Objectives
To present a longitudinal overview of the sleep schedules during weekdays and the weekend in a large sample of children and to examine the sleep-wake schedules in relation to nocturnal sleep duration and birth season.
Design
Longitudinal survey design.
Setting
Data were collected by home interviews and questionnaires.
Participants
Data for sleep schedules at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years on weekdays were obtained for 1112 children. Data for bedtimes on the weekend were available for 1094 children, and data for wake times on the weekend were available for 1083 children.
Outcome Measures
Habitual bedtimes and wake times during weekdays and the weekend were reported by the mothers at the 3 ages. Nighttime sleep duration was calculated from bedtime to wake time. Birth characteristics came from medical records.
Results
As children grow up, we found later bedtimes on the weekend for a given bedtime on weekdays. Interestingly, most children slept less during the weekend compared with weekdays. No significant difference (P > .10 for all) was found in the seasonal distribution of births in any of the sleep-wake schedules.
Conclusions
Despite a lack of a strong preference for sleep timing during childhood, the displacement of bedtime toward later hours during the weekend starts early in life. More important, sleep-wake patterns are associated with sleep duration and later bedtimes on the weekend seem to shorten sleep duration in most children.
Sleep patterns vary greatly during development. It is known that nocturnal sleep consolidation occurs at around the age of 5 months,1,2 while a progressive sleep schedule delay is observed during adolescence.3 How do sleep schedules change during childhood?
A number of cross-sectional studies4-8 reported different average sleep-wake schedules in children between the ages of 3 and 6 years. However, these studies did not restrict their sample to a specific age. Moreover, the evolution of sleep-wake schedules during childhood is poorly documented longitudinally.
Sleep plays a crucial role in many important spheres, such as mood, neurocognition, and academic performance.9-11 One group11 showed that children who had a short sleep duration had more behavioral problems and worse performances on neuropsychological tests compared with children who persistently slept at least 10 hours at night in early childhood. A reduction in sleep duration may be due to later bedtimes, earlier rising times, or both. One study6 addressed this issue and highlighted a marked shift of bedtime to later hours with age, especially on weekdays.
In addition, these researchers found a longer sleep duration on the weekend compared with weekdays from the age of 9 years. However, they did not study sleep duration as a function of sleep-wake schedules.
The preference for particular sleep timing is called “chronotype”;
some people are morning types and choose to go to bed early and wake up early, whereas others are evening types and go to bed late and awaken late. Parents will usually know whether their child is an early bird or a morning sleeper. Nevertheless, the studies that looked at sleep-wake schedules in preschool-aged children do not permit the characterization of the chronotype mainly because the sleep-wake schedules have been averaged for the whole group. In Japan, a specific chronotype has been self-determined in children as young as 6 years of age.12 Also, in young adults and teenagers, chronotype has been associated with sleep duration,12-15 with evening types having a shorter sleep duration on weekdays and a longer sleep duration on the weekend. Parental evaluation of the child's habitual sleep patterns, especially when there are no timing constraints (as may be observed on the weekend), may give valuable information about preferred sleep timing in childhood and its association with sleep duration.
Chronotype in young adults and teenagers is in part influenced by birth season.16-19 A spring birth increases the tendency toward eveningness, while a fall birth increases the tendency for morningness. An association between birth season and sleep patterns in childhood could indicate that sleep-timing preference at that age is already an indication of chronotype.
This study presents, for the first time to our knowledge, a longitudinal overview of the sleep schedules during weekdays and the weekend in a large sample of children. It also examines the sleep-wake schedules in relation to nocturnal sleep duration. Finally, if sleep-wake schedules are an indication of preferred sleep timing in childhood,
we predict an association between extreme schedules and specific seasons of birth.
This study is part of the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development, a large epidemiological study conducted by the Quebec Institute of Statistics. The children were recruited from the Quebec Master Birth Registry of the Ministry of Health and Social Services.
