[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure.  Teacher-Rated Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years
Teacher-Rated Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years

Behavior scores at age 6 split by quartile with group 1 having the lowest score and group 4 the highest. Aggression displayed as terciles owing to insufficient variance in the data. Boxes represent the interquartile range, where the central horizontal line represents the median and the lower and upper horizontal lines represent the 25th and 75th percentiles respectively. Whiskers represent 1.5 times the interquartile range. Outliers suppressed in accordance with Statistics Canada data protection requirements. Earnings are in US dollars.

Table 1.  Personal and Family Characteristics of the Overall Sample vs Study Sample at Age 6 Years
Personal and Family Characteristics of the Overall Sample vs Study Sample at Age 6 Years
Table 2.  Earnings and Family Characteristics at Follow-up
Earnings and Family Characteristics at Follow-up
Table 3.  Partially Adjusted Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years
Partially Adjusted Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years
Table 4.  Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Employment Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years
Mixed-Effects Linear Regression Models of Association Between Behaviors at Age 6 Years and Employment Earnings at Age 35 to 36 Years
1.
Christenson  JD, Crane  DR, Malloy  J, Parker  S.  The cost of oppositional defiant disorder and disruptive behavior: a review of the literature.  J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25(9):2649-2658. doi:10.1007/s10826-016-0430-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Polanczyk  G, de Lima  MS, Horta  BL, Biederman  J, Rohde  LA.  The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942-948. doi:10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Doshi  JA, Hodgkins  P, Kahle  J,  et al.  Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(10):990-1002. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Costello  EJ, Mustillo  S, Erkanli  A, Keeler  G, Angold  A.  Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):837-844. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Knapp  M, King  D, Healey  A, Thomas  C.  Economic outcomes in adulthood and their associations with antisocial conduct, attention deficit and anxiety problems in childhood.  J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2011;14(3):137-147.PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Healey  A, Knapp  M, Farrington  DP.  Adult labour market implications of antisocial behaviour in childhood and adolescence: findings from a UK longitudinal study.  Appl Econ. 2004;36(2):93-105. doi:10.1080/0003684042000174001Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Alatupa  S, Pulkki-Råback  L, Hintsanen  M, Elovainio  M, Mullola  S, Keltikangas-Järvinen  L.  Disruptive behavior in childhood and socioeconomic position in adulthood: a prospective study over 27 years.  Int J Public Health. 2013;58(2):247-256. doi:10.1007/s00038-012-0408-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Feinstein  L.  The Relative Economic Importance of Academic, Psychological and Behavioural Attributes Developed on Childhood. London, UK: Centre for Economic Performance, London School of Economics and Political Science; 2000.
9.
Paul  KI, Moser  K.  Unemployment impairs mental health: meta-analyses.  J Vocat Behav. 2009;74(3):264-282. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2009.01.001Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Jin  RL, Shah  CP, Svoboda  TJ.  The impact of unemployment on health: a review of the evidence.  CMAJ. 1995;153(5):529-540.PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Roelfs  DJ, Shor  E, Davidson  KW, Schwartz  JE.  Losing life and livelihood: a systematic review and meta-analysis of unemployment and all-cause mortality.  Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(6):840-854. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Chetty  R, Stepner  M, Abraham  S,  et al.  The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001-2014.  JAMA. 2016;315(16):1750-1766. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.4226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Santiago  CD, Wadsworth  ME, Stump  J.  Socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related stress: prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse low-income families.  J Econ Psychol. 2011;32(2):218-230. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.008Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Heckman  JJ.  Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children.  Science. 2006;312(5782):1900-1902. doi:10.1126/science.1128898PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Kautz  T, Heckman  JJ, Diris  R, ter Weel  B, Borghans  L.  Fostering and Measuring Skills: Improving Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skills to Promote Lifetime Success. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2014. doi:10.3386/w20749
16.
Campbell  F, Conti  G, Heckman  JJ,  et al.  Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health.  Science. 2014;343(6178):1478-1485. doi:10.1126/science.1248429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fergusson  DM, Horwood  LJ, Ridder  EM.  Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):850-858. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Moffitt  TE, Arseneault  L, Belsky  D,  et al.  A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693-2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Jones  DE, Greenberg  M, Crowley  M.  Early social-emotional functioning and public health: the relationship between kindergarten social competence and future wellness.  Am J Public Health. 2015;105(11):2283-2290. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2015.302630PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Furnham  A, Cheng  H.  Factors influencing adult earnings: findings from a nationally representative sample.  J Socio-Econ.2013;44(suppl C):120-125. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.008Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Hanushek  EA, Woessmann  L.  The role of cognitive skills in economic development.  J Econ Lit. 2008;46(3):607-668. doi:10.1257/jel.46.3.607Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Schmidt  FL, Hunter  J.  General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment and job performance.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86(1):162-173. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Schmidt  FL. Select on Intelligence. In: Locke  EA, ed.  Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2012:1-17.
24.
Heckman  JJ, Stixrud  J, Urzua  S.  The Effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior.  J Labor Econ. 2006;24(3):411-482. doi:10.1086/504455Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Almlund  M, Duckworth  AL, Heckman  JJ, Kautz  TD.  Personality Psychology and Economics. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2011. doi:10.3386/w16822
26.
