[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Statistical Maps of Prenatal Drug Exposure Associations With Anatomical Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Measures
Statistical Maps of Prenatal Drug Exposure Associations With Anatomical Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) Measures

A, The scale represents false discovery rate–corrected P values for the statistical significance of the correlations of cumulative prenatal exposure to each drug with measures of local volume at each point on the surface of the brain in statistical models that included newborns separately in each drug-exposed group and their matched unexposed controls. B, Association of cumulative tobacco and alcohol exposure across all newborns in the cohort (N = 118). C, False discovery rate–corrected statistical maps correlating cumulative drug exposure with local volumes in a linear regression model including all newborns in the drug-exposed group but excluding the unexposed controls. ACA indicates anterior cingulate cortex; FG, fusiform gyrus; GR, gyrus rectus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; ITG, inferior temporal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; MOG, middle occipital gyrus; MTG, middle temporal gyrus; OFG, orbitofrontal gyrus; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobule; PrPo, pre- and postcentral gyrus; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; SG, supramarginal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus.

Figure 2.  Statistical Maps of Prenatal Drug Exposure Associations With Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), Relaxometry, and Spectroscopy Measures
Statistical Maps of Prenatal Drug Exposure Associations With Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), Relaxometry, and Spectroscopy Measures

The scale represents false discovery rate–corrected P values for the statistical significance of the correlations of cumulative prenatal exposure to each drug with brain imaging measures at each voxel. The model was applied separately to each exposure group with its matched unexposed controls. Significant correlations of cumulative exposure with each imaging measure were similar in location across exposures. aCR indicates anterior corona radiata; aCS, anterior centrum semiovale; ALIC, anterior limb of the internal capsule; BG, basal ganglia; EC, external capsule; NAA, N-acetyl aspartate; pCR, posterior corona radiata; pCS, posterior centrum semiovale; PLIC, posterior limb of the internal capsule; Th, thalamus.

Figure 3.  Statistical Maps Showing the Correlations of Newborn Age With Imaging Measures
Statistical Maps Showing the Correlations of Newborn Age With Imaging Measures

The color scale represents false discovery rate–corrected P values for the statistical significance of the voxelwise correlations of age with each imaging measure. Sex was included as a covariate. A, Correlation of age with local volumes when brains were not rescaled to a common volume. Nearly all regions correlated positively with age; thus, we inferred that all regions were growing through this period of development (postmenstrual ages of 37-46 weeks). Exceptions included regions that did not vary significantly with age (small portions of the inferior frontal gyri [IFG], superior frontal gyri [SFG], and orbital frontal gyri [OFG]) or that declined with age (parahippocampal gyrus [PHG]). B, Correlation of age with local volumes are shown for brains when rescaled to a common volume. We inferred that regions growing more slowly than overall brain volume included those at the dorsal (pre- and postcentral gyrus [PrPo], SFG), inferior (orbitofrontal gyrus [OFG] and PHG), and medial (SFG) brain surfaces, and that regions growing faster than overall brain volume included those over the lateral surface of the temporal (superior temporal gyrus [STG], middle temporal gyrus, [MTG], and inferior temporal gyrus [ITG]) and parietal (inferior parietal lobe [IPL]) lobes, as well as the medial posterior (posterior cingulate cortex [PCC]) and orbital (gyrus rectus [GR]) surfaces. Note that the prenatal exposure associations in Figure 1 map onto these age correlations, suggesting that the exposures are associated with accelerated growth of regions that normally grow quickly and with slowed growth of regions that normally grow slowly compared with overall brain volume. ADC indicates average diffusion coefficient; FA, fractional anisotropy; NAA, N-acetyl aspartate.

Table 1.  Maternal Demographic Characteristics
Maternal Demographic Characteristics
Table 2.  Newborn Demographic Characteristics
Newborn Demographic Characteristics
Supplement.

eMethods. MRI Pulse Sequences, Image Processing Methods, Post Hoc Statistical Analyses

eResults. Additional Maternal Characteristics

eDiscussion. Neurochemical Action of Exposure, Drugs Relation to Prior Studies, and Additional Limitations

eReferences.

eTable. Infant Outcomes at Postnatal Age 12 Months

eFigure 1. Examples of MPCSI Data Quality, T2 Relaxometry Image Quality, DTI Data Quality: ADC Map, and DTI Data Quality: Fiber Direction Map

eFigure 2. Anatomical MRI: Continuous Exposure Measures, with Rescaling

eFigure 3. Anatomical MRI: Dichotomous Exposure, without Rescaling

eFigure 4. Anatomical MRI: Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures, Without Rescaling

eFigure 5. Anatomical MRI: Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures, with Rescaling

eFigure 6. Anatomical MRI: Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Dichotomous Exposure, Without Rescaling

eFigure 7. Anatomical MRI: Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Dichotomous Exposure, with Rescaling

eFigure 8. Dichotomous Exposure and Dose-Response Association: Fractional Anisotropy

eFigure 9. Dichotomous Exposure and Dose-Response Association: Average Diffusion Coefficient

eFigure 10. Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures: Fractional Anisotropy

eFigure 11. Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures: Average Diffusion Coefficient

eFigure 12. Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures: NAA

eFigure 13. Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures: Choline

eFigure 14. Modeling of Co-Occurring Exposures, Continuous Exposure Measures: Creatine

eFigure 15. Mediation Model

eFigure 16. Anatomical MRI Mediation: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scores

eFigure 17. Anatomical MRI Mediation: Vineland Communication Scores

eFigure 18. Anatomical MRI Mediation: Bayley Gross Motor

eFigure 19. DTI Mediation: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scores

eFigure 20. DTI Mediation: Vineland Living Skill Scores

eFigure 21. DTI Mediation: Bayley Receptive Communication Scores

eFigure 22. Scatterplots for Exposure Associations in Matched Groups: Anatomical MRI

eFigure 23. Scatterplots for Exposure Associations in Matched Groups: DTI

eFigure 24. Scatterplots for Exposure Associations in Matched Groups: Relaxometry & MRSI

eFigure 25. Scatterplots for Dose-Response Associations: Anatomical MRI

eFigure 26. Scatterplots for Inter-Modality Correlations

1.
Vesga-López  O, Blanco  C, Keyes  K, Olfson  M, Grant  BF, Hasin  DS.  Psychiatric disorders in pregnant and postpartum women in the United States.   Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(7):805-815. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.7.805 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Forray  A.  Substance use during pregnancy.   F1000Res. 2016;5:F1000 Faculty Rev-887. Published online May 13, 2016. doi:10.12688/f1000research.7645.1 PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
US Department of Health and Human Services. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2015 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: detailed tables. Published September 8, 2016. Accessed April 28, 2020. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/results-2015-national-survey-drug-use-and-health-detailed-tables
4.
Brown  QL, Sarvet  AL, Shmulewitz  D, Martins  SS, Wall  MM, Hasin  DS.  Trends in marijuana use among pregnant and nonpregnant reproductive-aged women, 2002-2014.   JAMA. 2017;317(2):207-209. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.17383 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Young-Wolff  KC, Tucker  L-Y, Alexeeff  S,  et al.  Trends in self-reported and biochemically tested marijuana use among pregnant females in California from 2009-2016.   JAMA. 2017;318(24):2490-2491. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.17225 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Volkow  ND, Han  B, Compton  WM, McCance-Katz  EF.  Self-reported medical and nonmedical cannabis use among pregnant women in the United States.   JAMA. 2019;322(2):167-169. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.7982 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Desai  RJ, Hernandez-Diaz  S, Bateman  BT, Huybrechts  KF.  Increase in prescription opioid use during pregnancy among Medicaid-enrolled women.   Obstet Gynecol. 2014;123(5):997-1002. doi:10.1097/AOG.0000000000000208 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Martin  CE, Longinaker  N, Terplan  M.  Recent trends in treatment admissions for prescription opioid abuse during pregnancy.   J Subst Abuse Treat. 2015;48(1):37-42. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.07.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Patrick  SW, Davis  MM, Lehmann  CU, Cooper  WO.  Increasing incidence and geographic distribution of neonatal abstinence syndrome: United States 2009 to 2012  [published correction in J Perinatol. 2015;35(8):667].  J Perinatol. 2015;35(8):650-655. doi:10.1038/jp.2015.36 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Cain  MA, Bornick  P, Whiteman  V.  The maternal, fetal, and neonatal effects of cocaine exposure in pregnancy.   Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2013;56(1):124-132. doi:10.1097/GRF.0b013e31827ae167 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Bigsby  R, LaGasse  LL, Lester  B,  et al.  Prenatal cocaine exposure and motor performance at 4 months.   Am J Occup Ther. 2011;65(5):e60-e68. doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.001263 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gouin  K, Murphy  K, Shah  PS; Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Low Birth Weight and Preterm Births.  Effects of cocaine use during pregnancy on low birthweight and preterm birth: systematic review and metaanalyses.   Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;204(4):340.e1-340.e12. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2010.11.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Mayes  LC, Cicchetti  D, Acharyya  S, Zhang  H.  Developmental trajectories of cocaine-and-other-drug-exposed and non–cocaine-exposed children.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24(5):323-335. doi:10.1097/00004703-200310000-00003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Lester  BM, Tronick  EZ, LaGasse  L,  et al.  The maternal lifestyle study: effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants.   Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):1182-1192. doi:10.1542/peds.110.6.1182 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Willford  J.  The effects of prenatal cocaine use on infant development.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2008;30(2):96-106. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2007.12.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Singer  LT, Arendt  R, Minnes  S, Farkas  K, Salvator  A.  Neurobehavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(5):653-666. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(00)00092-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Tronick  EZ, Messinger  DS, Weinberg  MK,  et al.  Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants’ and mothers’ social-emotional behavior and dyadic features of their interaction in the face-to-face still-face paradigm.   Dev Psychol. 2005;41(5):711-722. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.711 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Larkby  C, Day  NL.  Effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior and growth at 10 years of age.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;40:1-8. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2013.08.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Minnes  S, Robin  NH, Alt  AA,  et al.  Dysmorphic and anthropometric outcomes in 6-year-old prenatally cocaine-exposed children.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2006;28(1):28-38. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2005.09.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Covington  CY, Nordstrom-Klee  B, Ager  J, Sokol  R, Delaney-Black  V.  Birth to age 7 growth of children prenatally exposed to drugs: a prospective cohort study.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24(4):489-496. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00233-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Bandstra  ES, Morrow  CE, Accornero  VH, Mansoor  E, Xue  L, Anthony  JC.  Estimated effects of in utero cocaine exposure on language development through early adolescence.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;33(1):25-35. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.07.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Lewis  BA, Kirchner  HL, Short  EJ,  et al.  Prenatal cocaine and tobacco effects on children’s language trajectories.   Pediatrics. 2007;120(1):e78-e85. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-2563 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Lewis  BA, Minnes  S, Short  EJ,  et al.  The effects of prenatal cocaine on language development at 10 years of age.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;33(1):17-24. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Minnes  S, Min  MO, Short  EJ,  et al.  Executive function in children with prenatal cocaine exposure (12-15 years).   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2016;57:79-86. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2016.07.002 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Lambert  BL, Bauer  CR.  Developmental and behavioral consequences of prenatal cocaine exposure: a review.   J Perinatol. 2012;32(11):819-828. doi:10.1038/jp.2012.90 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Ackerman  JP, Riggins  T, Black  MM.  A review of the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure among school-aged children.   Pediatrics. 2010;125(3):554-565. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0637 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Buckingham-Howes  S, Berger  SS, Scaletti  LA, Black  MM.  Systematic review of prenatal cocaine exposure and adolescent development.   Pediatrics. 2013;131(6):e1917-e1936. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-0945 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
El Marroun  H, Tiemeier  H, Steegers  EAP,  et al.  Intrauterine cannabis exposure affects fetal growth trajectories: the Generation R Study.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(12):1173-1181. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181bfa8ee PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Fried  PA, Watkinson  B, Gray  R.  A follow-up study of attentional behavior in 6-year-old children exposed prenatally to marihuana, cigarettes, and alcohol.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1992;14(5):299-311. doi:10.1016/0892-0362(92)90036-A PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Leech  SL, Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Day  NL.  Prenatal substance exposure: effects on attention and impulsivity of 6-year-olds.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1999;21(2):109-118. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(98)00042-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Goldschmidt  L, Richardson  GA, Willford  J, Day  NL.  Prenatal marijuana exposure and intelligence test performance at age 6.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(3):254-263. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318160b3f0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Goldschmidt  L, Day  NL, Richardson  GA.  Effects of prenatal marijuana exposure on child behavior problems at age 10.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(3):325-336. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(00)00066-0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Fried  PA, Watkinson  B.  Visuoperceptual functioning differs in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(1):11-20. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(99)00046-X PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Guo  X, Spencer  JW, Suess  PE, Hickey  JE, Better  WE, Herning  RI.  Cognitive brain potential alterations in boys exposed to opiates: in utero and lifestyle comparisons.   Addict Behav. 1994;19(4):429-441. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)90065-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Hunt  RW, Tzioumi  D, Collins  E, Jeffery  HE.  Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero.   Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(1):29-35. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.01.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Hickey  JE, Suess  PE, Newlin  DB, Spurgeon  L, Porges  SW.  Vagal tone regulation during sustained attention in boys exposed to opiates in utero.   Addict Behav. 1995;20(1):43-59. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)00044-Y PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ornoy  A, Michailevskaya  V, Lukashov  I, Bar-Hamburger  R, Harel  S.  The developmental outcome of children born to heroin-dependent mothers, raised at home or adopted.   Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20(5):385-396. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(96)00014-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ornoy  A, Segal  J, Bar-Hamburger  R, Greenbaum  C.  Developmental outcome of school-age children born to mothers with heroin dependency: importance of environmental factors.   Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43(10):668-675. doi:10.1017/S0012162201001219 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Ross  EJ, Graham  DL, Money  KM, Stanwood  GD.  Developmental consequences of fetal exposure to drugs: what we know and what we still must learn.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(1):61-87. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.147 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Behnke  M, Smith  VC; Committee on Substance Abuse; Committee on Fetus and Newborn.  Prenatal substance abuse: short- and long-term effects on the exposed fetus.   Pediatrics. 2013;131(3):e1009-e1024. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-3931 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
McLellan  AT, Luborsky  L, Cacciola  J,  et al.  New data from the Addiction Severity Index: reliability and validity in three centers.   J Nerv Ment Dis. 1985;173(7):412-423. doi:10.1097/00005053-198507000-00005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
McLellan  AT, Kushner  H, Metzger  D,  et al. The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1992;9(3):199-213. doi:10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-s
43.
Zanis  DA, McLellan  AT, Cnaan  RA, Randall  M.  Reliability and validity of the Addiction Severity Index with a homeless sample.   J Subst Abuse Treat. 1994;11(6):541-548. doi:10.1016/0740-5472(94)90005-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Duyn  JH, Gillen  J, Sobering  G, van Zijl  PC, Moonen  CT.  Multisection proton MR spectroscopic imaging of the brain.   Radiology. 1993;188(1):277-282. doi:10.1148/radiology.188.1.8511313 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
First  MB, Spitzer  RL, Gibbon  M, Williams  JBW.  Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Sisorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002.
46.
Hollingshead  AB.  Four-Factor Index of Social Status. Yale University Press; 1975.
47.
Hamilton  M.  Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.   Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6(4):278-296. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Hamilton  M.  The assessment of anxiety states by rating.   Br J Med Psychol. 1959;32(1):50-55. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Wechsler  D.  Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed. Pearson; 2003.
50.
Bayley  N.  Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. 3rd ed. Harcourt Assessment; 2006.
51.
Sparrow  SS, Balla  DA, Cicchetti  DV, Doll EA.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Survey Form Manual. American Guidance Service; 1984.
52.
Reichle  CW, Smith  GM, Gravenstein  JS, Macris  SG, Beecher  HK.  Comparative analgesic potency of heroin and morphine in postoperative patients.   J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1962;136:43-46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Manfredonia  JF.  Prescribing methadone for pain management in end-of-life care.   J Am Osteopath Assoc. 2005;105(3)(suppl 1):S18-S21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Johnson  NL.  Systems of frequency curves generated by methods of translation.   Biometrika. 1949;36(pt 1-2):149-176. doi:10.1093/biomet/36.1-2.149 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Burbidge  JB, Magee  L, Robb  AL.  Alternative transformations to handle extreme values of the dependent variable.   J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(401):123-127. doi:10.1080/01621459.1988.10478575 Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Benjamini  Y, Yekutieli  D.  The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency.   Ann Stat. 2001;29(4):1165-1188.Google ScholarCrossref
57.
MacKinnon  DP, Krull  JL, Lockwood  CM.  Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect.   Prev Sci. 2000;1(4):173-181. doi:10.1023/A:1026595011371 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Zhao  X, Lynch  J, Chen  Q.  Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis.   J Consum Res. 2010;37(2):197-206. doi:10.1086/651257 Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Hermoye  L, Saint-Martin  C, Cosnard  G,  et al.  Pediatric diffusion tensor imaging: normal database and observation of the white matter maturation in early childhood.   Neuroimage. 2006;29(2):493-504. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Dubois  J, Dehaene-Lambertz  G, Kulikova  S, Poupon  C, Hüppi  PS, Hertz-Pannier  L.  The early development of brain white matter: a review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants.   Neuroscience. 2014;276:48-71. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.044 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ouyang  M, Dubois  J, Yu  Q, Mukherjee  P, Huang  H.  Delineation of early brain development from fetuses to infants with diffusion MRI and beyond.   Neuroimage. 2019;185:836-850. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Concha  L.  A macroscopic view of microstructure: using diffusion-weighted images to infer damage, repair, and plasticity of white matter.   Neuroscience. 2014;276:14-28. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Ding  X-Q, Kucinski  T, Wittkugel  O,  et al.  Normal brain maturation characterized with age-related T2 relaxation times: an attempt to develop a quantitative imaging measure for clinical use.   Invest Radiol. 2004;39(12):740-746. doi:10.1097/00004424-200412000-00005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Leppert  IR, Almli  CR, McKinstry  RC,  et al; Brain Development Cooperative Group.  T(2) relaxometry of normal pediatric brain development.   J Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;29(2):258-267. doi:10.1002/jmri.21646 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Haynes  RL, Borenstein  NS, Desilva  TM,  et al.  Axonal development in the cerebral white matter of the human fetus and infant.   J Comp Neurol. 2005;484(2):156-167. doi:10.1002/cne.20453 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Tau  GZ, Peterson  BS.  Normal development of brain circuits.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):147-168. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.115 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Blüml  S, Wisnowski  JL, Nelson  MD  Jr,  et al.  Metabolic maturation of the human brain from birth through adolescence: insights from in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy.   Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(12):2944-2955. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs283 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Tomiyasu  M, Aida  N, Endo  M,  et al.  Neonatal brain metabolite concentrations: an in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy study with a clinical MR system at 3 Tesla.   PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e82746. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082746 PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Rae  CD.  A guide to the metabolic pathways and function of metabolites observed in human brain 1H magnetic resonance spectra.   Neurochem Res. 2014;39(1):1-36. doi:10.1007/s11064-013-1199-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Clark  JB.  N-acetyl aspartate: a marker for neuronal loss or mitochondrial dysfunction.   Dev Neurosci. 1998;20(4-5):271-276. doi:10.1159/000017321 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Šlamberová  R.  Drugs in pregnancy: the effects on mother and her progeny.   Physiol Res. 2012;61(suppl 1):S123-S135.PubMedGoogle Scholar
72.
Barres  BA, Raff  MC.  Proliferation of oligodendrocyte precursor cells depends on electrical activity in axons.   Nature. 1993;361(6409):258-260. doi:10.1038/361258a0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Demerens  C, Stankoff  B, Logak  M,  et al.  Induction of myelination in the central nervous system by electrical activity.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(18):9887-9892. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.18.9887 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Gibson  EM, Purger  D, Mount  CW,  et al.  Neuronal activity promotes oligodendrogenesis and adaptive myelination in the mammalian brain.   Science. 2014;344(6183):1252304. doi:10.1126/science.1252304 PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Almeida  RG, Lyons  DA.  On myelinated axon plasticity and neuronal circuit formation and function.   J Neurosci. 2017;37(42):10023-10034. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3185-16.2017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
June 15, 2020