Most children (84.5%) had a Canadian nonimmigrant mother. Most of the sample was white (88.4%). Other ethnic groups, such as black Africans,
Native Amerindians, Arabs, Asians, and others represent 3.4%, 0.3%,
2.0%, 1.6%, and 4.3% of the sample, respectively. Most mothers spoke French as a first language (76.3%), 8.7% spoke English, and 15.0%
had another first language. Children with serious neurodevelopmental or health problems were excluded from the study.
The present article focused on the habitual bedtimes and wake times during weekdays and the weekend when children were approximately aged 4 years (mean [SD] age, 49.5 [3.1] months), 5 years (mean [SD]
age, 61.4 [3.1] months), and 6 years (mean [SD] age, 73.8 [3.1] months).
Longitudinal data for sleep schedules at the 3 ages on weekdays were obtained for 1112 children; data were available for 1094 children for bedtimes on the weekend and for 1083 children for wake times on the weekend. Before participating in the study, all families received detailed information by mail on the aims and procedures of the research program and signed a consent form. The protocol was approved by the Sacré-Coeur Hospital ethics committee.
The data for the present article were collected from a questionnaire and an interview with the person who best knew the child. First, the Self-administered Questionnaire for the Mother, which took about 20
minutes to complete, provided information on the infant's sleep-wake schedules at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years, such as bedtimes and wake times during weekdays and the weekend in the following wording: “In general, at what time do you put your child to bed for the night?”
and “In general, at what time does your child wake up or that you wake up your child in the morning?” Friday and Saturday nights had been used to assess habitual bedtime during the weekend.
Saturday and Sunday mornings had been used to evaluate habitual wake time during the weekend. Mothers had to write down the exact times (no intervals because of multiple choice answers). Nighttime sleep duration was calculated from bedtime to wake time. Birth characteristics came from medical records: birth date, sex of the child, and prematurity (defined as < 37 weeks' gestation).
The comparisons of sleep-wake schedules between age and between weekdays and the weekend were assessed using repeated-measures analyses of variance. The effect size of analyses of variance was determined using the Cohen f value from the partial η2 value to give a sense of the level of importance of the effects.20 An effect size around 0.1 is considered small, one around 0.25 is medium, and one around 0.4 is large. Pearson product moment correlations were used to assess relationships between sleep variables and between ages. Then, we compared the relationship between sleep variables (intercepts and slopes) in the 3 age groups'
linear regressions with F tests.
A semiparametric group-based modeling strategy21,22 was performed to obtain longitudinal overviews of 4 sleep timing variables (bedtime on weekdays,
bedtime on the weekend, wake time on weekdays, and wake time on the weekend) by using computer software (Proc Traj, an extension of SAS for Windows, version 9.1; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, North Carolina).
Three-way analyses of variance were used to assess differences in sleep duration across 3 patterns (pattern factor: early, middle, and late) for each sleep time across ages and between weekdays and the weekend. The analysis of variance probabilities were adjusted for repeated measures using the Huynh-Feldt correction.
The association between extreme schedules (early vs late patterns)
and birth seasons was compared by χ2 tests. Birth seasons were evaluated from birth dates.23 Sampling was predominantly made during spring and fall (n = 1005
and n = 892, respectively), and few children of the studied population were born in summer or winter (n = 66 and n = 157,
respectively). Therefore, long and short photoperiod seasons were grouped together to normalize the distribution: spring-summer and fall-winter, respectively.
Sleep-wake schedule comparisons and correlations
Table 1 shows averaged bedtimes and wake times on weekdays and the weekend at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years. The average bedtime was later during the weekend compared with weekdays (F1,1092 = 3639.1, P < .001,
Cohen f = 1.8). Also, bedtime during weekdays was earlier at the age of 6 years than at the ages of 4 and 5 years, while bedtime during the weekend was earlier at the age of 4 years than at the ages of 5 and 6 years (F2,2184 = 18.4, P < .001, Cohen f = 0.18).