Heckman  J, Pinto  R, Savelyev  P.  Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes.  Am Econ Rev. 2013;103(6):2052-2086. doi:10.1257/aer.103.6.2052PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Fergusson  DM, Boden  JM, Horwood  LJ.  Childhood self-control and adult outcomes: results from a 30-year longitudinal study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):709-7171. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bound  J, Krueger  AB.  The extent of measurement error in longitudinal earnings data: do two wrongs make a right?  J Labor Econ. 1991;9(1):1-24. doi:10.1086/298256Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Converse  PD, Piccone  KA, Tocci  MC.  Childhood self-control, adolescent behavior, and career success.  Pers Individ Dif. 2014;59(suppl C):65-70. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.007Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Boisjoli  R, Vitaro  F, Lacourse  E, Barker  ED, Tremblay  RE.  Impact and clinical significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys: 15-year follow-up.  Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:415-419. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.030007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Castellanos-Ryan  N, Brière  FN, O’Leary-Barrett  M,  et al; IMAGEN Consortium.  The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125(8):1039-1052. doi:10.1037/abn0000193PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Nagin  D, Tremblay  RE.  Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency.  Child Dev. 1999;70(5):1181-1196. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00086PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Tremblay  RE, Loeber  R, Gagnon  C, Charlebois  P, Larivée  S, LeBlanc  M.  Disruptive boys with stable and unstable high fighting behavior patterns during junior elementary school.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1991;19(3):285-300. doi:10.1007/BF00911232PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Swanson  JM, Arnold  LE, Molina  BSG,  et al; MTA Cooperative Group.  Young adult outcomes in the follow-up of the multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: symptom persistence, source discrepancy, and height suppression.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(6):663-678. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12684PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Murray  AL, Obsuth  I, Eisner  M, Ribeaud  D.  Evaluating longitudinal invariance in dimensions of mental health across adolescence: an analysis of the social behavior questionnaire.  Assessment. 2017;1073191117721741. doi:10.1177/1073191117721741PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Weir  K, Stevenson  J, Graham  P.  Behavioral deviance and teacher ratings of prosocial behavior: preliminary findings.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1980;19(1):68-77. doi:10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60653-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Findlay  LC, Beasley  E, Park  J,  et al.  Longitudinal child data: what can be gained by linking administrative data and cohort data?  Int J Popul Data Sci. 2018;3(21). doi:10.23889/ijpds.v3i1.451Google Scholar
38.
Lorge  I, Thorndike  R.  The Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test. New York, NY: Houghton; 1950.
39.
Mackintosh  NJ.  IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2011.
40.
Blishen  BR, Carroll  WK, Moore  C.  The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada.  Can Rev Sociol. 1987;24(4):465-488. doi:10.1111/j.1755-618X.1987.tb00639.xGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Selya  AS, Rose  JS, Dierker  LC, Hedeker  D, Mermelstein  RJ.  A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED.  Front Psychol. 2012;3:111. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Cohen  J.  Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioral Science. 2nd ed. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988.
43.
Fletcher  J.  The Effects of Childhood ADHD on Adult Labor Market Outcomes. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2013. doi:10.3386/w18689
44.
Loe  IM, Feldman  HM.  Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD.  Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(1)(suppl):82-90. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2006.05.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Pingault  J-B, Tremblay  RE, Vitaro  F,  et al.  Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: a 16-year longitudinal population-based study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(11):1164-1170. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Burke  JD, Rowe  R, Boylan  K.  Functional outcomes of child and adolescent oppositional defiant disorder symptoms in young adult men.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):264-272. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12150PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Mrug  S, Molina  BSG, Hoza  B,  et al.  Peer rejection and friendships in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: contributions to long-term outcomes.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40(6):1013-1026. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9610-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Asher  S, Coie  J. Peer Rejection in Childhood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
49.
Bierman  K.  Peer Rejection: Developmental Processes and Intervention Strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2004.
50.
Barkley  RA, Murphy  KR.  Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests.  Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(3):157-173. doi:10.1093/arclin/acq014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Clarke  TL.  Executive Functions and Overt/Covert Patterns of Conduct Disorder Symptoms in Children With ADHD [dissertation]. College Park, Maryland: University of Maryland; 2009.
52.
Fletcher  J, Wolfe  B.  Long-term consequences of childhood ADHD on criminal activities.  J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2009;12(3):119-138.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Shaw  M, Hodgkins  P, Caci  H,  et al.  A systematic review and analysis of long-term outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of treatment and non-treatment.  BMC Med. 2012;10:99. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-10-99PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Molina  BSG, Hinshaw  SP, Swanson  JM,  et al; MTA Cooperative Group.  The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(5):484-500. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
The MTA Cooperative Group. Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD.  A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1073-1086. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Diamond  A, Lee  K.  Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old.  Science. 2011;333(6045):959-964. doi:10.1126/science.1204529PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Wyman  PA, Cross  W, Hendricks Brown  C, Yu  Q, Tu  X, Eberly  S.  Intervention to strengthen emotional self-regulation in children with emerging mental health problems: proximal impact on school behavior.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(5):707-720. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9398-xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Parker  J, Wales  G, Chalhoub  N, Harpin  V.  The long-term outcomes of interventions for the management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials.  Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2013;6:87-99. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S49114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Durlak  JA, Weissberg  RP, Dymnicki  AB, Taylor  RD, Schellinger  KB.  The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions.  Child Dev. 2011;82(1):405-432. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Durlak  JA, Weissberg  RP, Pachan  M.  A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek to promote personal and social skills in children and adolescents.  Am J Community Psychol. 2010;45(3-4):294-309. doi:10.1007/s10464-010-9300-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
February 11, 2019

