in ways that do not directly involve aggression, such as the decision whether to cooperate or compete with a peer.
A second less understood issue is whether some youth are at particular risk for experiencing adverse outcomes when exposed to media violence. Huesmann and colleagues22 have conceptualized societal “cultures of violence” as a potentially important moderator of media violence effects. Acute exposure to media violence may activate aggressive patterns of thought, affect, and behavior more strongly among individuals who are accustomed to aggressive styles of interaction and who have potentially developed more aggressive attitudes, appraisals of others' actions, and behavioral scripts as a result. In a comprehensive review of children's adjustment to real-world violence, Margolin and Gordis23 highlighted the need to examine the cumulative effects of different types of violence exposure (eg, family, community) as well as unique effects. Previous studies have not tested whether exposure to violence within the home and/or community interacts with media violence exposure to influence outcomes.
The present study examines the effects of acute media violence exposure and lifetime home and community violence exposure on cognitive-behavioral, affective, and physiological health outcomes within the context of a quasi-experimental study design. Young men exposed to differing amounts of lifetime violence were randomly assigned to play a video game high or low in violent content. Consistent with the General Aggression Model,6 we hypothesized that both home and community and media violence exposure would be associated with greater increases in blood pressure during game play, greater negative affect and more permissive attitudes toward violence immediately after game play, and more hostile social information processing during a laboratory task subsequent to game play. Because violence models general risk taking and disregard for the well-being of others, we also hypothesized that home and community and media violence exposure would be associated with more permissive attitudes toward health risk behavior and with greater uncooperative behavior. Finally, we hypothesized that effects of the media violence condition would be greater among those young men high in lifetime home and community violence exposure.
We recruited 100 participants from a pool of 180 undergraduate men aged 18 to 21 years who completed a survey of violence exposure and engagement in harmful health behaviors. We decided to conduct the present laboratory experiment on men because young men are known to have greater exposure to violent media in comparison with young women.24
Participants identified their ethnicity as white/Caucasian (n = 83), black/African American (n = 10), Asian/Asian American (n = 4), and Hispanic/Latino (n = 2). One participant declined to identify his ethnicity. In some studies, individuals of black ethnicity have exhibited greater stress-related increases in blood pressure in comparison with individuals of white ethnicity.25-27 All but 1 of the black participants were in the highest tertile for home and community violence exposure, raising the possibility that effects of lifetime violence exposure would be confounded with ethnicity. We thus included a dichotomous ethnicity variable (white/Caucasian or minority) as a covariate in supplemental analyses.
The university institutional review board approved all parts of the study protocol and participants provided their informed consent. We matched pairs of men on ethnicity and lifetime home and community violence exposure tertile and randomly assigned them to the high- or low-violence video game. All participants began their laboratory visit by watching a relaxing video about Hawaii for 10 minutes. They then viewed 4 minutes of videotaped game play demonstrating the “mission” they would be asked to complete, followed by 10 minutes of video game play. We made blood pressure readings at minutes 5, 3, and 1 of the 10-minute resting baseline period and minutes 10, 7.5, 5, and 2.5 during game play. After game play, participants completed an affect checklist and a measure of attitudes toward harmful health behaviors and violence. In 2 subsequent tasks administered in counterbalanced order, participants watched a videotaped social scenario and responded to a structured clinical interview and then engaged in a game during which they could cooperate or compete with a confederate. At the end of the study, participants completed a paper-and-pencil measure asking them to rate features of the video game. We reimbursed participants for their time.
Previous Violence Exposure
Participants completed the Community Experiences Questionnaire18 during the survey, prior to their laboratory visit. The Community Experiences Questionnaire contains items assessing direct victimization (11 items [eg, hit/punched/slapped; coerced through violence/threats]) and witnessed victimization (14 items [eg, robbery; gunshots]). Participants indicated whether each item had occurred never (0), once (1), a few times (2), or lots of times (3) and indicated whether they had experienced each item within the home and within their community (including school and work). Violence exposure scores were summed to create total overall violence (α = .90), total community violence (α = .89), and total home violence (α = .82) scales. Because of slight positive skew, home violence exposure was transformed by taking the square root of the original variable.
During the survey, participants estimated the typical number of hours per week they spent playing video games during grades 7 through 9 (mean [SD], 11.0 [11.9] hours per week), grades 10 through 12 (mean [SD], 10.1 [10.1] hours per week), and college (mean [SD], 6.6 [7.3] hours per week). We created a composite score by weighting each estimation according to recency of period (1, 2, or 3) and summing across scores. Because of positive skew, this composite score was transformed by taking the square root of the original variable. The resulting variable was examined as a covariate in supplemental analyses.