Wake time was also later during the weekend compared with weekdays (F1,1080 = 1678.0, P < .001, Cohen f = 1.2).
Wake time during weekdays was earlier at the age of 6 years than at the ages of 4 and 5 years, while wake time during the weekend was earlier at the age of 4 years than at the ages of 5 and 6 years (F2,2160 = 23.1, P < .001, Cohen f = 0.2).
In general, sleep schedule variables were highly correlated with each other. Bedtimes and wake times were highly correlated on weekdays at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years (r = 0.44, r = 0.47, and r = 0.47, respectively; P < .001 for all) and on the weekend (r = 0.53, r = 0.51,
and r = 0.50, respectively; P < .001 for all). Weekday and weekend bedtimes were also highly correlated at the ages of 4, 5, and 6 years (r = 0.73, r = 0.68, and r = 0.63,
respectively; P < .001 for all);
the same applies to wake times (r = 0.73, r = 0.67, and r = 0.50, respectively; P < .001
for all). However, the shape of the association differed across ages for correlations between bedtime and wake time on weekdays. Accordingly,
the relationship between bedtime and wake time was modified at the age of 6 years, showing that wake time occurred earlier for bedtimes later than 8 PM (different slope: F2,4600 = 13.8, P < .001). Correlations between weekdays and the weekend for bedtimes differed between ages, with a later weekend bedtime for a given weekday bedtime at the age of 6 years compared with the ages of 4 and 5 years; and at the age of 5 years compared with the age of 4 years (different intercepts: F2,4576 = 90.0, P < .001).
Correlations between weekdays and the weekend for wake times also showed between-ages differences, with a later weekend wake time for a given weekday wake time at the age of 5 years compared with the ages of 4 and 6 years; and at the age of 6 years compared with the age of 4 years (different intercepts: F2,4562 = 46.6, P < .001). Table 2 reveals that between-age sleep-wake schedule correlations were stronger between the ages of 4 and 5 years than between the ages of 4 and 6 years. Sleep-wake schedules observed at the age of 5 years were highly correlated with those observed at the age of 6 years for the weekend, whereas they were modestly associated for weekdays.
Longitudinal overview of sleep-wake schedules
Although averaged sleep-wake schedules are generally stable throughout early childhood, this pattern may not be typical for all children. Rather than assume that all children follow the same developmental pattern of sleep-wake schedule over time, Figure 1 shows 3 patterns (early, medium, and late) for habitual bedtimes and wake times on weekdays and on the weekend. Most of the children were put to bed around 8 PM (51.4%)
and woke up between 6 and 7 AM (53.8%) on weekdays.
On the weekend, most children were put to bed around 9 PM (58.0%) and woke up between 7 and 8 AM (46.7%).
Often, children were not classified in the same pattern for all sleep-wake variables. For example, a child in the early pattern for wake time on weekdays may be found in the middle pattern for wake time on the weekend. The 2 most common combinations of sleep-wake schedules were as follows: (1) middle patterns for bedtimes (weekdays and the weekend) and wake time on weekend, with an early pattern for wake time on weekdays (n = 141) and (2) early patterns for bedtimes (weekdays and the weekend) and wake time on the weekend,
with a middle pattern for wake time on weekdays (n = 129).
Association between sleep-wake patterns and sleep duration
A significant effect of sleep pattern was found for all sleep-wake variables (F2,1066 ≥ 52.7, P < .001). In general, for bedtime patterns, sleep duration was the longest in the early pattern, shorter in the middle pattern, and shortest in the late pattern, whereas the late patterns of wake time showed the longest sleep duration.
For bedtime on weekdays (Figure 2A), significant pattern × week/weekend interaction (F2,1066 = 28.0, P < .001) showed that sleep duration decreases on the weekend in a stronger way in the early pattern compared with the middle pattern. In the late pattern, the sleep duration increases in the weekend compared with the weekdays, which differs significantly from middle and early patterns. For wake time on weekdays (Figure 2B), we found a significant pattern × age × week/weekend interaction (F4,2132 = 9.8, P < .001).