Association of Behavior in Boys From Low Socioeconomic Neighborhoods With Employment Earnings in Adulthood

Author Affiliations
  • 1Research Unit on Children’s Psychosocial Maladjustment (GRIP), Université de Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
  • 2Department of Pediatrics and Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
  • 3School of Public Health, Physiotherapy and Sport Science, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 4Ste-Justine Hospital Research Center, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
  • 5Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
  • 6Sciences Po Economic Department, L’Observatoire Français des Conjonctures Économiques (OFCE), Paris, France
  • 7Centre pour la recherche économique et ses applications (CEPREMAP), Paris, France
  • 8Statistics Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
  • 9Université de Bordeaux, INSERM U1219, Centre Hospitalier Perrens, Bordeaux, France
  • 10School of Psycho-Education, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada
JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(4):334-341. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5375
Key Points

Question  Which disruptive behaviors in kindergarten are associated with employment earnings in adulthood for boys from low socioeconomic backgrounds?

Findings  This 30-year follow-up study of 920 boys found that kindergarten teachers’ ratings of inattention were associated with lower earnings at age 35 to 36 years and prosocial behavior with higher earnings after adjustment for child IQ and family adversity. Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not associated with earnings.

Meaning  Preventive interventions targeting children’s inattention and/or limited prosocial behavior could have positive impacts on future employment earnings.

Abstract

Importance  Identifying early childhood behavioral problems associated with economic success/failure is essential for the development of targeted interventions that enhance economic prosperity through improved educational attainment and social integration.

Objective  To test the association between kindergarten teacher–rated assessments of inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality in boys with their employment earnings at age 35 to 36 years as measured by government tax return data.

Design, Setting, and Participants  A 30-year prospective follow-up study analyzing low socioeconomic neighborhoods in Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Boys aged 5 to 6 years attending kindergarten in low socioeconomic neighborhoods were recruited. Teacher-rated behavioral assessments were obtained for 1040 boys. Data were collected from April 1984 to December 2015. Analysis began January 2017.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Mixed-effects linear regression models were used to examine the association between teacher ratings of inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality at age 6 years and individual earnings obtained from government tax returns at age 35 to 36 years. The IQ of the child and family adversity were adjusted for in the analysis.

Results  Complete data were available for 920 study participants (mean age at follow-up was 36.3 years). Mean (SD) personal earnings at follow-up were $28 865.53 ($24 103.45) (range, $0-$142 267.84). A 1-unit increase in inattention (mean [SD], 2.66 [2.34]; range, 0-8) at age 6 years was associated with decrease in earnings at age 35 to 36 years of $1295.13 (95% CI, −$2051.65 to −$538.62), while a unit increase in prosociality (mean [SD], 8.0 [4.96]; range, 0-20) was associated with an increase in earnings of $406.15 (95% CI, $172.54-$639.77). Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not significantly associated with earnings. Child IQ was associated with higher earnings and family adversity with lower earnings in all models. A 1-SD reduction in inattention at age 6 years was associated with a theoretical increase in annual earnings of $3040.41, a similar magnitude to an equivalent increase in IQ.

Conclusions and Relevance  Teacher ratings of inattention and prosociality in kindergarten boys from low socioeconomic neighborhoods are associated with earnings in adulthood after adjustment for hyperactivity, aggression, and opposition, which were not associated with earnings. Interventions beginning in kindergarten that target boys’ inattention and enhance prosociality could positively impact workforce integration and earnings.

Introduction

Childhood disruptive behaviors are among the most prevalent and costly mental health problems in industrialized countries and are associated with significant negative long-term outcomes for individuals and society.1-4 Recent evidence suggests that the presence of disruptive behavioral problems in the first years of life is an important early predictor of lower employment earnings in adulthood.5-8 Low earnings can harm individual and family well-being for many years and are associated with increased risk of financial dependence, stress, psychopathology, and early mortality.9-13 It is therefore essential to determine which individual behaviors contribute specifically to low earnings so that policy and preventive interventions can be used to target at-risk children with interventions and support.14-16

The longitudinal association between childhood traits and employment earnings is well documented,5-8,17-19 and both cognitive and noncognitive traits are believed to contribute to future earnings. The most frequently examined cognitive trait is intelligence, usually measured in terms of academic performance or IQ, which is strongly associated with occupational attainment, performance, and earnings.17,20-23 However, so-called noncognitive traits, such as self-control, self-esteem, and personality, are also known to be important.24 Interest in the role of noncognitive behavioral determinants of later earnings has grown recently on the back of evidence demonstrating that they are modifiable, perhaps more so than cognitive abilities, and therefore important targets for intervention.24-26

The longitudinal association between childhood disruptive behaviors and earnings in early adulthood is documented in a small but growing literature.5-8 Most studies have focused on the traits of aggression, opposition, hyperactivity, and inattention, as well as the related concept of self-control. Their findings are relatively consistent: higher levels of disruptive behaviors and lower levels of self-control, assessed when children are aged between 3 and 11 years, are associated with lower earnings and less wealth in early adulthood (age 26 to 36 years) after adjustment for the child’s intelligence and family socioeconomic status (SES).5-8,18,27 These disruptive behavioral traits are highly relevant from a research and policy point of view because they underpin some of the most prevalent1,2 and costly3,4 childhood psychiatric disorders, including conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Early interventions targeting clinical or subclinical disruptive behavioral problems therefore have the potential to yield wide-ranging social and economic benefits for individuals and society.26