Video Games and Manipulation Check
We randomly assigned participants to play a video game high in violence, Grand Theft Auto III, or a video game comparatively low in violence, The Simpsons: Hit and Run. Both games involve driving through a virtual city to complete various missions. Participants randomly assigned to play Grand Theft Auto III played the role of a criminal and were instructed by a member of the mafia to “introduce a bat” to the face of a drug dealer who was supplying drugs to prostitutes employed by the mafia. Participants randomly assigned to play The Simpsons: Hit and Run played the role of Homer Simpson and were instructed by Homer's wife, Marge, to deliver daughter Lisa's science project to school before Principal Skinner arrived and Lisa's science project would be marked late.
All participants began their mission in the same game location as that shown in the videotape prior to game play. Participants were instructed to complete the mission they had just viewed and to play the game “however you would like” in any remaining time after completing the mission. The percentage of participants able to complete the Grand Theft Auto III and Simpsons missions in the allotted 10-minute period was 78% and 96%, respectively. Only 1 person appeared not to attempt any part of his mission. The mean (SD) time spent engaging in mission-related game play was 5 minutes, 24 seconds (2 minutes, 34 seconds) for Grand Theft Auto III and 4 minutes, 42 seconds (1 minute, 21 seconds) for The Simpsons. Thus, virtually all participants completed the mission or spent a substantial amount of time attempting to complete the mission as instructed.
As a manipulation check, participants completed a paper-and-pencil Video Game Rating Sheet allowing them to rate features of the game on a 7-point Likert scale (1, “not at all” to 7, “very”).28,29 Participants perceived Grand Theft Auto III to be more violent in content (mean, 6.0 vs 3.1; t = 10.8; P<.001) and graphics (mean, 5.6 vs 2.4; t = 12.8; P<.001) in comparison with The Simpsons: Hit and Run. Ratings of difficulty, enjoyability, and frustration did not significantly differ between games. Participants perceived Grand Theft Auto III to be slightly more exciting and faster in action than The Simpsons: Hit and Run (mean differences were <1 scale point). Within-game correlations between ratings of excitement and fast action were both 0.50. We summed ratings of excitement and fast action to create a composite score and included the score as a covariate in supplemental analyses.
We monitored systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure using an IBS Model SD-700A automated blood pressure monitor (IBS Corp, Waltham, Mass) with a standard occluding cuff placed on the participant's left arm. We averaged all measures taken during a particular period. Participants' resting SBP ranged from 96 to 136 mm Hg (mean [SD], 113 [9] mm Hg), while resting diastolic blood pressure ranged between 41 to 84 mm Hg (mean [SD] 61 [9] mm Hg). Increases in physiological parameters from the baseline rest period to playing the video game were all significantly different from zero (Table 1). Change scores from the baseline rest period to playing the video game were regressed on the baseline rest measure to control for potential “ceiling effects” among individuals with greater resting blood pressures. These residualized change scores were analyzed in subsequent analyses.
Laboratory-Assessed Negative Affect
Immediately after video game play, we measured negative affect with the Reduced Profile of Mood States.30 The 9-item negative affect score (α = .79) is the sum of 3 affective subscores (anxiety, depression, and anger). Because of slight positive skew, scores were transformed by computing the square root of the original variable.
Permissive Attitudes Toward Violence and Other Harmful Health Behaviors
After video game play, participants completed the Funk et al31 15-item Attitudes Towards Violence Scale (α = .76). Participants used a 7-point Likert scale (−3, “very harmful to my health to +3, “very helpful to my health”) to indicate the perceived health benefit or risk of engaging in alcohol use, marijuana use, and sex without use of a condom. Attitudes with greater negative values indicate greater perception of harm.
Hostile Social Information Processing
Participants watched a videotaped social scenario and responded to a structured clinical interview designed to measure hostile cognitive bias in evaluating the scenario.32 In the scenario, a teacher alerts his class to his suspicion that some students have cheated and dwells at length on his disappointment in students who have cheated and his pride in students who have earned their test scores. The teacher hands back a high test score to Billy and asks to speak with him at the end of class. Participants imagined that they were Billy and rated the likelihood that the teacher would accuse them of cheating on a 5-point Likert scale (1, “not at all likely” to 5, “very likely”).
Participants engaged in a “mixed-motive” game with a confederate, during which they could cooperate or compete to earn points exchangeable for money at the completion of the study (all participants were later given $5).33 On each of 30 trials, the participant chose to divide points worth money with his “partner” (in actuality, a preprogrammed computer), either equally by clicking on the “cooperate” box on the computer screen or unequally by clicking on the “compete” box. The “partner” cooperated only 30% of the time, creating a situation that could be construed by the participant as provocation, while also providing an economical incentive for the participant to cooperate (if both the participant and partner chose to compete, both lost the maximum number of points). “Compete” responses were tallied across 30 trials.