Children in the early and late patterns at age 4 decreased their sleep duration from weekdays to the weekend, which significantly differs from the middle pattern decrease. At the age of 5 years, the sleep duration decreased on the weekend in the 3 patterns of wake time on weekdays and to a larger extent in the late pattern compared with the middle one. At the age of 6 years, the decrease in sleep duration from weekdays to the weekend was similar in the early and middle patterns but differed from the late pattern in which no difference in sleep duration was found between weekdays and the weekend.
For bedtime on the weekend (Figure 2C), significant pattern × week/weekend interaction (F2,1066 = 4.1, P = .02) revealed that the sleep duration significantly decreases on the weekend in a weaker way in the early pattern compared with the middle and late patterns. The late and middle patterns had a similar decrease of sleep duration between weekdays and the weekend. For wake time on the weekend (Figure 2D), a significant 3-way interaction was also found (F4,2132 = 14.2, P < .001). Children in the early and middle patterns at age 4 decreased their sleep duration from weekdays to the weekend, which significantly differs from the late pattern's nonsignificant increase. The difference in sleep duration between weekdays and the weekend differs significantly between the 3 patterns of wake time at the ages of 5 and 6 years. The sleep duration decreases on the weekend in the early pattern and increases on the weekend in the late pattern, whereas it was equivalent during weekdays and the weekend in the middle pattern, at the age of 5 years. The sleep duration decreases on the weekend in the early and middle patterns and increases on the weekend in the late pattern at the age of 6 years.
Birth season distribution of early and late sleep-wake schedules
A recent study24 found that preterm birth shifts toward morningness the distribution of chronotypes in adolescence. We found more preterm-born children in the early bedtime on weekend group (6.5% vs 3.3% and 1.9% in the middle and late groups,
respectively; χ2 = 7.3, P = .03) and a similar trend in the early wake time on weekend group (6.2% vs 3.3% and 2.8% in the middle and late groups,
respectively; χ2 = 5.6, P = .06). No significant difference appears for weekday's sleep-wake schedules. Nevertheless, preterm-born children were excluded in further analysis to minimize the effects of this factor. The distributions of full-term children in early and late patterns of sleep schedules in function of seasons were as follows.
(1) For bedtime on weekdays, in spring-summer, early included 51.7%
and late included 50.0%; and in fall-winter, early included 48.3%
and late included 50.0% (P = .82).
(2) For wake time on weekdays, in spring-summer, early included 53.7%
and late included 56.7%; and in fall-winter, early included 46.3%
and late included 43.3% (P = .57).
(3) For bedtime on the weekend, in spring-summer, early included 59.1%
and late included 51.5%; and in fall-winter, early included 40.9%
and late included 48.5% (P = .18).
(4) For wake time on the weekend, in spring-summer, early included 50.6% and late included 58.7%; and in fall-winter, early included 49.4% and late included 41.3% (P = .10).
In sum, no significant difference was found in the distribution of births in spring-summer and fall-winter in any of the 4 variables of sleep-wake schedules.
Our results showed that the 6-year-old subjects in our sample had earlier bedtimes and wake times during weekdays than when their were either 4 or 5 years old. Moreover, the correlation between bedtimes and wake times on weekdays revealed that the wake time at the age of 6 years occurred earlier for bedtimes later than 8 PM. Therefore, between-age correlations for bedtimes and wake times during weekdays became weaker through the years. These age-dependent differences in schedules during weekdays most probably result from the beginning of school at the age of 6 years, producing an advance in the sleep schedule and a modification of the bedtime–wake time relationship toward a decrease in sleep duration. School start time has also been reported as an environmental constraint on sleep schedule and sleep duration in teenagers.25
A displacement of sleep-wake patterns toward later hours during the weekend was shown as early as the first years of life.6 There was a shift of bedtimes during the weekend to later hours with age in comparisons with sleep schedules during weekdays. We also observed a shift of wake times during the weekend to later hours between 4 and 6 years of age compared with sleep schedules during weekdays. Two reasons might explain the whole displacement of sleep-wake patterns toward later hours during the weekend. First, the parents could be less strict with bedtime on the weekend knowing that their children do not have to get up early the next morning. Second, this phase delay could be related to characteristics of the endogenous circadian clock. The average human circadian period has been reported to be close to 24.2 hours in adults26 and 24.3 hours in teenagers.27 If the endogenous circadian period in children is already longer than 24 hours, the sleep schedule would naturally tend to delay.