The body of research documenting longitudinal associations between childhood disruptive behaviors and later earnings has important limitations. First, previous studies have used self-reported income. This methodology is subject to limitations including selective dropout (eg, individuals from low SES backgrounds), refusal to report income, social desirability bias (eg, overreporting or underreporting value of earnings), nondeliberate missreporting (eg, lack of knowledge about income or the value of welfare/insurance/childcare entitlements), and deliberate falsification. The use of government tax records is therefore preferable: they account for all sources of income (eg, salaries/wages, tax reductions, welfare credits), provide almost complete population coverage, and are usually reported by impartial third parties (eg, employers) and are consequently more accurate than self-reports, particularly for male individuals.28 Second, previous studies of childhood self-control18,27,29 and antisocial behavior5-7 have typically combined multiple behavioral dimensions (eg, inattention/hyperactivity, aggression/opposition) to create composite indices. The use of composite measures does not allow quantification of the cumulative or interactive contributions of behaviors, making it difficult to identify behaviors that can be targeted with preventive interventions. Identifying specific behaviors is important because it helps to specify causal pathways (ie, mediators) that can be used in targeted intervention programs, which are more effective and efficient than nontargeted generalized interventions.14 Third, several studies have averaged behavioral assessment across multiple years (eg, age 3 to 11 years),18,27 used assessments made in middle childhood (eg, age 10 years),5,6,8 or used parents’ rather than teachers’ ratings. Ideally, from the point of view of the development of preventive interventions, assessments should be made by teachers (they have a sense of normative behavior that parents often lack) at a single time point and as early as possible when children are most likely to benefit (ie, in kindergarten).30,31 These early assessments also provide the first opportunity for populationwide assessment that include children from low SES backgrounds who are less likely to attend daycare. Fourth, few previous studies have considered the effects of positive traits, such as prosociality, which could counteract the negative effects of disruptive behaviors and enhance earnings. Finally, previous studies have not examined effects in samples of male individuals from low SES neighborhoods. This group is at risk of both early behavioral problems and lower lifetime earnings, so early identification and intervention should yield high returns for individuals and society.

The aim of this study was to examine the association between 5 prevalent behaviors assessed in kindergarten—inattention, hyperactivity, aggression, opposition, and prosociality—and earnings averaged across age 35 and 36 years in a sample of boys from low SES neighborhoods. Behavioral assessments were based on teacher ratings at age 5 to 6 years, and earnings data were obtained from government tax return records. To test the possibility that some behaviors operate interactively rather than additively, 2-way interactions between all behaviors were examined.

Methods
Participants and Behavior Assessment

Teacher-rated behavioral assessments were obtained for boys aged 6 years (n = 1040) attending 53 schools in the poorest neighborhoods of Montreal, Quebec, Canada, using data from 1984.32 The study was approved by the University of Montreal ethics board. Informed written consent was obtained from children’s parents prior to participation. Data were collected from April 1984 to December 2015 and analyzed from January 2017 to May 2018.

Behavioral ratings were made by the child’s teacher using the well-validated Social Behavior Questionnaire.33-35 Inattention was assessed with 4 items: poor concentration, distracted, head in the clouds, and lack of persistence. Hyperactivity was assessed with 2 items: agitated/fidgety and moves constantly. Opposition was assessed with 5 items: disobeys, does not share materials, blames others, inconsiderate, and irritable. Physical aggression was assessed with 3 items: fights with other children, bullies/intimidates other children, and kicks/bites. Prosociality, defined as behaviors intended to benefit others or society including helping, sharing, and cooperating,36 was assessed with 10 items; examples of prosociality are tries to stop quarrels or disputes, will invite bystanders to join in a game, and will try to help someone who has been hurt. Items were rated on a 3-point scale with 0 indicating never/not true; 1, sometimes/somewhat true; and 2, often/very true. These scores were summed for each behavior. α Scores for inattention, hyperactivity, opposition, aggression, and prosociality were .81, .89, .84, .87, and .92, respectively.

Outcomes and Control Variables

Outcome data were obtained from government tax return records (Statistics Canada) and linked to participants’ study data (a description of the methods of linking administrative tax information with child-focused survey data has been previously published37). The successful linkage rate for participants with at least 1 tax return for 2014 or 2015 was 87.53%; there were no significant differences in baseline characteristics for cases with and without successful linkages (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Tax return data for each year included personal pretax earnings, marital status, and the number of children in the household. Earnings were defined as all pretax wages, salaries, and commissions, not including income from capital gains, and averaged for the 2 most recent years (2014 and 2015; r = 0.93). Currency data were collected as Canadian dollars but are reported as 2019 US dollars (CaD $1 = US $0.75) throughout this article.

Child IQ was assessed at age 13 years using the Sentence Completion Task.38 IQ is generally stable between childhood and adolescence39 so the assessment at age 13 years provides a good estimate of the child’s cognitive abilities at age 6 years. The correlation between the Sentence Completion Task at age 13 years and the vocabulary and block design subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised at age 10 years was 0.67 when conducted on a subsample of 80.