Associations of Maternal Prenatal Drug Abuse With Measures of Newborn Brain Structure, Tissue Organization, and Metabolite Concentrations

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Pediatrics, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles
  • 3Department of Pediatrics, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, New York
  • 4Department of Psychology, Marist College, Poughkeepsie, New York
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Columbia University Medical Center, New York
  • 6Department of Radiology and Physiology, Albert Einstein School of Medicine, Bronx, New York
  • 7Department of Biophysics, Albert Einstein School of Medicine, Bronx, New York
  • 8Department of Pediatrics, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles
JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(9):831-842. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1622
Key Points

Question  What are the associations of prenatal illicit drug exposure to marijuana, cocaine, and methadone and/or heroin with measures of brain organization in newborns?

Findings  In this cohort study of 118 economically disadvantaged mothers and their newborns, prenatal exposure to marijuana, cocaine, or opioids was associated with measures of brain structure, tissue organization, and metabolite concentrations; the associations were similar to those of advancing age with brain measures in unexposed newborns.

Meaning  Prenatal drug exposure to marijuana, cocaine, and opioids is associated with measures of newborn brain tissue in patterns that suggest an exaggeration of normal fetal brain maturation.

Abstract

Importance  Increasing rates of illicit drug use during pregnancy may be associated with risk for long-term health problems in prenatally exposed children.

Objective  To identify the associations of prenatal exposure to illicit drugs with organization of the newborn brain.

Design, Setting, and Participants  For this cohort study, a volunteer sample of 210 illicit drug–using and nonusing mothers and their newborns was enrolled from prenatal clinics and drug abuse treatment programs in New York, New York. Enrollment, scanning, and long-term follow-up occurred from September 2004 through February 2012, and image processing and statistical analyses continued through fall 2018. In addition to 26 participants with incomplete data, a total of 64 mothers were lost to follow-up during pregnancy, and 13 newborns were lost to follow-up at birth because of perinatal complications.

Exposures  Newborns were assigned to 1 of 4 primary exposure groups based on the history of most frequent maternal drug use: marijuana, cocaine, methadone maintenance, and/or heroin. Unexposed newborns were controls.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Unsedated magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of newborn brains was performed shortly after birth. Infant neurodevelopmental outcomes were assessed at age 12 months. MRI modalities included anatomical imaging, diffusion tensor imaging, T2 relaxometry, and magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Infant neurodevelopmental outcomes included Bayley scales of infant development–III and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Statistical analyses were performed with results represented on the brain images.

Results  Of 118 mothers, 42 (35%) were in the control group (mean [SD] age, 25.9 [6.1] years), 29 (25%) were in the cocaine group (mean [SD] age, 29.0 [6.1] years), 29 (25%) were in the marijuana group (mean [SD] age, 24.3 [5.5] years), and 18 (15%) were in the methadone and/or heroin group (mean [SD] age, 30.9 [5.7] years). Not all newborns could be scanned successfully; therefore, usable MRIs were acquired for 118 newborns from predominantly minority groups and with economically disadvantaged mothers. Anatomic abnormalities were detected in similar locations across all 3 drug exposures and included smaller volumes in the dorsal, medial, and ventral surfaces of the frontal lobe and dose-related increases in volumes in the lateral temporal lobe, dorsal parietal lobe, and superior frontal gyrus. Dose-related increases in diffusion tensor measures of tissue organization, decreases in T2 relaxometry times, and increases in spectroscopy metabolite concentrations were similar across exposures. These associations of exposures with brain measures were similar to the associations of newborn age with brain measures. The anatomic and diffusion tensor imaging measures suppressively mediated the associations of prenatal exposure with poorer 12-month infant outcomes.

Conclusions and Relevance  The findings suggest that prenatal drug exposure is associated with measures of newborn brain tissue in patterns that may indicate that exposures accelerated normal fetal brain maturation, which in turn mediated the associations with poorer 12-month infant outcomes.

Introduction

Substance use during pregnancy has become commonplace and may be associated with risk for long-term cognitive, behavioral, and emotional health problems in prenatally exposed children. More than 60% of pregnant women report using illegal substances, and 15% have a substance use disorder1,2; these women are often younger and are experiencing material and social deprivation.3 Owing partially to its decriminalization, marijuana use during pregnancy has increased nearly 7-fold in the past decade.4-6 The US opioid crisis has been associated with a concomitant epidemic of use among pregnant women and with withdrawal syndromes in their prenatally exposed newborns.7-9 Cocaine use remains relatively stable, with approximately 750 000 pregnant US women using this drug annually.10

Long-term outcome studies face the challenge of disentangling the effects of prenatal illicit drug exposure from those of co-occurring substance use, poverty, maternal stress, and poor prenatal care. Nevertheless, consensus is emerging that prenatal cocaine exposure is associated with preterm birth,11-13 sustained newborn jitteriness,11,13-17 slowed fetal and postnatal somatic growth,18-20 delayed language development,21-23 impaired self-regulation,24 and poor sustained attention.25-27 Prenatal marijuana exposure slows somatic growth28; impairs memory, verbal reasoning, and attention; and increases impulsivity, hyperactivity, and oppositional behaviors.29-33 Prenatal methadone exposure produces motor and cognitive impairments,34,35 inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity.36-38 Prenatal alcohol and tobacco exposure can lead to similar problems in childhood.39 Thus, although differing exposures are associated with unique developmental problems, they have in common an association with the development of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in later childhood.39,40

This study evaluated the associations of prenatal exposure to marijuana, opioids, or cocaine with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures in the neonatal brain as soon after exposure as possible. Given the common associations that prenatal cocaine, marijuana, and opioid exposure have with reducing brain growth and increasing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder–like symptoms, we hypothesized that we would detect exposure-associated reductions in brain volumes in anatomic MRI scans localized to the frontal lobe and other higher-order association areas. Other MRI modalities were acquired for post hoc analyses to help identify the likely cellular bases for volume reductions.

Methods

For this cohort study, illicit drug–using pregnant women were recruited from prenatal clinics and drug abuse treatment programs in Manhattan, New York City. Healthy pregnant women were recruited from prenatal clinics at New York Presbyterian Hospital, New York, New York. All women provided informed written consent for participation. Procedures were approved by the institutional review board of the New York State Psychiatric Institute; participants were protected by a certificate of confidentiality. Mothers were paid for participation in gift cards for newborn supplies. Statistical analyses were performed with results represented on the brain images.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Inclusion criteria consisted of (1) maternal age of 18 to 45 years; (2) maternal use of marijuana, cocaine, or methadone and/or heroin for the exposed groups or no history of illicit drug or alcohol use during pregnancy for the control group; and (3) newborn gestational age at birth of at least 37 weeks.

Exclusion criteria consisted of (1) maternal HIV infection or AIDS, seizure disorder, malignant tumors, diabetes, or use of psychoactive medications; (2) infant congenital anomalies, genetic syndromes, or significant neonatal complications; (3) maternal alcohol use of more than 2 ounces per day for the exposed group or more than 1 ounce per day in any trimester for the control group; and (4) urine toxicology or self-reports indicating use of other illicit substances.

Illicit drug use history was obtained using drug use questions from the Addiction Severity Index41-43 administered at enrollment, during each subsequent trimester, and at birth. Drug use was confirmed with random urine toxicology screens for the mother during pregnancy and for the newborn at delivery and through maternal and medical record reviews. Exposed newborns were divided into 3 groups based on most frequent maternal drug use: marijuana, cocaine, or methadone maintenance therapy for narcotic addiction with or without concurrent use of heroin (methadone and/or heroin). Every effort was made to recruit mothers who were using only a single drug.

Sample

We enrolled 210 mother and newborn pairs: controls (n = 63), cocaine users (n = 53), marijuana users (n = 55), and methadone and/or heroin users (n = 39). During pregnancy, 64 of 210 (30.5%) were lost to follow-up because of disinterest, maternal incarceration, changes in residence, or spontaneous abortions. At birth, 13 of the remaining 146 newborns (8.9%) were lost to follow-up owing to prematurity (methadone and/or heroin [n = 2], cocaine [n = 1]), major congenital heart or pulmonary disease (cocaine [n = 1], methadone and/or heroin [n = 1], and marijuana [n = 1]), abnormal anatomic scan findings reflecting infarction (cocaine [n = 1], marijuana [n = 1]), or foster care (cocaine [n = 2], methadone and/or heroin [n = 2], and marijuana [n = 1]).

We acquired anatomical MRIs to study brain structure, diffusion tensor imaging and T2 time relaxometry to study brain tissue organization, and magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging to study chemical metabolites. MRIs were obtained for 133 unsedated newborns at 37 to 46 weeks’ postmenstrual age.

Usable MRI data in at least 1 modality were obtained in 118 newborns: marijuana (n = 29), cocaine (n = 29), methadone and/or heroin (n = 18), and unexposed controls (n = 42). Not all MRI modalities could be acquired for every newborn, leaving a subset with usable MRIs in each modality. We group matched each drug-exposed group to a randomly sampled subset of the unexposed newborns, with the constraint that groups did not differ significantly by sex or postmenstrual age at the time of MRI, yielding the following: (1) anatomic MRI: marijuana (n = 23) and controls (n = 37), cocaine (n = 25) and controls (n = 37), methadone and/or heroin (n = 17) and controls (n = 33); (2) diffusion tensor imaging: marijuana (n = 20) and controls (n = 26), cocaine (n = 16) and controls (n = 21), methadone and/or heroin (n = 13) and controls (n = 20); (3) T2 relaxometry: cocaine (n = 17) and controls (n = 20), marijuana (n = 14) and controls (n = 18), methadone and/or heroin (n = 11) and controls (n = 18); and (4) 1H-multi-planar chemical shift imaging44: marijuana (n = 22) and controls (n = 29), cocaine (n = 18) and controls (n = 29), and methadone and/or heroin (n = 13) and controls (n = 29). Enrollment, scanning, and long-term follow-up occurred from September 2004 through February 2012, and image processing and statistical analyses continued through fall 2018.

Assessments

English- or Spanish-language interviews were completed to collect maternal demographics, psychiatric history,45 socioeconomic status,46 depression severity,47 and anxiety severity.48 Maternal IQ was assessed prenatally.49 Newborn assessments at age 12 months included Bayley Scales of Infant Development–III50 and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales.51 Race/ethnicity was self-reported. MRI, pulse sequences, and image processing procedures are detailed in the eMethods in the Supplement.

Statistical Analyses

We applied multiple linear regression at each voxel in template space within each MRI modality to test our a priori hypothesis that prenatal drug exposure would be associated with alterations in the newborn brain. The general model for testing exposure correlates in each modality was

Imaging Measure = β0 + (β1 × Drug Exposure) + (β2 × Alcohol) + (β3 × Tobacco) + (β4 × Postmenstrual Age) + (β5 × Sex) + ϵ.

The dependent measure for anatomical analyses was the signed euclidean distance at each point on the surface of each newborn brain from the corresponding point on the surface of the template, which we designate for simplicity here as a measure of local volume. For diffusion tensor imaging, the dependent measure was either fractional anisotropy (FA) or average diffusion coefficient (ADC), and for relaxometry, it was T2 relaxation time; for magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging, it was N-acetylaspartate (NAA) concentration. The independent variable (drug exposure) was prenatal exposure to a particular drug in the specific exposure group and its matched unexposed controls, with exposure coded as a continuous variable representing cumulative exposure throughout pregnancy based on maternal reports of daily use, converted to either grams of cocaine, joints of marijuana, grams of methadone, cigarette packs, or ounces of alcohol (Table 1).52,53 Cumulative exposure distributions were highly skewed, with most values being small but some large. Preliminary analyses showed that drug exposure had logarithmic rather than linear associations with brain measures. We considered various log transformations, but the one that could best address 0 values was the inverse hyperbolic sine (IHS)54,55:

Image description not available.

where the transformed value approaches the log of twice the exposure for large values and is 0 for no exposure. Covariates in all analyses were newborn postmenstrual age at MRI, newborn sex, and cumulative maternal tobacco and alcohol use during pregnancy. We also assessed the effects of including as additional covariates in our models maternal age, socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, depression severity, anxiety severity, or prenatal stress.

P values in each statistical map within each imaging modality were corrected for the number of statistical comparisons. We applied the Benjamini–Yekutieli procedure56 for false discovery rate to control for the expected number of false-positive findings. False discovery rate–corrected, 2-sided P values were color coded and displayed on the template brain.

We assessed dose-response associations by correlating cumulative drug use with imaging measures within each exposure group excluding the unexposed controls and covarying for postmenopausal age and sex.

Testing brain-based mediation of the associations of exposures with 12-month infant outcomes was performed voxelwise using the Sobel test (eFigure 15 in the Supplement), and we assessed whether mediation was complementary or suppressive (eMethods and eFigure 15 in the Supplement).57,58

Results

Of 118 mothers whose newborns had usable MRI data, 42 (35%) were in the control group (mean [SD] age, 25.9 [6.1] years), 29 (25%) were in the cocaine group (29.0 [6.1] years), 29 (25%) were in the marijuana group (24.3 [5.5] years), and 18 (15%) were in the methadone and/or heroin group (30.9 [5.7] years). The 133 mothers with newborns who underwent MRI did not differ significantly in demographics from the 77 enrolled mothers with newborns who did not undergo MRI (eResults in the Supplement). Among the 118 mothers of newborns with usable MRI scans, polydrug use was present in 4 of 29 (13.8%) in the cocaine group, 1 of 29 (3.5%) in the marijuana group, and 6 of 18 (33.3%) in the methadone and/or heroin group (Table 1). By design, reported alcohol use was minimal in each exposure group for the duration of pregnancy. The number of women who smoked more than half a pack per day of cigarettes was 6 (20.7%) in the cocaine group, 2 (11.1%) in the marijuana group, and 6 (33.3%) in the methadone and/or heroin group (Table 1). Additional maternal and newborn characteristics are shown in eFigure 12 and the eResults in the Supplement.

Anatomical MRI

MRI data quality is shown in eFigure 1 in the Supplement. Compared with their matched, unexposed controls, all 3 exposure groups had dose-related volume reductions in the same general locations, including dorsal and lateral surfaces of the frontal lobe and mesial and inferior cerebral surfaces. The cocaine and marijuana groups also had smaller volumes in most of the lateral surface of the temporal lobe, extending to the inferolateral occipital surface (Figure 1). Reduced volumes associated with co-occurring alcohol use were present in similar locations although smaller in spatial extent (Figure 1).

Prenatal exposure was also associated with regional enlargement, particularly of the lateral temporal and inferolateral frontal lobes and mesial wall in the methadone and/or heroin group, with smaller foci in similar locations in the cocaine group (Figure 1).

Findings were similar when assessing exposure correlates for all newborns entered into a single exposure model and when treating individual drug exposures as dichotomous variables for any exposure during pregnancy (eFigures 3-7 in the Supplement). Regional volume reductions exhibited linear associations with IHS-transformed measures of cumulative exposure during pregnancy within each exposure group (eFigure 22 in the Supplement).

Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Prenatal cocaine, marijuana, and methadone and/or heroin exposure were all significantly associated with increased FA (Figure 2) and reduced ADC (Figure 2) throughout frontal and parietal white matter. Marijuana was additionally associated with increased FA in the anterior limb of the internal capsule and marijuana and cocaine with reduced FA in the posterior limb of the internal capsule. Scattered significant voxels at the interfaces of gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid may represent partial volume effects, but large clusters in regions in deep white matter distant from tissue boundaries likely represent findings in only white matter.

Relaxometry and Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopic Imaging

All 3 exposures were associated with reduced T2 relaxation times in frontal and parietal white matter; cocaine and methadone and/or heroin were associated with increased T2 times in ventral striatum and thalamus (Figure 2). Cocaine and marijuana exposure but not methadone and/or heroin were associated with increased NAA in deep white matter of the frontal and parietal lobes (Figure 2). Post hoc analyses showed that choline and creatine concentrations exhibited similar associations as NAA (eFigures 12-14 in the Supplement).

Age Correlates

Advancing newborn age was associated with smaller volumes of frontal, posterior, and inferior brain surfaces when controlling for overall brain size, suggesting slower growth compared with overall brain growth. Newborn age was also associated with widespread increases in FA and reductions in ADC and T2 times throughout the brain, and with increased NAA concentrations in basal ganglia and thalamus (Figure 3).

Influences of Potential Confounders

Findings were similar when maternal age, socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, depression severity, anxiety severity, or prenatal stress were included as additional covariates in our statistical models.

Brain-Based Mediation

Twelve-month Bayley and Vineland scores were lower for exposed infants (eTable in the Supplement). Exposure-associated reductions in dorsal, inferior, and medial frontal volumes partially mediated the associations of any prenatal drug exposure with 12-month Vineland measures of adaptive behavior (eFigure 16 in the Supplement) and communication (eFigure 17 in the Supplement), and reduced volumes in premotor regions partially mediated exposure associations with lower Bayley gross motor scores (eFigure 18 in the Supplement). In all regions, mediation was suppressive rather than complementary (eFigures 16-18 in the Supplement).57,58 Regional enlargements did not significantly mediate outcomes. Lower mean diffusivity measures partially mediated exposure associations with lower scores for Vineland adaptive behavior and living skills and Bayley receptive communication (eFigures 19-21 in the Supplement).

Intermodality Correlations

We assessed the intercorrelations of imaging measures across MRI modalities for all available newborns in regions exhibiting a commonality of findings across exposures. Scatterplots for imaging measures adjusted for newborn postmenstrual age and sex showed significant linear correlations between measures in all MRI modalities (eFigure 26 in the Supplement).

Discussion

In this study, we detected significant associations of prenatal illicit drug exposure with measures of brain structure, tissue organization, and metabolites in newborns. Overall head circumference and brain volume were smaller in drug-exposed newborns (significantly in the methadone and/or heroin group) when controlling for length as a scaling effect and postmenstrual age as a maturational index (Table 2) and thus exceeded the anticipated association between illicit drug use and slowing of overall somatic growth.18-20,28,39 Smaller overall brain volumes derived from smaller regions in similar locations across all 3 exposures, including dorsal, medial, and ventral surfaces of the frontal lobe (Figure 1). These findings were present when comparing newborns in individual exposure groups with unexposed controls, when controlling for co-occurring exposures in a single exposure model that included all newborns, and when treating individual drug exposures as either dichotomous variables (eFigures 6-9 in the Supplement) or continuous measures of cumulative exposure (eFigures 2-7 and 10-14 in the Supplement). Each exposure was also associated with regional enlargements (lateral temporal lobe, dorsal parietal lobe, and superior frontal gyrus) in direct proportion to cumulative exposure measures and in similar locations across exposures, reaching statistical significance for cocaine and methadone and/or heroin (Figure 1), especially when adjusting for overall brain volume (eFigures 2, 5, and 7 in the Supplement).

These common associations of prenatal exposures with brain structure are similar in direction and location to the overall correlates of advancing age on brain structure, which included slower growth of frontal, posterior, and inferior brain surfaces and more rapid growth of lateral temporal surfaces compared with overall brain growth (Figure 3). Prenatal drug exposure therefore seemed to be associated with slowed brain growth disproportionately in regions that grow more slowly and with accelerated growth in regions that grow more quickly. Accelerated growth in lateral temporal cortices was associated with cumulative exposure (eFigure 25 in the Supplement). Because most drug exposure occurred in the first trimester, however, dose-related acceleration in growth could instead represent a timing-specific consequence of exposure early in gestation. Regions of exaggerated slowing of growth were not associated with cumulative dose and may be independent of both exposure timing and dose.

Developmental interpretations of our anatomical findings are supported by findings in each of the other modalities. Advancing age was associated with widespread increases in FA and reductions in ADC and T2 times in our data and previous studies59-61 and in our findings of exposure-related increases in NAA concentrations in subcortical white and gray matter (Figure 2). All 3 exposures were associated, to varying degrees, with abnormalities in these same regions and in the same direction, supporting interpretation of the anatomic findings that exposures may be associated with accelerated fetal brain maturation. Each exposure, for example, was associated with increased FA and reduced ADC (Figure 2), indicating a dose-related greater restriction on the diffusion of water perpendicular to the direction of axons within white matter, most likely from more myelin or greater axon density62 throughout frontal and parietal white matter and in proportion to cumulative exposure (eFigure 23 in the Supplement). The dose-related decrease in T2 relaxation times in these same regions for each exposure (Figure 2) was also consistent with more myelin or greater cell density because both reduce brain free water content and thereby reduce T2 times with advancing age in early postnatal life.60,63,64 Finally, previous studies have reported that NAA concentrations and axon density increase rapidly in white matter after birth.65-68 In our data, higher NAA concentrations were associated with newborn age and with prenatal cocaine and marijuana exposure in large white matter fiber bundles and basal ganglia (Figure 2 and eFigure 12 in the Supplement), suggesting that increased density or metabolism of neurons69,70 accompanies advancing newborn age and higher exposures.

Regions of slow anatomic maturation suppressively mediated lower infant 12-month Vineland social communication and adaptive behavior scores (eFigures 16 and 17 in the Supplement). Significant suppressive mediation indicates that slowed regional maturation was likely an adaptive response to prenatal illicit drug exposure, attenuating what would otherwise have been worse 12-month outcomes.57,58 We speculate that slowed maturation in dorsal and inferior frontal brain regions is a response to the deleterious consequences of illicit drug exposure elsewhere in the brain.

Although the specific biochemical consequences of each drug would indicate specific brain effects (eDiscussion in the Supplement),39,71 our findings instead suggest mostly a commonality of drug effects, presumably operating through a final common pathway. The commonality of drug effects is underscored by similar findings across exposures in each MRI modality and by the significant correlations of imaging measures across modalities (eFigure 25 in the Supplement) showing that newborns who had the largest exposure correlates in one modality also had the largest exposure correlates in another modality. Preclinical studies (eDiscussion in the Supplement) suggest that the reductions in neuronal growth, differentiation, dendritic complexity, synaptic density, and neurotransmitter signaling that all 3 illicit drugs produce may be associated with slowed development in regions where growth is normally slower than in the rest of the brain. Regional accelerations in growth and increased density or myelination of white matter, in contrast, may be associated with general drug effects on increasing cellular excitation, which would in turn influence the activity-dependent survival of axons, dendrites, and synapses66 and proliferation of oligodendrocyte precursors72-75 during circuit formation. Activity-dependent processes would account for the dose-dependent increases in volumes of the lateral temporal, posterior parietal, and dorsal medial surfaces (Figure 1).

Limitations

This study has limitations. Co-exposure is a possible reason for the commonality of findings across exposure groups, particularly because women with substance abuse typically use a cocktail of drugs, making differentiation of specific effects difficult even with very large samples. Nevertheless, we strove to exclude women using illicit drugs other than those in the exposure groups, rates of co-occurring drug use were relatively low (Table 1), and findings persisted when all newborns were entered into a single model for all concurrent exposures. The most common co-exposures were tobacco or alcohol use, and all statistical analyses covaried for them. Moreover, alcohol use was minimal and tobacco use was limited in each exposure group (Table 1). We also modeled in each MRI modality the correlates of drug, alcohol, and tobacco use (Figure 1) (eFigures 4-7 and 10-14 in the Supplement). Tobacco correlates were dissimilar to the correlates of the other exposures; alcohol correlates included some regions of reduced volume in anatomic images overlapping with drug associations but did not show the regional enlargements associated with drug exposures. Furthermore, the associations of tobacco and alcohol with FA, ADC, and NAA measures were few and were not similar to the drug correlates (Figure 1). In addition, we demonstrated a dose-response association of drug exposures with brain measures, even when controlling for co-occurring tobacco and alcohol exposures, when using continuous measures of drug exposure in models for the correlates of co-occurring exposures and in scatterplots displaying the dose-response associations (eFigures 22-25 in the Supplement).

We also cannot exclude the possibility that other potential confounders, such as socioeconomic status, prenatal nutrition, or stress, contributed to the commonality of findings, especially because the sample size limited our ability to model confounder effects. However, covarying for these factors had negligible effects on our findings. In addition, the demographics of the exposure and control groups were similar and controlled for many of these potential confounders.

Sample sizes for individual exposure were modest, limiting our statistical power to discern associations. Moreover, study participation, sample size, and eligibility criteria limit the generalizability of our findings. Many mothers discontinued illicit drug use early in their pregnancies, limiting exposures in the second and third trimesters and the brain correlates specific to those periods of fetal development. Unassessed postnatal exposures could have contributed to poorer 12-month neurodevelopmental outcomes in the drug-exposed groups but would not have contributed to neonatal MRI measures. Furthermore, inconsistent follow-up assessments limited our analyses of brain-based mediators of the associations of exposure with long-term outcomes, which should be regarded as heuristic rather than conclusive. In addition, inferring developmental correlates from cross-sectional data such as ours can be hazardous; our inference that exposure associations represent an exaggeration of normal brain maturation requires confirmation with repeated scanning in a long-term study that defines growth trajectories in the newborn brain.

Conclusions

The findings suggest that prenatal illicit drug exposure is associated with measures of newborn brain tissue and that these associations may represent an exaggeration of normal fetal brain maturation, which in turn may mediate the association of prenatal exposures with poorer 12-month infant outcomes.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: April 15, 2020.

Corresponding Author: Bradley S. Peterson, MD, Department of Pediatrics, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, 4650 Sunset Blvd, Mail Stop #135, Los Angeles, CA 90027 (bpeterson@chla.usc.edu).

Published Online: June 15, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1622

Author Contributions: Dr Peterson had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: B.S. Peterson, Rosen, Dingman, Branch.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: B.S. Peterson, Dingman, Toth, Sawardekar, Hao, Liu, Xu, Dong, J.B. Peterson, Ryoo, Serino, Branch, Bansal.

Drafting of the manuscript: B.S. Peterson, Toth, Ryoo, Branch, Bansal.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: B.S. Peterson, Rosen, Dingman, Sawardekar, Hao, Liu, Xu, Dong, J.B. Peterson, Serino, Bansal.