Most children in our cohort were put to bed around 8 PM and woke up between 6 and 7 AM during weekdays.
Similar bedtimes have been reported in the United States,5 Switzerland,7 and Belgium.8 On the other hand,
several studies found later bedtimes in other countries, eg, in Italy4 and Iceland6 or earlier bedtimes, eg, in Australia.28 In addition, similar wake times were shown in the United States,5 whereas most countries reported later wake times, as in Italy,4 Iceland,6 Switzerland,7 and Belgium.8 The between-country discrepancies about sleep-wake patterns are probably because of the influence of environmental and sociocultural differences. Indeed,
a cross-national survey on sleep habits of adolescents revealed significant variations between the 11 European countries studied.29 However, these studies did not examine the distinction between weekdays and weekend sleep schedules, except for the study of Thorleifsdottir and coworkers.6
The present study has been able to identify a minority of children with late sleep patterns. In particular, we observed that 9.4% of children were put to bed around 10 PM and 13.4% awaken around 9 AM on the weekend. It would be of particular interest and possible with this ongoing project to verify the sleep patterns of these children when they reach adolescence. Those children might be at particular risk of developing a delayed sleep phase syndrome,
a condition characterized by abnormally late sleep-wake patterns and often showing early signs of schedule delay.30
More important, we observed that most children slept less during the weekend compared with weekdays, while the reverse is habitually observed in adults and teenagers. This probably results from 2 combined factors: (1) a more liberal parental attitude during the weekend toward bedtime and (2) a spontaneous early wake time in most children. Moreover,
later bedtimes on weekdays and the weekend are specifically associated with shorter nocturnal sleep duration. Touchette and coworkers11 have found that children with short sleep are at greater risk of having a high hyperactivity score or a lower cognitive performance. Short sleep duration was also associated with increased risk of excess weight and obesity in children.31,32 Future studies should seek to verify whether health or family conditions are potentially associated with different sleep-wake patterns.
However, a few children slept more during the weekend than during weekdays: children with late weekday bedtimes and children with late weekend wake times. Our results showed that children with a late bedtime on weekdays have the shortest sleep duration; we can speculate that they are attempting to recover sleep during the weekend, as adolescents do.33,34 For the children with a late wake time on the weekend, the lengthening of their sleep on the weekend might indicate that this small proportion of children has early signs of capacity for delaying sleep schedules.
In our study, although there was a trend for children with late wake times on weekdays and the weekend to be born more often during spring-summer than during fall-winter, extreme sleep-wake schedules were not associated with birth season. This could be explained by several factors. First, sleep-wake schedules assessed by parents may not be a reliable sign of diurnal preference partly because sleep-wake schedules in early childhood might mostly represent parental choice rather than the endogenous preferred schedules of the children. This might be linked to the question used for determining bedtime in the present study: “At what time do you put your child to bed?”
Then, variations in wanted sleep timing during early childhood might not correspond to the diurnal preference observed in teenagers and adults given the larger sleep need in children. Presently, our results do not permit strong support of this last assumption.