Family adversity was calculated by combining the following variables assessed at age 6 years: parents’ educational level, family structure (intact vs nonintact), parents’ age at birth of the first child, and parents’ occupational status based on the criteria described by Blishen et al.40 Families at or below the 30th percentile for each of these indices (or a nonintact family) were coded as having 1 adversity point. Scores were standardized on a scale of 0 to 1.

Statistical Analyses

After confirming the missing at random assumption, missing data were managed using multiple imputations by chained equations. Models were estimated across 50 data sets, and the results were pooled. Mixed-effects linear regression models were used to control for clustering effects in predictors at the school (n = 53) and classroom (n = 152) levels with fixed-effects estimates reported. Five multivariable partially adjusted models were estimated (1 for each behavior) controlling for child IQ and family adversity in each model. Next, a fully adjusted model including all behaviors, IQ, and adversity was estimated. Robust SEs were used to account for heteroskedasticity. Tests for 2-way continuous interactions between the 5 childhood behaviors were conducted in a single model. Behaviors and their interaction terms were centered prior to analysis. Effect sizes were calculated as the ratio of the coefficient estimate to the variable’s SD. Standardized effect sizes were calculated for the overall models from R2 values41 and interpreted using Cohen ƒ2, where ƒ2 ≥ 0.02, ƒ2 ≥ 0.15, and ƒ2 ≥ 0.35 represent small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.42 Finally, to estimate lost earnings over the course of a career, a financial effect was calculated:

Image description not available.

 in which βj is the estimate effect of variable j (eg, inattention) on earnings and stdj is the SD of variable j. The financial effect measures the present value over a 40-year work career of a 1-SD improvement in variable j assuming the commonly used discount rate of 3%. Thus, for inattention, financial effect measures the earnings gain from a 1-SD reduction, whereas for prosociality, it measures the earnings gain from a 1-SD increase.

Sensitivity Analyses

First, a variance inflation factor test of multicollinearity revealed that aggression and opposition were highly correlated; they approached but did not exceed the recommended threshold (variance inflation factor scores >5). As a precaution, these variables were standardized then summed to create a single item that was entered into the fully adjusted model. Second, a number of participants reported 0 earnings for the 2014 to 2015 tax years. To confirm that the results were not influenced by the presence of these zeros, a tobit regression with left censoring was used. Tobit regression simultaneously considers the likelihood of having data (any earnings greater than 0) and the value score for cases that have earnings greater than 0. Third, to test the robustness of associations between childhood behavior and earnings, we repeated the analysis using data from 2011 to 2012. Analyses were conducted using Stata version 14 (StataCorp). Significance was set at .05, and tests were 2 tailed.

Results
Participant Characteristics

Participants with complete outcome data were retained for analysis (N = 920). Child and family characteristics at age 6 years are shown in Table 1. There were no significant differences between participants with and without outcome data (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Participant earnings and family circumstances at follow-up are presented in Table 2. The mean (SD) earnings at age 35 to 36 years was $28 865.53 ($24 103.45) (range, $0-$142 129.75). Earnings for each behavior, split by quartile, are shown in the Figure.

In partially adjusted models, which controlled for child IQ and family adversity only, all behaviors at age 6 years were associated with earnings at age 35 to 36 years at the P < .001 level. (Table 3). The fully adjusted model is shown in Table 4. A 1-unit increase in inattention at age 6 years was associated with decrease in earnings of $1295.13 (95% CI, −$2051.65 to −$538.62), while a unit increase in prosociality was associated with an increase in earnings of $406.15 (95% CI, $172.54-$639.77), holding all other variables constant. Hyperactivity did not pass the significance threshold, although it had an effect size equivalent to that of prosociality. In all models, IQ was associated with higher earnings and family adversity with lower earnings. No 2-way interactions were found. Bivariate correlations are presented in eTable 2 in the Supplement.

Standardized effect sizes were largest for inattention followed by hyperactivity and prosociality. Viewed in monetary terms, a 1-SD reduction in inattention (SD, 2.34) was associated with an increase in annual earnings of $3040.41. Calculation of the financial effect for inattention over a 40-year career amounts to $70 532.97.

In the first sensitivity analysis, the merged opposition-aggression variable was nonsignificant (β = −162.17; P = .69; 95% CI, −633.96 to 958.31) and did not alter the overall model. Participants reporting no earnings in 2014 and 2015 (140 [15.2%]) had lower IQs, higher family adversity, and more behavioral problems (eTable 3 in the Supplement). Results from the tobit sensitivity analysis, which censored cases with no earnings, confirmed the results of the main analysis (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Replication of the analysis using 2011 to 2012 income data supported the robustness of the findings (eTable 5 in the Supplement).

Discussion

This study found that in a sample of boys living in low socioeconomic neighborhoods of a large North American city, behavioral assessments made by kindergarten teachers were associated with employment earnings 30 years later, as measured by government tax returns. Inattention was associated with lower earnings, while prosociality was associated with higher earnings after adjustment for the child’s IQ and family background. Hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression were not independently associated with earnings.

Our results are consistent with several previous studies showing that lower earnings are longitudinally associated with ratings of childhood attention,5,8,43 hyperactivity,7 and prosocial traits.19 Previous studies of the association between childhood opposition/aggression and earnings have produced mixed results. For instance, childhood aggression has been associated with lower SES in early adulthood,7 while antisocial behavior has been linked with lower earnings5,6 due largely to reduced rates of labor force participation. When only economically active men have been considered, however, 1 study6 found that childhood antisocial behavior/conduct problems were not associated with earnings, while another found that these men actually earned more.5 Our results help to clarify the contribution of distinct disruptive behaviors in early childhood by showing that while hyperactivity, opposition, and aggression are associated with later earnings on their own, these associations disappear once inattention and prosociality are adjusted for. They also highlight, for the first time to our knowledge, the positive association between childhood prosocial behaviors and later earnings. The failure to find significant interactions between behaviors is consistent with at least 1 previous study.5

Standardized effect sizes for individual behaviors and the overall model were in the small range. This was expected given that behavioral ratings were obtained from a single assessment rather than multiple assessments over multiple years, they were highly specific rather than composite scores of multiple behavioral dimensions, and the long duration of follow-up (30 years). The observed associations are therefore likely to represent an underestimation of the effect. Nevertheless, the effect size of individual behaviors (eg, inattention) on future earnings was of similar magnitude to that of IQ.

In monetary terms, the loss of employment earnings was nontrivial. A 1-SD reduction in inattention would be associated with an increase in annual earnings of $2963.18. Over a 40-year career, this would amount to $70 532.97. Since prosociality was associated with earnings after adjustment for inattention, a change in this behavior could further affect earnings. It is also highly likely that the harmful effects of childhood disruptive behaviors on earnings increase over time. This could occur through the accumulation of negative life events (eg, school failure, criminal convictions) that compound lost earnings, as well as through sectorial effects whereby people with low education and skills become trapped in job sectors with little or no wage growth.

Several plausible mechanisms may account for the associations observed in this study. There is already a well-established literature documenting the association between childhood disruptive behaviors and poor academic and educational attainment,44-46 and both are likely to mediate the association with employment earnings. Peer rejection, which is strongly associated with childhood symptoms of inattention-hyperactivity and low prosociality,47,48 could also function as an important mediator by lowering academic achievement and opportunities to learn and practice social skills.49 Other consequences of childhood disruptive behaviors, such as comorbid psychiatric problems in adulthood (eg, personality disorder, depression) or executive function deficits, which frequently cooccur with disruptive behaviors,50,51 could disrupt the ability to find and retain paid work and compromise workplace functioning, further diminishing earnings. Lower earnings could also be influenced by higher rates of substance use in adolescence and criminal convictions in early adulthood.52

Childhood inattention (which frequent cooccurs with hyperactivity) has been repeatedly linked with a wide range of negative long-term outcomes including higher rates of substance misuse, criminal conviction, educational failure, and unemployment,53 so targeting these behaviors could yield large returns across multiple life domains, including earnings. There are numerous interventions designed to target inattention-hyperactivity and low prosociality in children aged 5 to 8 years. The majority have been evaluated in terms of their impact on putative intermediary mechanisms, for example, education attainment, peer relationships, and executive function, described above, rather than on employment outcomes per se. Beyond the standard interventions, targeting inattention and hyperactivity symptoms (eg, pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic)54-58 and prosocial behaviors,59,60 educational interventions that improve academic outcomes should also be considered.

Strengths and Limitations

The strengths of this study were its long duration of follow-up (30 years), its use of objective measures of earnings (ie, government tax returns), its focus on a wider range of disruptive behaviors than previous studies, the use of teacher-rated behavioral assessments, the early age at which assessments were obtained (school entry), and the focus on male individuals from low SES backgrounds. However, this was an observational association study, and causal mechanisms underpinning the associations were not assessed. Future studies should investigate the causal pathways through which these associations occur to identify milestones that are important in the process linking early behavior problems to later earnings, such as educational attainment and social integration. This study examined earning as measured by government tax returns; it did not account for earnings obtained through the informal economy. Finally, this study focused on male individuals recruited from low-income neighborhoods of Montreal, which limits its generalizability. Future studies should examine these associations using low- and high-income neighborhood schools in other cultures and in larger population samples of female and male individuals.

Conclusions

Kindergarten teachers can identify boys from low socioeconomic backgrounds at risk of lower employment earnings 3 decades later. Monitoring of inattention and low prosocial behavior should begin in kindergarten so that at-risk boys can be identified and targeted with early intervention and support.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author Sylvana M. Côté, PhD, Ste-Justine’s Hospital Research Center, 3175, Côte Sainte-Catherine, Étage A, Local A-568, Montréal, QC H3T 1C5, Canada (sylvana.cote.1@umontreal.ca).

Accepted for Publication: December 3, 2018.

Published Online: February 11, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5375

Author Contributions: Dr Vergunst had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Vergunst, Tremblay, Nagin, Algan, Park, Côté.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Vergunst, Tremblay, Beasley, Park, Galera, Vitaro, Côté.

Drafting of the manuscript: Vergunst, Tremblay, Nagin, Côté.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Vergunst, Tremblay, Algan, Beasley, Park, Galera, Vitaro, Côté.

Statistical analysis: Vergunst, Tremblay, Nagin, Park, Vitaro, Côté.

Obtained funding: Vergunst, Tremblay, Algan, Vitaro, Côté.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Tremblay, Côté.