Parental Socioeconomic Status—Covariate
When completing the survey, participants reported the maximum years of education achieved by any parent in their primary household (mean [SD], 16.6 [3.1] years [range, 11-24 years]).
Association With Deviant Peers—Covariate
During the survey, participants completed a 13-item questionnaire to indicate their association with deviant peers,34 both for current (α = .83) and high school (α = .84) friends. The 2 scores (r = 0.65; P<.001) were summed to create an overall composite for association with deviant peers (mean [SD], 35.6 [14.0]). Low scores indicated that few of a participant's friends had ever engaged in any deviant behavior (eg, smoking cigarettes, hitting other people, cheating on tests).
For each outcome separately, we tested main effects of the media violence condition (dichotomous) and real-world violence exposure (continuous) by including both variables in the first step of a univariate general linear model. When main effects were significant, we calculated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for mean differences between violence exposure groups based on a median split for the continuous lifetime violence exposure variables. When interaction terms (tested in a second univariate model) were significant, we conducted simple effects tests to determine whether the media violence condition was associated with an outcome within high– and low–home and community violence exposure groups and compared effect sizes within each group using partial η2. For all significant main effects and interactions, analyses were performed again with inclusion of covariates (composite rating of video game excitement and fast action, prior experience playing video games, ethnicity, parental socioeconomic status, association with deviant peers). We conducted separate analyses to test the effects of home, community, and total real-world violence exposure. P values ≤.05 (2-tailed) were considered statistically significant.
Table 1 presents means and standard deviations of violence exposure and laboratory outcome measures and their intercorrelations. Table 2 presents correlations between these measures and covariates.
As hypothesized, young men randomly assigned to play Grand Theft Auto III exhibited greater changes in SBP (mean difference, 3.58 [95% CI, 0.41-6.75]) and diastolic blood pressure (mean difference, 2.84 [95% CI, 0.29-5.39]), greater negative affect (mean difference, 1.86 [95% CI, 0.34-3.38]), more permissive attitudes toward using alcohol (mean difference, 0.46 [95% CI, 0.09-0.83]) and marijuana (mean difference, 0.62 [95% CI, 0.18-1.06]), and greater uncooperative behavior (mean difference, 1.89 [95% CI, 0.13-3.65]) in comparison with young adults randomly assigned to play The Simpsons: Hit and Run (Table 3). Contrary to hypothesis, the violent video game condition was not associated with hostile social information processing, permissive attitudes toward violence, and attitudes toward sexual activity without condom use.
Consistent with our hypothesis, greater total home and community violence exposure was associated with greater changes in SBP (mean difference, 3.52 [95% CI, 0.26-6.78]) (Table 3). Domain-specific analyses (Table 1) suggested that community violence exposure, in particular, was associated with greater changes in SBP. Greater total home and community violence exposure was associated with more permissive attitudes toward violence (mean difference, 0.30 [95% CI, 0.13-0.47]) and drinking alcohol (mean difference, 0.42 [95% CI, 0.05-0.79] (Table 3). Associations remained when home and community violence exposure were examined separately. Domain-specific analyses revealed that greater home violence exposure was associated with more hostile attributions during the social scenario (mean difference, 0.50 [95% CI, 0.11-0.88]). Contrary to hypothesis, real-world violence exposure variables were not associated with negative affect and uncooperative behavior.
Two interactions between lifetime violence exposure variables and media violence condition emerged (Table 3). Within the low–total violence exposure group, the video game condition was not associated with change in SBP, while within the high–total violence exposure group, play of Grand Theft Auto III predicted greater changes in SBP than play of The Simpsons: Hit and Run. The violent video game condition explained roughly 21% of the variance in SBP change within the high–total violence exposure group and virtually none of the variance in SBP change within the low–total violence exposure group. Interactions between the video game condition and both community and home violence exposure yielded identical patterns to that described for total violence exposure.
Violence exposure within the community interacted with the video game condition to predict hostile attributions during the social scenario (F = 4.0; P<.05). The video game condition was not associated with hostile attributions within the low–community violence exposure group (F = 0.96; partial η2 = 0.02) but was associated with more hostile attributions within the high–community violence exposure group (F = 3.0; P<.10; partial η2 = 0.06).
Analyses controlling for covariates showed the same pattern of results with few exceptions: the violent video game condition was no longer associated with negative affect; lifetime violence exposure was no longer associated with SBP and alcohol use.