Statistical analysis: B.S. Peterson, Sawardekar, Hao, Xu, Ryoo, Bansal.

Obtained funding: B.S. Peterson, Dingman.

Administrative, technical, or material support: B.S. Peterson, Rosen, Dingman, Toth, Sawardekar, Xu, J.B. Peterson, Branch.

Supervision: B.S. Peterson, Dingman, Xu, Bansal.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant R01 DA017820 (Dr Peterson) from the National Institutes of Health and funding from Children’s Hospital Los Angeles and the University of Southern California.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Information: This research was made possible by provision of data by New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University, New York, New York.

References
1.
Vesga-López  O, Blanco  C, Keyes  K, Olfson  M, Grant  BF, Hasin  DS.  Psychiatric disorders in pregnant and postpartum women in the United States.   Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(7):805-815. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.7.805 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Forray  A.  Substance use during pregnancy.   F1000Res. 2016;5:F1000 Faculty Rev-887. Published online May 13, 2016. doi:10.12688/f1000research.7645.1 PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
US Department of Health and Human Services. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2015 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: detailed tables. Published September 8, 2016. Accessed April 28, 2020. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/results-2015-national-survey-drug-use-and-health-detailed-tables
4.
Brown  QL, Sarvet  AL, Shmulewitz  D, Martins  SS, Wall  MM, Hasin  DS.  Trends in marijuana use among pregnant and nonpregnant reproductive-aged women, 2002-2014.   JAMA. 2017;317(2):207-209. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.17383 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Young-Wolff  KC, Tucker  L-Y, Alexeeff  S,  et al.  Trends in self-reported and biochemically tested marijuana use among pregnant females in California from 2009-2016.   JAMA. 2017;318(24):2490-2491. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.17225 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Volkow  ND, Han  B, Compton  WM, McCance-Katz  EF.  Self-reported medical and nonmedical cannabis use among pregnant women in the United States.   JAMA. 2019;322(2):167-169. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.7982 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Desai  RJ, Hernandez-Diaz  S, Bateman  BT, Huybrechts  KF.  Increase in prescription opioid use during pregnancy among Medicaid-enrolled women.   Obstet Gynecol. 2014;123(5):997-1002. doi:10.1097/AOG.0000000000000208 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Martin  CE, Longinaker  N, Terplan  M.  Recent trends in treatment admissions for prescription opioid abuse during pregnancy.   J Subst Abuse Treat. 2015;48(1):37-42. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.07.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Patrick  SW, Davis  MM, Lehmann  CU, Cooper  WO.  Increasing incidence and geographic distribution of neonatal abstinence syndrome: United States 2009 to 2012  [published correction in J Perinatol. 2015;35(8):667].  J Perinatol. 2015;35(8):650-655. doi:10.1038/jp.2015.36 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Cain  MA, Bornick  P, Whiteman  V.  The maternal, fetal, and neonatal effects of cocaine exposure in pregnancy.   Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2013;56(1):124-132. doi:10.1097/GRF.0b013e31827ae167 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Bigsby  R, LaGasse  LL, Lester  B,  et al.  Prenatal cocaine exposure and motor performance at 4 months.   Am J Occup Ther. 2011;65(5):e60-e68. doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.001263 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gouin  K, Murphy  K, Shah  PS; Knowledge Synthesis Group on Determinants of Low Birth Weight and Preterm Births.  Effects of cocaine use during pregnancy on low birthweight and preterm birth: systematic review and metaanalyses.   Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;204(4):340.e1-340.e12. doi:10.1016/j.ajog.2010.11.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Mayes  LC, Cicchetti  D, Acharyya  S, Zhang  H.  Developmental trajectories of cocaine-and-other-drug-exposed and non–cocaine-exposed children.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24(5):323-335. doi:10.1097/00004703-200310000-00003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Lester  BM, Tronick  EZ, LaGasse  L,  et al.  The maternal lifestyle study: effects of substance exposure during pregnancy on neurodevelopmental outcome in 1-month-old infants.   Pediatrics. 2002;110(6):1182-1192. doi:10.1542/peds.110.6.1182 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Willford  J.  The effects of prenatal cocaine use on infant development.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2008;30(2):96-106. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2007.12.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Singer  LT, Arendt  R, Minnes  S, Farkas  K, Salvator  A.  Neurobehavioral outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(5):653-666. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(00)00092-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Tronick  EZ, Messinger  DS, Weinberg  MK,  et al.  Cocaine exposure is associated with subtle compromises of infants’ and mothers’ social-emotional behavior and dyadic features of their interaction in the face-to-face still-face paradigm.   Dev Psychol. 2005;41(5):711-722. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.711 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Larkby  C, Day  NL.  Effects of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior and growth at 10 years of age.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2013;40:1-8. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2013.08.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Minnes  S, Robin  NH, Alt  AA,  et al.  Dysmorphic and anthropometric outcomes in 6-year-old prenatally cocaine-exposed children.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2006;28(1):28-38. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2005.09.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Covington  CY, Nordstrom-Klee  B, Ager  J, Sokol  R, Delaney-Black  V.  Birth to age 7 growth of children prenatally exposed to drugs: a prospective cohort study.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24(4):489-496. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00233-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Bandstra  ES, Morrow  CE, Accornero  VH, Mansoor  E, Xue  L, Anthony  JC.  Estimated effects of in utero cocaine exposure on language development through early adolescence.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;33(1):25-35. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.07.001 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Lewis  BA, Kirchner  HL, Short  EJ,  et al.  Prenatal cocaine and tobacco effects on children’s language trajectories.   Pediatrics. 2007;120(1):e78-e85. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-2563 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Lewis  BA, Minnes  S, Short  EJ,  et al.  The effects of prenatal cocaine on language development at 10 years of age.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2011;33(1):17-24. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Minnes  S, Min  MO, Short  EJ,  et al.  Executive function in children with prenatal cocaine exposure (12-15 years).   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2016;57:79-86. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2016.07.002 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Lambert  BL, Bauer  CR.  Developmental and behavioral consequences of prenatal cocaine exposure: a review.   J Perinatol. 2012;32(11):819-828. doi:10.1038/jp.2012.90 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Ackerman  JP, Riggins  T, Black  MM.  A review of the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure among school-aged children.   Pediatrics. 2010;125(3):554-565. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0637 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Buckingham-Howes  S, Berger  SS, Scaletti  LA, Black  MM.  Systematic review of prenatal cocaine exposure and adolescent development.   Pediatrics. 2013;131(6):e1917-e1936. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-0945 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
El Marroun  H, Tiemeier  H, Steegers  EAP,  et al.  Intrauterine cannabis exposure affects fetal growth trajectories: the Generation R Study.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(12):1173-1181. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181bfa8ee PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Fried  PA, Watkinson  B, Gray  R.  A follow-up study of attentional behavior in 6-year-old children exposed prenatally to marihuana, cigarettes, and alcohol.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1992;14(5):299-311. doi:10.1016/0892-0362(92)90036-A PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Leech  SL, Richardson  GA, Goldschmidt  L, Day  NL.  Prenatal substance exposure: effects on attention and impulsivity of 6-year-olds.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1999;21(2):109-118. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(98)00042-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Goldschmidt  L, Richardson  GA, Willford  J, Day  NL.  Prenatal marijuana exposure and intelligence test performance at age 6.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(3):254-263. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318160b3f0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Goldschmidt  L, Day  NL, Richardson  GA.  Effects of prenatal marijuana exposure on child behavior problems at age 10.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(3):325-336. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(00)00066-0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Fried  PA, Watkinson  B.  Visuoperceptual functioning differs in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally exposed to cigarettes and marihuana.   Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(1):11-20. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(99)00046-X PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Guo  X, Spencer  JW, Suess  PE, Hickey  JE, Better  WE, Herning  RI.  Cognitive brain potential alterations in boys exposed to opiates: in utero and lifestyle comparisons.   Addict Behav. 1994;19(4):429-441. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)90065-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Hunt  RW, Tzioumi  D, Collins  E, Jeffery  HE.  Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of infants exposed to opiate in-utero.   Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(1):29-35. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.01.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Hickey  JE, Suess  PE, Newlin  DB, Spurgeon  L, Porges  SW.  Vagal tone regulation during sustained attention in boys exposed to opiates in utero.   Addict Behav. 1995;20(1):43-59. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)00044-Y PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ornoy  A, Michailevskaya  V, Lukashov  I, Bar-Hamburger  R, Harel  S.  The developmental outcome of children born to heroin-dependent mothers, raised at home or adopted.   Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20(5):385-396. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(96)00014-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ornoy  A, Segal  J, Bar-Hamburger  R, Greenbaum  C.  Developmental outcome of school-age children born to mothers with heroin dependency: importance of environmental factors.   Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43(10):668-675. doi:10.1017/S0012162201001219 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Ross  EJ, Graham  DL, Money  KM, Stanwood  GD.  Developmental consequences of fetal exposure to drugs: what we know and what we still must learn.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(1):61-87. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.147 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Behnke  M, Smith  VC; Committee on Substance Abuse; Committee on Fetus and Newborn.  Prenatal substance abuse: short- and long-term effects on the exposed fetus.   Pediatrics. 2013;131(3):e1009-e1024. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-3931 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
McLellan  AT, Luborsky  L, Cacciola  J,  et al.  New data from the Addiction Severity Index: reliability and validity in three centers.   J Nerv Ment Dis. 1985;173(7):412-423. doi:10.1097/00005053-198507000-00005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
McLellan  AT, Kushner  H, Metzger  D,  et al. The fifth edition of the Addiction Severity Index. J Subst Abuse Treat. 1992;9(3):199-213. doi:10.1016/0740-5472(92)90062-s
43.
Zanis  DA, McLellan  AT, Cnaan  RA, Randall  M.  Reliability and validity of the Addiction Severity Index with a homeless sample.   J Subst Abuse Treat. 1994;11(6):541-548. doi:10.1016/0740-5472(94)90005-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Duyn  JH, Gillen  J, Sobering  G, van Zijl  PC, Moonen  CT.  Multisection proton MR spectroscopic imaging of the brain.   Radiology. 1993;188(1):277-282. doi:10.1148/radiology.188.1.8511313 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
First  MB, Spitzer  RL, Gibbon  M, Williams  JBW.  Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Sisorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002.
46.
Hollingshead  AB.  Four-Factor Index of Social Status. Yale University Press; 1975.
47.
Hamilton  M.  Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.   Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6(4):278-296. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Hamilton  M.  The assessment of anxiety states by rating.   Br J Med Psychol. 1959;32(1):50-55. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Wechsler  D.  Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed. Pearson; 2003.
50.
Bayley  N.  Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. 3rd ed. Harcourt Assessment; 2006.
51.
Sparrow  SS, Balla  DA, Cicchetti  DV, Doll EA.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Survey Form Manual. American Guidance Service; 1984.
52.
Reichle  CW, Smith  GM, Gravenstein  JS, Macris  SG, Beecher  HK.  Comparative analgesic potency of heroin and morphine in postoperative patients.   J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1962;136:43-46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Manfredonia  JF.  Prescribing methadone for pain management in end-of-life care.   J Am Osteopath Assoc. 2005;105(3)(suppl 1):S18-S21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Johnson  NL.  Systems of frequency curves generated by methods of translation.   Biometrika. 1949;36(pt 1-2):149-176. doi:10.1093/biomet/36.1-2.149 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Burbidge  JB, Magee  L, Robb  AL.  Alternative transformations to handle extreme values of the dependent variable.   J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(401):123-127. doi:10.1080/01621459.1988.10478575 Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Benjamini  Y, Yekutieli  D.  The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency.   Ann Stat. 2001;29(4):1165-1188.Google ScholarCrossref
57.
MacKinnon  DP, Krull  JL, Lockwood  CM.  Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect.   Prev Sci. 2000;1(4):173-181. doi:10.1023/A:1026595011371 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Zhao  X, Lynch  J, Chen  Q.  Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis.   J Consum Res. 2010;37(2):197-206. doi:10.1086/651257 Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Hermoye  L, Saint-Martin  C, Cosnard  G,  et al.  Pediatric diffusion tensor imaging: normal database and observation of the white matter maturation in early childhood.   Neuroimage. 2006;29(2):493-504. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Dubois  J, Dehaene-Lambertz  G, Kulikova  S, Poupon  C, Hüppi  PS, Hertz-Pannier  L.  The early development of brain white matter: a review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants.   Neuroscience. 2014;276:48-71. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.044 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ouyang  M, Dubois  J, Yu  Q, Mukherjee  P, Huang  H.  Delineation of early brain development from fetuses to infants with diffusion MRI and beyond.   Neuroimage. 2019;185:836-850. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Concha  L.  A macroscopic view of microstructure: using diffusion-weighted images to infer damage, repair, and plasticity of white matter.   Neuroscience. 2014;276:14-28. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Ding  X-Q, Kucinski  T, Wittkugel  O,  et al.  Normal brain maturation characterized with age-related T2 relaxation times: an attempt to develop a quantitative imaging measure for clinical use.   Invest Radiol. 2004;39(12):740-746. doi:10.1097/00004424-200412000-00005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Leppert  IR, Almli  CR, McKinstry  RC,  et al; Brain Development Cooperative Group.  T(2) relaxometry of normal pediatric brain development.   J Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;29(2):258-267. doi:10.1002/jmri.21646 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Haynes  RL, Borenstein  NS, Desilva  TM,  et al.  Axonal development in the cerebral white matter of the human fetus and infant.   J Comp Neurol. 2005;484(2):156-167. doi:10.1002/cne.20453 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Tau  GZ, Peterson  BS.  Normal development of brain circuits.   Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):147-168. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.115 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Blüml  S, Wisnowski  JL, Nelson  MD  Jr,  et al.  Metabolic maturation of the human brain from birth through adolescence: insights from in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy.   Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(12):2944-2955. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs283 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Tomiyasu  M, Aida  N, Endo  M,  et al.  Neonatal brain metabolite concentrations: an in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy study with a clinical MR system at 3 Tesla.   PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e82746. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082746 PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Rae  CD.  A guide to the metabolic pathways and function of metabolites observed in human brain 1H magnetic resonance spectra.   Neurochem Res. 2014;39(1):1-36. doi:10.1007/s11064-013-1199-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Clark  JB.  N-acetyl aspartate: a marker for neuronal loss or mitochondrial dysfunction.   Dev Neurosci. 1998;20(4-5):271-276. doi:10.1159/000017321 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Šlamberová  R.  Drugs in pregnancy: the effects on mother and her progeny.   Physiol Res. 2012;61(suppl 1):S123-S135.PubMedGoogle Scholar
72.
Barres  BA, Raff  MC.  Proliferation of oligodendrocyte precursor cells depends on electrical activity in axons.   Nature. 1993;361(6409):258-260. doi:10.1038/361258a0 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Demerens  C, Stankoff  B, Logak  M,  et al.  Induction of myelination in the central nervous system by electrical activity.   Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(18):9887-9892. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.18.9887 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Gibson  EM, Purger  D, Mount  CW,  et al.  Neuronal activity promotes oligodendrogenesis and adaptive myelination in the mammalian brain.   Science. 2014;344(6183):1252304. doi:10.1126/science.1252304 PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Almeida  RG, Lyons  DA.  On myelinated axon plasticity and neuronal circuit formation and function.   J Neurosci. 2017;37(42):10023-10034. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3185-16.2017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×