The major strength of this project was the high number of children measured longitudinally. However, spurious significant results could have occurred because of the large sample (eg, the between-age differences in sleep schedules might not all be clinically relevant, given the small effect sizes). Moreover, the study is limited by parental reports of sleep-wake schedules. Sleep duration reported herein should be interpreted as time in bed because sleep onset and time spent awake may particularly be difficult to estimate for parents.8 Fortunately, the reliability between actigraphy and parental reports for sleep duration is high.35-38 Another limitation is that daytime sleep data were not taken into consideration in the present study. However, in a previous study11 based on the same cohort, daytime napping was not associated with patterns of nighttime sleep duration. Finally,
children were generally whites; thus, the results might not be applicable to other ethnic groups.
Despite these limitations, this research draws a large overview of the development of sleep-wake schedules during the first part of childhood. Our results support earlier findings showing that the displacement of bedtime and wake times toward later hours during the weekend begins early in life. We also observed different patterns of sleep schedules that seemed relatively stable across ages. Despite the observation of differences in schedule timing, our results indicated that the sleep pattern for most children is probably the reflection of the absence of a strong preference for sleep timing in combination with a sleep-wake schedule most likely influenced by parental choice and school constraints. Surprisingly, we found that most children slept less during the weekend than during weekdays. Finally, this article highlights the importance of putting children early to bed and maintaining regular sleep schedules during weekdays and the weekend to optimize sleep duration and, in turn, maximize development.
Correspondence: Jacques Y. Montplaisir,
MD, PhD, Sleep Research Center, Sacré-Coeur Hospital, 5400
Gouin West Blvd, Montreal, QC H4J 1C5, Canada (jy.montplaisir@umontreal.ca).
Accepted for Publication: October 23,
2007.
Author Contributions: Dr Touchette had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Touchette, Mongrain,
and Montplaisir. Acquisition of data: Tremblay and Montplaisir. Analysis and interpretation of data: Touchette, Mongrain, Petit, and Montplaisir. Drafting of the manuscript: Touchette and Mongrain. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Touchette, Mongrain, Petit, Tremblay, and Montplaisir. Statistical analysis: Touchette. Obtained funding: Tremblay and Montplaisir. Administrative, technical, and material support:Montplaisir. Study supervision:Mongrain, Petit, and Montplaisir.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Ministry of Health and Social Services; the Canadian Institutes of Health Research; the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada; the Quebec Fund for Research on Society and Culture; the Quebec Fund for Research on Nature and Technology; the Health Research Fund of Quebec; the Molson Foundation; the Ministry of Research, Science and Technology; Human Resources Development Canada; the Canadian Institute for Advanced Research; Health Canada; the National Science Foundation;
University of Montreal; Laval University; and McGill University.
Role of the Sponsor: The funding bodies had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection,
analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review,
or approval of the manuscript.
Additional Contributions: Jean Paquet,
PhD, and Gaétan Poirier, MSc, provided ssistance. We acknowledge the children and families who participated and the assiduous work of all interviewers who assessed mothers and children during the research program.
1.de Roquefeuil
GDjakovic
MMontagner
H New data on the ontogeny of the child's sleep-wake rhythm.
Chronobiol Int 1993;10
(1)
43- 53
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Touchette
EPetit
DPaquet
J
et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across early childhood.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2005;159
(3)
242- 249
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Carskadon
MAAcebo
CJenni
OG Regulation of adolescent sleep: implications for behavior.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004;1021276- 291
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Ottaviano
SGiannotti
FCortesi
FBruni
OOttaviano
C Sleep characteristics in healthy children from birth to 6 years of age in the urban area of Rome.
Sleep 1996;19
(1)
1- 3
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Owens
JASpirito
A McGuinn
MNobile
C Sleep habits and sleep disturbances in elementary school-aged children.
J Dev Behav Pediatr 2000;21
(1)
27- 36
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Thorleifsdottir
BBjörnsson
JKBenediktsdottir
BGislason
THKristbjarnarson
H Sleep and sleep habits from childhood to young adulthood over a 10-year period.
J Psychosom Res 2002;53
(1)
529- 537
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Iglowstein
IJenni
OGMolinari
LLargo
RH Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends.