Supervision: Tremblay, Algan, Park, Côté.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Vergunst is funded by a Fonds de Recherche du Québec Santé postdoctoral fellowship. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study was conducted with support from the Quebec social and health research funds (Fond de Recherche du Québec–Société et Culture and Fond de Recherche du Québec–Santé), the Idex Fund from University of Bordeaux; the European Commission’s Horizon 2020 Program under European Research Council Consolidator Grant (grant 647870); Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, the Molson Foundation, the National Consortium 10 on Violence, and Statistics Canada.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: We thank all participating families for their continued commitment to the study.

References
1.
Christenson  JD, Crane  DR, Malloy  J, Parker  S.  The cost of oppositional defiant disorder and disruptive behavior: a review of the literature.  J Child Fam Stud. 2016;25(9):2649-2658. doi:10.1007/s10826-016-0430-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Polanczyk  G, de Lima  MS, Horta  BL, Biederman  J, Rohde  LA.  The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942-948. doi:10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Doshi  JA, Hodgkins  P, Kahle  J,  et al.  Economic impact of childhood and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the United States.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(10):990-1002. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.07.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Costello  EJ, Mustillo  S, Erkanli  A, Keeler  G, Angold  A.  Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(8):837-844. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Knapp  M, King  D, Healey  A, Thomas  C.  Economic outcomes in adulthood and their associations with antisocial conduct, attention deficit and anxiety problems in childhood.  J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2011;14(3):137-147.PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Healey  A, Knapp  M, Farrington  DP.  Adult labour market implications of antisocial behaviour in childhood and adolescence: findings from a UK longitudinal study.  Appl Econ. 2004;36(2):93-105. doi:10.1080/0003684042000174001Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Alatupa  S, Pulkki-Råback  L, Hintsanen  M, Elovainio  M, Mullola  S, Keltikangas-Järvinen  L.  Disruptive behavior in childhood and socioeconomic position in adulthood: a prospective study over 27 years.  Int J Public Health. 2013;58(2):247-256. doi:10.1007/s00038-012-0408-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Feinstein  L.  The Relative Economic Importance of Academic, Psychological and Behavioural Attributes Developed on Childhood. London, UK: Centre for Economic Performance, London School of Economics and Political Science; 2000.
9.
Paul  KI, Moser  K.  Unemployment impairs mental health: meta-analyses.  J Vocat Behav. 2009;74(3):264-282. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2009.01.001Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Jin  RL, Shah  CP, Svoboda  TJ.  The impact of unemployment on health: a review of the evidence.  CMAJ. 1995;153(5):529-540.PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Roelfs  DJ, Shor  E, Davidson  KW, Schwartz  JE.  Losing life and livelihood: a systematic review and meta-analysis of unemployment and all-cause mortality.  Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(6):840-854. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Chetty  R, Stepner  M, Abraham  S,  et al.  The association between income and life expectancy in the United States, 2001-2014.  JAMA. 2016;315(16):1750-1766. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.4226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Santiago  CD, Wadsworth  ME, Stump  J.  Socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related stress: prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse low-income families.  J Econ Psychol. 2011;32(2):218-230. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.008Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Heckman  JJ.  Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children.  Science. 2006;312(5782):1900-1902. doi:10.1126/science.1128898PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Kautz  T, Heckman  JJ, Diris  R, ter Weel  B, Borghans  L.  Fostering and Measuring Skills: Improving Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Skills to Promote Lifetime Success. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2014. doi:10.3386/w20749
16.
Campbell  F, Conti  G, Heckman  JJ,  et al.  Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health.  Science. 2014;343(6178):1478-1485. doi:10.1126/science.1248429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fergusson  DM, Horwood  LJ, Ridder  EM.  Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):850-858. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Moffitt  TE, Arseneault  L, Belsky  D,  et al.  A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693-2698. doi:10.1073/pnas.1010076108PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Jones  DE, Greenberg  M, Crowley  M.  Early social-emotional functioning and public health: the relationship between kindergarten social competence and future wellness.  Am J Public Health. 2015;105(11):2283-2290. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2015.302630PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Furnham  A, Cheng  H.  Factors influencing adult earnings: findings from a nationally representative sample.  J Socio-Econ.2013;44(suppl C):120-125. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.008Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Hanushek  EA, Woessmann  L.  The role of cognitive skills in economic development.  J Econ Lit. 2008;46(3):607-668. doi:10.1257/jel.46.3.607Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Schmidt  FL, Hunter  J.  General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment and job performance.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86(1):162-173. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Schmidt  FL. Select on Intelligence. In: Locke  EA, ed.  Handbook of Principles of Organizational Behavior. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2012:1-17.
24.
Heckman  JJ, Stixrud  J, Urzua  S.  The Effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior.  J Labor Econ. 2006;24(3):411-482. doi:10.1086/504455Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Almlund  M, Duckworth  AL, Heckman  JJ, Kautz  TD.  Personality Psychology and Economics. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2011. doi:10.3386/w16822
26.
Heckman  J, Pinto  R, Savelyev  P.  Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes.  Am Econ Rev. 2013;103(6):2052-2086. doi:10.1257/aer.103.6.2052PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Fergusson  DM, Boden  JM, Horwood  LJ.  Childhood self-control and adult outcomes: results from a 30-year longitudinal study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(7):709-7171. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Bound  J, Krueger  AB.  The extent of measurement error in longitudinal earnings data: do two wrongs make a right?  J Labor Econ. 1991;9(1):1-24. doi:10.1086/298256Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Converse  PD, Piccone  KA, Tocci  MC.  Childhood self-control, adolescent behavior, and career success.  Pers Individ Dif. 2014;59(suppl C):65-70. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.007Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Boisjoli  R, Vitaro  F, Lacourse  E, Barker  ED, Tremblay  RE.  Impact and clinical significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys: 15-year follow-up.  Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:415-419. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.030007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Castellanos-Ryan  N, Brière  FN, O’Leary-Barrett  M,  et al; IMAGEN Consortium.  The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125(8):1039-1052. doi:10.1037/abn0000193PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Nagin  D, Tremblay  RE.  Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency.  Child Dev. 1999;70(5):1181-1196. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00086PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Tremblay  RE, Loeber  R, Gagnon  C, Charlebois  P, Larivée  S, LeBlanc  M.  Disruptive boys with stable and unstable high fighting behavior patterns during junior elementary school.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1991;19(3):285-300. doi:10.1007/BF00911232PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Swanson  JM, Arnold  LE, Molina  BSG,  et al; MTA Cooperative Group.  Young adult outcomes in the follow-up of the multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: symptom persistence, source discrepancy, and height suppression.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(6):663-678. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12684PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Murray  AL, Obsuth  I, Eisner  M, Ribeaud  D.  Evaluating longitudinal invariance in dimensions of mental health across adolescence: an analysis of the social behavior questionnaire.  Assessment. 2017;1073191117721741. doi:10.1177/1073191117721741PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Weir  K, Stevenson  J, Graham  P.  Behavioral deviance and teacher ratings of prosocial behavior: preliminary findings.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1980;19(1):68-77. doi:10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60653-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Findlay  LC, Beasley  E, Park  J,  et al.  Longitudinal child data: what can be gained by linking administrative data and cohort data?  Int J Popul Data Sci. 2018;3(21). doi:10.23889/ijpds.v3i1.451Google Scholar
38.
Lorge  I, Thorndike  R.  The Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test. New York, NY: Houghton; 1950.
39.
Mackintosh  NJ.  IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2011.
40.
Blishen  BR, Carroll  WK, Moore  C.  The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada.  Can Rev Sociol. 1987;24(4):465-488. doi:10.1111/j.1755-618X.1987.tb00639.xGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Selya  AS, Rose  JS, Dierker  LC, Hedeker  D, Mermelstein  RJ.  A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED.  Front Psychol. 2012;3:111. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Cohen  J.  Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioral Science. 2nd ed. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; 1988.
43.
Fletcher  J.  The Effects of Childhood ADHD on Adult Labor Market Outcomes. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2013. doi:10.3386/w18689
44.
Loe  IM, Feldman  HM.  Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD.  Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(1)(suppl):82-90. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2006.05.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Pingault  J-B, Tremblay  RE, Vitaro  F,  et al.  Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: a 16-year longitudinal population-based study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(11):1164-1170. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Burke  JD, Rowe  R, Boylan  K.  Functional outcomes of child and adolescent oppositional defiant disorder symptoms in young adult men.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):264-272. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12150PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Mrug  S, Molina  BSG, Hoza  B,  et al.  Peer rejection and friendships in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: contributions to long-term outcomes.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2012;40(6):1013-1026. doi:10.1007/s10802-012-9610-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Asher  S, Coie  J. Peer Rejection in Childhood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1990.
49.
Bierman  K.  Peer Rejection: Developmental Processes and Intervention Strategies. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2004.
50.
Barkley  RA, Murphy  KR.  Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests.  Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(3):157-173. doi:10.1093/arclin/acq014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Clarke  TL.  Executive Functions and Overt/Covert Patterns of Conduct Disorder Symptoms in Children With ADHD [dissertation]. College Park, Maryland: University of Maryland; 2009.
52.
Fletcher  J, Wolfe  B.  Long-term consequences of childhood ADHD on criminal activities.  J Ment Health Policy Econ. 2009;12(3):119-138.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Shaw  M, Hodgkins  P, Caci  H,  et al.  A systematic review and analysis of long-term outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of treatment and non-treatment.  BMC Med. 2012;10:99. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-10-99PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Molina  BSG, Hinshaw  SP, Swanson  JM,  et al; MTA Cooperative Group.  The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(5):484-500. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
The MTA Cooperative Group. Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD.  A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1073-1086. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Diamond  A, Lee  K.  Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old.  Science. 2011;333(6045):959-964. doi:10.1126/science.1204529PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Wyman  PA, Cross  W, Hendricks Brown  C, Yu  Q, Tu  X, Eberly  S.  Intervention to strengthen emotional self-regulation in children with emerging mental health problems: proximal impact on school behavior.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2010;38(5):707-720. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9398-xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Parker  J, Wales  G, Chalhoub  N, Harpin  V.  The long-term outcomes of interventions for the management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials.  Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2013;6:87-99. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S49114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Durlak  JA, Weissberg  RP, Dymnicki  AB, Taylor  RD, Schellinger  KB.  The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: a meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions.  Child Dev. 2011;82(1):405-432. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Durlak  JA, Weissberg  RP, Pachan  M.  A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek to promote personal and social skills in children and adolescents.  Am J Community Psychol. 2010;45(3-4):294-309. doi:10.1007/s10464-010-9300-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×