In response to playing a very popular video game, Grand Theft Auto III, young men exhibited increased blood pressure, more negative affect, and more hostile social information processing, consistent with the results of other experimental studies and the General Aggression Model.1,6 Participants randomly assigned to play Grand Theft Auto III also expressed more permissive attitudes toward using alcohol and marijuana and were less cooperative during a subsequent task than participants randomly assigned to play a less violent video game. To our knowledge, the present study is the first experimental study to show that media violence is associated with permissive attitudes toward health risk behaviors that do not directly involve hostility or aggression.
Young men with higher levels of home and community violence exposure may be more susceptible to certain outcomes as a result of media violence exposure. In the present study, media violence exposure was associated with greater SBP and hostile social information processing among participants with greater lifetime violence exposure. The act in Grand Theft Auto III that participants were asked to commit (ie, striking another person with a baseball bat) may have appeared more realistic to those who had witnessed or been victimized by violence within the home or community, thus resulting in greater physiological response. Perhaps owing to learning experiences, individuals from violent communities may have greater difficulty imagining a nonhostile interpretation for the ambiguous actions of others after playing a video game that “primes” hostile thought patterns. Main effects of media violence exposure on negative affect, uncooperative behavior, and permissive attitudes toward health risk behavior were also observed in the present study. These findings demonstrate that all young adults, regardless of background, are potentially affected by media violence exposure.
Strengths of the present study include the use of random assignment and the selection of commonly played games, which are likely to increase the generalizability of results. Limitations include the inability to generalize findings beyond young men who were sufficiently advantaged to attend a major public university. In addition, selected video games may have differed in ways other than violence; these differences, rather than violent content, may have influenced outcomes. The finding most vulnerable to this possibility is the link observed between play of Grand Theft Auto III and permissive attitudes toward using alcohol and marijuana. Because Grand Theft Auto III includes characters who are drug dealers, it is possible that observation of a drug dealer may have relaxed players' attitudes toward alcohol and marijuana use. However, participants randomly assigned to play Grand Theft Auto III took a punitive stance against drugs during their “mission” (ie, striking a drug dealer with a baseball bat), raising the possibility that other mechanisms may have been responsible for the relaxing of attitudes toward alcohol and marijuana use (eg, violent content may not only appear to condone aggression but also general risk-taking and law-breaking behavior). The General Aggression Model also suggests that media violence exposure may increase the likelihood that individuals engage in impulsive rather than thoughtful courses of action.6
School-based research on young children shows that negative effects of media violence exposure are reduced when use of television and video games are reduced,35 when attitude-change interventions emphasize the unrealistic nature of media,36,37 and when parents coview and comment on violent TV programs.38 Results of the present study suggest that media violence exposure may predispose adolescents and young adults toward greater engagement in general health risk behaviors and toward tension and conflict in social interactions with others. Adolescents and their parents may benefit from media education campaigns.
Correspondence: Sonya S. Brady, PhD, Health Psychology Program, University of California, San Francisco, 3333 California St, Suite 465, Box 0848, San Francisco, CA 94143-0848 (sonya.brady@ucsf.edu).
Accepted for Publication: December 22, 2005.
Funding/Support: This research was supported by Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award T32 MH019391 and grants HL07852, HL07858, and HL25767 and a professional development award through the University of Pittsburgh Provost's Office, Pittsburgh, Pa.
Acknowledgment: We thank Sabrina DellaMea, BS, Heather Koston, BS, Diana Buck, BA, Leslie Mitrik, BS, and Karen Kenyon, BS, for assistance during data collection, entry, and coding. We also thank Adam Rixey, BA, for his skill in playing the 4-minute video game segments that were recorded and shown to participants.
1.Anderson
CABerkowitz
LDonnerstein
E
et al. The influence of media violence on youth
Psychological Sci Public Interest 2003;481- 110
Google Scholar 3.Huesmann
LR Psychological processes promoting the relation between exposure to media violence and aggressive behavior by the viewer
J Soc Issues 1986;42125- 139
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Huesmann
LRMoise-Titus
JPodolski
CEron
LD Longitudinal relations between children's exposure to TV violence and their aggressive and violent behavior in young adulthood: 1977-1992
Dev Psychol 2003;39201- 221
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Johnson
JGCohen
PSmailes
EMKasen
SBrook
JS Television viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood
Science 2002;2952468- 2471
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Berkowitz
L On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: a cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis
Am Psychol 1990;45494- 503
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Huesmann
LRGeen
RGedDonnerstein
Eed The role of social information processing and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior
Human Aggression: Theories, Research and Implications for Policy. New York, NY Academic Press1998;73- 109
Google Scholar 10.Acierno
RKilpatrick
DGResnick
HSaunders
BDe Arellano
MBest
C Assault, PTSD, family substance use, and depression as risk factors for cigarette use in youth: findings from the National Survey of Adolescents
J Trauma Stress 2000;13381- 396
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Albus
KEWeist
MDPerez-Smith
AM Associations between youth risk behavior and exposure to violence: implications for the provision of mental health services in urban schools
Behav Modif 2004;28548- 564
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Berenson
ABWiemann
CMMcCombs
S Exposure to violence and associated health-risk behaviors among adolescent girls
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2001;1551238- 1242
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Kilpatrick
DGAcierno
RSaunders
BResnick
HBest
CLSchnurr
PP Risk factors for adolescent substance abuse and dependence: data from a national sample
J Consult Clin Psychol 2000;6819- 30
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Schwab-Stone
MEAyers
TSKasprow
W
et al. No safe haven: a study of violence exposure in an urban community
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995;341343- 1352
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Silverman
JGRaj
AMucci
LAHathaway
JE Dating violence against adolescent girls and associated substance use, unhealthy weight control, sexual risk behavior, pregnancy, and suicidality
JAMA 2001;286572- 579
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Sullivan
TNKung
EMFarrell
AD Relation between witnessing violence and drug use initiation among rural adolescents: parental monitoring and family support as protective factors
J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2004;33488- 498
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Krcmar
MGreene
K Connections between violent television exposure and adolescent risk taking
Media Psychology 2000;2195- 217
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Schwartz
DProctor
LJ Community violence exposure and children's social adjustment in the school peer group: the mediating roles of emotion regulation and social cognition
J Consult Clin Psychol 2000;68670- 683
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Shahinfar
AKupersmidt
JBMatza
LS The relation between exposure to violence and social information processing among incarcerated adolescents
J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110136- 141
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Kirsh
SJOlczak
PV Violent comic books and perceptions of ambiguous provocation situations
Media Psychology 2000;247- 62
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Bushman
BJAnderson
CA Violent video games and hostile expectations: a test of the General Aggression Model
Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2002;281679- 1686
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Huesmann
LRMoise
JFPodolski
CStoff
DMedBreiling
JedMaser
JDed The effects of media violence on the development of antisocial behavior
Handbook of Antisocial Behavior. New York, NY John Wiley & Sons, Inc1997;181- 193
Google Scholar 25.Dysart
JMTreiber
FAPflieger
KDavis
HStrong
WB Ethnic differences in the myocardial and vascular reactivity to stress in normotensive girls
Am J Hypertens 1994;715- 22
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Treiber
FADavis
HMusante
L
et al. Ethnicity, gender, family history of myocardial infarction, and hemodynamic responses to laboratory stressors in children
Health Psychol 1993;126- 15
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Anderson
CADill
KE Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and life
J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;78772- 790
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Anderson
CAFord
CM Affect of the game player: short-term effects of highly and mildly aggressive video games
Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1986;12390- 402
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Usala
PDHertog
C Measurement of affective states in adults: evaluation of an adjective rating scale instrument
Res Aging 1989;11403- 426
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Funk
JBElliott
RUrman
MLFlores
GTMock
RM The Attitudes Towards Violence Scale: a measure for adolescents
J Interpers Violence 1999;141123- 1136
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Chen
EMatthews
KA Development of the Cognitive Appraisal and Understanding of Social Events (CAUSE) videos
Health Psychol 2003;22106- 110
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Van Egeren
LFFabrega
HThornton
DW Electrocardiographic effects of social stress on coronary-prone (type A) individuals
Psychosom Med 1983;45195- 203
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Metzler
CWNoell
JBiglan
AAry
DSmolkowski
K The social context for risky sexual behavior among adolescents
J Behav Med 1994;17419- 438
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Robinson
TNWilde
MLNavracruz
LCHaydel
KFVarady
A Effects of reducing children's television and video game use on aggressive behavior: a randomized controlled trial
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2001;15517- 23
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Huesmann
LREron
LDKlein
RBrice
PFischer
P Mitigating the imitation of aggressive behaviors by children's attitudes about media violence
J Pers Soc Psychol 1983;44899- 910
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Rosenkoetter
LIRosenkoetter
SEOzretich
RAAcock
AC Mitigating the harmful effects of violent television
J Appl Dev Psychol 2004;2525- 47
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Nathanson
AI Identifying and explaining the relationship between parental mediation and children's aggression
Communic Res 1999;26124- 143
Google ScholarCrossref