Pediatrics 2003;111
(2)
302- 307
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Spruyt
KO’Brien
LMCluydts
RVerleye
GBFerri
R Odds, prevalence and predictors of sleep problems in school-age normal children.
J Sleep Res 2005;14
(2)
163- 176
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Dahl
RE The consequences of insufficient sleep for adolescents: links between sleep and emotional regulation.
Phi Delta Kappan 1999;80
(5)
354- 359
Google Scholar 11.Touchette
EPetit
DSéguin
JBoivin
MTremblay
REMontplaisir
J Associations between sleep duration patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry.
Sleep 2007;30
(9)
1213- 1219
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Park
YMMatsumoto
KSeo
YJShinkoda
H Sleep and chronotype for children in Japan.
Percept Mot Skills 1999;88
(3, pt 2)
1315- 1329
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Giannotti
FCortesi
FSebastiani
TOttaviano
S Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence.
J Sleep Res 2002;11
(3)
191- 199
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Roenneberg
TWirz-Justice
AMerrow
M Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes.
J Biol Rhythms 2003;18
(1)
80- 90
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Natale
VAdan
AChotai
J Further results on the association between morningness-eveningness preference and the season of birth in human adult.
Neuropsychobiology 2002;46
(4)
209- 214
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Caci
HRobert
PDossios
CBoyer
P L’échelle de matinalité pour enfants et adolescents: propriétés psychométriques et effet du mois de naissance.
Encephale 2005;31
(1, pt 1)
56- 64
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Mongrain
VPaquet
JDumont
M Contribution of the photoperiod at birth to the association between season of birth and diurnal preference.
Neurosci Lett 2006;406
(1-2)
113- 116
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Cohen
J Statistical Power Analysis for Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A. Erlbaum Associates1988;
21.Nagin
DSTremblay
RE Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58
(4)
389- 394
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Nagin
DS Analyzing developmental trajectories: a semiparametric, group-based approach.
Psychol Methods 1999;4139- 157
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Natale
VSansavini
ATrombini
EEsposito
MJAlessandroni
RFaldella
G Relationship between preterm birth and circadian typology in adolescence.
Neurosci Lett 2005;382
(1-2)
139- 142
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Szymczak
JTJasinska
MPawlak
EZwierzykowska
M Annual and weekly changes in the sleep-wake rhythm of school children.
Sleep 1993;16
(5)
433- 435
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Czeisler
CADuffy
JFShanahan
TL
et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker.
Science 1999;284
(5423)
2177- 2181
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Carskadon
MALabyak
SEAcebo
CSeifer
R Intrinsic circadian period of adolescent humans measured in conditions of forced desynchrony.
Neurosci Lett 1999;260
(2)
129- 132
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Armstrong
KLQuinn
RADadds
MR The sleep patterns of normal children.
Med J Aust 1994;161
(3)
202- 206
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.American Sleep Disorders Association, The International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Lawrence, KS Allen Press Inc1990;
31.Reilly
JJArmstrong
JDorosty
AR
et al. Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team, Early life risk factors for obesity in childhood: cohort study.
BMJ 2005;330
(7504)
1357
PubMed10.1136/bmj.38470.670903.E0
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Chaput
JPBrunet
MTremblay
A Relationship between short sleeping hours and childhood overweight/obesity:
results from the “Québec en Forme” Project.
Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30
(7)
1080- 1085
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Mindell
JAOwens
JACarskadon
MA Developmental features of sleep.
Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 1999;8
(4)
695- 725
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Carskadon
MAAcebo
CSeifer
R Extended nights, sleep loss, and recovery sleep in adolescents.
Arch Ital Biol 2001;139
(3)
301- 312
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Sadeh
A Assessment of intervention for infant night waking: parental reports and activity-based home monitoring.
J Consult Clin Psychol 1994;62
(1)
63- 68
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Sekine
MChen
XHamanishi
SWang
HYamagami
TKagamimori
S The validity of sleeping hours of healthy young children as reported by their parents.
J Epidemiol 2002;12
(3)
237- 242
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref