[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Article
March 2, 2009

Predicting Gambling Behavior in Sixth Grade From Kindergarten Impulsivity: A Tale of Developmental Continuity

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: École de Psychoéducation and Centre de Recherche de l’Hôpital Sainte-Justine, Université de Montréal (Dr Pagani), Centre for Youth Gambling Problems and High-Risk Behaviors, McGill University (Dr Derevensky), and Département d’Éducation et Formation Spécialisée, Université du Québec à Montréal (Dr Japel), Montréal, Québec, Canada.

Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(3):238-243. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.7
Abstract

Objective  To examine the relationship between early impulsive behavior, rated by kindergarten teachers, and self-reported gambling in sixth grade.

Design  Prospective longitudinal study.

Setting  The 1999 kindergarten cohort of the Montreal Longitudinal Preschool Study in Canada.

Participants  Written parental consent was obtained for 181 of the 377 children from intact families at kindergarten exclusively selected for follow-up telephone interviews in the fall of sixth grade, 6 years after the initial assessments. Of these, 163 children had complete data in kindergarten (mean age, 5.5 years) and sixth grade (mean age, 11.5 years) for the key variables in the analyses.

Main Outcome Measure  Self-reported gambling behavior in sixth grade.

Results  A 1-unit increase in kindergarten impulsivity corresponded to a 25% increase in later self-reported child involvement in gambling (SE = .02). This was above and beyond potential child- and family-related confounds, including parental gambling.

Conclusions  Our findings offer insight about how the nature and course of early impulsivity might relate to a significantly higher propensity toward involvement in games of chance in later childhood. It is suggested that developmentally continuous risks associated with early impulsivity place individuals on a risk trajectory toward excessive gambling involvement in adolescence and emerging adulthood.

Policy changes in the last century have transformed legalized gambling into a leisure activity. Social norms have progressed from early 20th-century prohibition to outright encouragement, especially given today's user-friendly, available, and seductive advertisement campaigns.1 Although gambling is considered an adult behavior, adolescents are especially receptive to this trend. In fact, gambling has become so normative that it characterizes typical Western youth.2

There are public health risks despite becoming statistically normative by adolescence. More than ever, greater numbers of adolescents and young adults are engaging in at-risk, problematic, and pathological gambling.3 Problematic gambling in adults is associated with substance use, depression and suicide, psychopathology, poor general health, and a multitude of family, legal, and criminal problems.1,4,5 Most disconcerting is that young people seem more vulnerable than adults to gambling-related morbidity2,6 and suicidality.7

Data suggest that in most cases, youthful recreational gambling predates pathological gambling in adulthood.1,2,8,9 A cornerstone question might be, what predates youthful gambling? Much like with other addictions, there is a proneness to dysfunctional behavior that explains the developmental nature of adolescent gambling as a risk behavior and its course from use to abuse.10 This represents part of a larger group of clinical problems that share common psychosocial underpinnings. This chain of risk factors predicts developmental psychopathology across the life course.

A tale of developmental continuity would need a theoretical latent characteristic that links problems from one childhood period to the next. Impulsivity represents an important diagnostic component of both pathological gambling in adults and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children.11 Although most hyperkinetic symptoms are outgrown by adulthood, impulsivity associated with ADHD persists throughout the life course.12 Pathological gambling and ADHD not only share impulsivity as a clinical feature13 but also harbor common risks for long-term impairment in the realm of psychosocial, familial, and occupational dysfunction.1,14,15 Impulsivity also represents a major risk indicator for suicidal ideation and behavior.16

Earlier neuroscience research using cross-sectional17 and retrospective18,19 methods with small clinical samples suggests a link between both disorders in adults. This has led to more recent confirmatory evidence of an executive function problem.20 Unfortunately, the methodological nature of these studies does not inform us about developmental issues. Moreover, laboratory experiments, although very controlled, need to be tested with a wider range of typically developing children found in population-based longitudinal studies.

One such data set with boys has found evidence of a longitudinal link. Impulsivity in early adolescence predicted involvement in gambling, substance use, and delinquency in later adolescence.21,22 This link has been replicated with impulsivity as a predictor at age 10 years.23 Although these results harvest better conclusions than in the past, one can never be sure whether the boys' gambling involvement did not overlap with the impulsivity measure (at ages 10, 12, and 14 years), creating a chicken-and-egg problem of which came first. Given that diagnosis of ADHD is often cued by teacher complaints during preschool or early schooling,12 it is unlikely that impulsivity and gambling are comorbid but rather might be operating in a developmental chain of prediction.

Finding prospective markers of later gambling behavior could facilitate developmental targets for preventive interventions. That is, a more rigorous understanding of developmental continuity of risk-oriented addictive behaviors will better inform the application of timely and effective clinical strategies. Early childhood interventions, often implemented before parenting and child interaction routines have crystallized, promise better long-term economic returns for both the individual and society as a whole.24-27

This study uses a prospective longitudinal design to examine the relationship between early impulsivity, rated by kindergarten teachers, and self-reported gambling involvement in sixth grade. Our definition of impulsivity is broad and reflects a lack of inhibitory control and cognitive self-regulation of attention. Of course, any observation of such a link could potentially be explained by other individual and family influences. As such, we tested whether children's early behavior remains predictive of later involvement in gambling activities while accounting for plausible alternative explanations.

Methods

Participants

Children in this study represent a selected subsample from the Montreal Longitudinal Preschool Study,28 which comprises several sequential longitudinal cohorts launched from the fall of 1997, 1998, 1999, and 2000. At the launch of each sequential cohort of the Montreal Longitudinal Preschool Study, 4- and 5-year-old children were enrolled in preschool in disadvantaged areas of Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Each sequential cohort represents one-third of its catchment area and was actually selected following a multilevel consent process involving school board administrators, local school committees, and teachers. Once parents gave consent, their children were assessed individually and by the children's teachers. Parents were requested to complete survey materials as well.

In spring 2005, funds were obtained for grafting a small institutional review board–approved gambling involvement research component with the Montreal Longitudinal Preschool Study. The 1999 kindergarten-entry cohort was selected for this purpose (n = 467), mainly because children would be in sixth grade in fall 2005. Children from intact families at kindergarten were exclusively selected for follow-up (n = 377) given that it was desirable to have information on gambling involvement for both natural parents. A divorce during the course of a longitudinal study of child development often means data loss for 1 parent (usually the father).

Written parental consent was first requested for eligibility to participate in a follow-up study on parent and child gambling involvement. Of these, 181 families remained for the follow-up gambling study. We conducted telephone interviews of parents and children in the fall of sixth grade, 6 years after the initial assessments.

The criteria for the study reported here (n = 163; 53.0% boys) required that the participant had complete data in kindergarten (mean age, 5.5 years) and sixth grade (mean age, 11.5 years) for the 2 key variables. Nonretained cases for analyses are attributable to incomplete child (n = 6) or parental (n = 12) gambling data at follow-up. Although there are socioeconomic differences between the larger Montreal Longitudinal Preschool Study and its subsample intended for study (maternal education is higher for the selected sample, P = .03), there are no remarkable differences in sociodemographic, family process, or behavioral characteristics between the subsample intended for study (n = 181) and the subsample finally retained for data analysis. Weighted propensity scores were computed to compare participants who were lost to follow-up with those who were not, revealing no significant differences (mean [SD] predicted probability, 0.31 [0.05] for the nonretained group and 0.32 [0.04] for the retained group).

Measures

Youth Gambling Behavior as Dependent Variable

The assessment by telephone interview began with a statement about gambling:

The following questions deal with gambling and money. Gambling is an activity where you bet money or objects of value in order to gain prizes of more money or objects. But, there is also a risk of losing everything. There are no right or wrong answers to these questions; we ask that you answer according to your own personal experience as a kid.

This 5-item scale aims to generate a global construct of child involvement in gambling in the past year.29 When youngsters were in sixth grade, they were asked, “How many times have you done the following?”: cards (14.0% of the sample had participated to some degree); bingo (7.9%); bought lottery, instant lottery (scratch and win), or sports lottery tickets (4.0%); played video games for money or video poker (eg, at arcades) (13.0%); and placed bets at sports venues with friends or on games that require skill (eg, billiards, pool, bowling) (8.0%). Lottery tickets, video poker, and video lottery terminals are considered adult-only venues in the Canadian province of Quebec and are thus illegal if used by youth younger than 18 years. Responses ranged from never (score of 0) to less than once per month (score of 1), 1 to 3 times per month (score of 2), and once per week or more (score of 3). The total score ranged between 5 and 15 with higher scores indicating greater levels of gambling behavior (α = .75; mean [SD] score, 6.4 [1.4]).

Early Impulsive Behavior as Independent Variable

At the beginning of kindergarten, teachers completed the Social Behavior Questionnaire, which comprises a number of factors that assess children's behavioral adjustment. The impulsivity measure in this study combines the inattentive, distractible, and hyperactive factors into one 9-item subscale (α = .91): inattentive (2 items: inattentive; does not listen attentively; α = .81); distractible (2 items: easily distractible; unable to concentrate; α = .82); and hyperactive (5 items: seems agitated and has difficulty staying in one place; keeps moving; seems impulsive; has difficulty waiting his or her turn; has difficulty staying calm; α = .90). All of the items are rated on a scale of 1 (often or very true) to 3 (never or not true), reverse scored (except for the negatively worded attentive items), and then summed. A higher value on the scale indicates a higher degree of teacher-reported symptoms associated with ADHD. The Social Behavior Questionnaire represents a reliable estimate of current and later psychosocial and academic adjustment.30-32 Childhood norms are available from the National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth.

Covariates

Sex

Given that there are sex differences for both gambling and ADHD,33-35 we considered this information from official records in our data-analytic strategy.

Maternal Education

Although indicators of socioeconomic status do not seem to play a role in risk for ADHD or gambling, this variable acts as a proxy for a number of often unmeasured endogenous parental characteristics such as economic resources, motivation, perseverance, cognitive stimulation, and achievement orientation in the home environment that are associated with proneness to problem behavior in youth.10 Parents completed a sociodemographic questionnaire when their children were in kindergarten.

Family Dysfunction

Problems in the home seem to be correlated with both ADHD and gambling.34,36 When their children were in kindergarten, mothers completed a 12-item scale developed by researchers at Chedoke-McMaster Hospital, McMaster University and used extensively in the National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth. This scale aims to generate a global construct of family functioning such as problem solving, communication, roles, affective involvement, affective responsiveness, and self-regulation in times of conflict. The unit of analysis for this Likert-response scale is the family and is rated 3 for strongly agree, 2 for agree, 1 for disagree, and 0 for strongly disagree.37,38 Negatively worded items were reverse scored so that a higher score indicates greater agreement with the statement. The total score varies between 0 and 36 with higher scores indicating greater levels of family dysfunction (α = .88).

Parental Gambling Involvement

There is an abundance of research on the intergenerational transmission of problem behaviors,39 ADHD,40 and gambling.41 Interviews began with the following statement:

Gambling is an activity where we bid money or objects in the intention of winning more money or more objects but where the risk of losing it all is present.

We then assessed a general construct of gambling involvement in the past year for both mothers and fathers using a 4-item scale when their children were in sixth grade. Parents responded to the interview question, “How often have you and your spouse played the following activities?”: cards (6.3% of mothers and 8.1% of fathers had participated in the past year); slot machines or video lottery (4.4% of mothers, 3.8% of fathers); lottery (21.9% of mothers, 21.3% of fathers); and casino games (12.5% of mothers, 10.6% of fathers). Responses ranged from never (score of 0) to less than once per month (score of 1), 1 to 3 times per month (score of 2), and once per week or more (score of 3). The items were answered by the mother and then for the respective spouse. Given that the scale addresses both parents, the total score varied between 4 and 24 with higher scores indicating greater levels of parental gambling behavior (α = .58).

Emotionally Distressed Behavior

Depressive symptoms and ADHD often coexist,42 as do depression and gambling.8 The depressed and anxious scales from the Social Behavior Questionnaire were combined into 1 reliable composite factor (α = .81) representing emotional distress manifested in the kindergarten classroom: anxious (3 items: seems worried or fearful; seems anxious; is nervous or tense) and depressed (2 items: seems unhappy, sad, or depressed; cries a lot).

Data-analytic strategy

We began by estimating an ordinary least squares regression in which sixth-grade gambling behavior is regressed on kindergarten impulsivity. Our interest was in correctly modeling this linear relationship, which can be interpreted as the effect of early childhood impulsivity on later childhood behaviors that are, in principle, precursors to adolescent gambling involvement. Once we established this link, we had to ensure that we accounted for the possibility of omitted variable bias, which was likely to arise if unobserved family or child characteristics were statistically or substantively correlated with our key variables. To secure an unbiased estimation of the variable, we took into account parental gambling involvement, aiming to approximately account for some genetic and environmental predispositions. The equation also controls for child (sex and early emotionally distressed behavior) and family (maternal education and family dysfunction) factors. Our results bear on this fully controlled model: CGBi6GR = a1 + β1 IMPiKE + β2 PGi6GR + γ1 CHILDiKE + γ2 FAMILYiKE + eit, where a and e represent the intercept and stochastic error, respectively, CGB indicates sixth-grade gambling behavior (6GR), IMP indicates kindergarten impulsivity, KE indicates kindergarten entry, and PG indicates parental gambling involvement, for each individual i.

Results

Descriptive statistics of child and family characteristics are reported in Table 1. The preliminary results revealed a significant bivariate correlation between our 2 key variables of interest (standardized β = .25; SE = .02; P < .001). In the fully controlled multiple regression equation, early impulsive behavior remained a significant predictor of self-reported gambling behavior in sixth grade (F6,111 = 3.44; P = .008; R2 = 0.16; multiple R2 = 0.40). As noted in Table 2, a 1-unit increase in kindergarten impulsivity was associated with a corresponding 25% increase in later self-reported child gambling involvement (SE = .02), holding all other variables in the model constant.

Table 1. 
Basic Child and Family Characteristics of Participants Retained for Analysis
Basic Child and Family Characteristics of Participants Retained for Analysis
Table 2. 
Prediction of Gambling Behavior in Sixth Grade by Kindergarten Impulsivity Operationalized as Teacher-Rated Inattentive, Distractible, and Hyperactive Behaviors
Prediction of Gambling Behavior in Sixth Grade by Kindergarten Impulsivity Operationalized as Teacher-Rated Inattentive, Distractible, and Hyperactive Behaviors

Comment

For quite some time, we have known that people who start gambling in youth are more likely to experience severe gambling problems in adulthood.9 This has stimulated an interest in youthful gambling involvement. The youngest in samples have been early adolescents. As this field of research becomes more developmentally oriented, the urge to address predictors from concomitants becomes stronger. This study gets us a bit closer to the chicken-and-egg issue. Our results suggest that early behaviors associated with ADHD symptoms reliably forecast gambling involvement in sixth grade. Higher levels of problematic kindergarten classroom behavior at age 5 years predicted a higher propensity toward later gambling behavior. This truly prospective association over a 6-year period is above and beyond important confounds that are not often considered in this type of research.

Our results suggest that behavioral features such as inattentiveness, distractibility, and hyperactivity at school entry represent a vulnerability factor for precocious risk-oriented behavior like gambling in sixth grade. It is very plausible that these childhood characteristics snowball into cumulative risks for youngsters who do not eventually outgrow the distractibility and inattentiveness from early childhood and become involved in gambling as a typical pastime for many youth. Most importantly, our observations suggest a developmentally continuous effect of impulsivity that places individuals on a life course trajectory toward gambling involvement in adolescence and emerging adulthood.

What might be the fundamental mechanisms operating in this developmental chain? The underlying impulsivity trait points to abnormalities in the mesolimbic reward circuits. From an executive control perspective, gambling involves risk estimation, decision making, and feedback processing.43 Such activities recruit the orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortices.44 Children with ADHD show significant aberrations in the neural substrates associated with decision making and reward processing during guessing and gambling, to the extent that they experience enhanced sensitivity to unfavorable outcomes.45 In turn, their greater vulnerability to a negative affective reaction to incorrect guessing and unexpected losses influences less insightful subsequent estimations in guessing and gambling. This creates a vicious circle of ineffective self-assessment and self-regulation processes. Interestingly, these characteristics are noted in adult pathological gamblers with a confirmed childhood history of ADHD.46

Most noteworthy is that the preschool period shows remarkable growth in the brain regions associated with affective decision making, thus explaining the rapid development of this characteristic prior to formal school entry.47 As such, there are direct clinical implications for prevention and intervention as important mechanisms in circumventing later risks associated with impulsivity. Early childhood is viewed as a critical period in the development of cognitive self-regulation.48 In fact, current data suggest that executive function training prior to first grade is most advantageous because it tends to influence general improvements in learning and social skills during the critical transition to formal schooling.49 With respect to intervention, training of cognitive control in attentional processes50 and working memory51 has shown very positive results in children with ADHD. This specific line of work, which focuses on effortful control, has been successfully extended to more general applications of attention training in preschool children.52 This research tells us that universal cognitive control training prior to first grade remains possible without being overly costly.

We are not overly concerned about the fact that the children in this study were living in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Residential settings in Canada are not as homogeneous as their American counterparts given differences in social policy and programming.53 That is, poor and nonpoor people live in close quarters of each other. The neighborhoods were identified as disadvantaged because the schools offered free breakfasts to their students. This program is in effect only in districts where welfare and unemployment are more prevalent. First, the application of abundant statistical control likely minimized any existing bias. Second, the parents who consented to participate in the longitudinal study, by nature of their consent, are intuitively less at risk than those who did not consent to participate. Third, assessing children from intact families afforded methodological strength. It not only balanced out some of the disadvantage issues but also restrained any possible influence of unmeasured family or parental correlates of single parenthood that could have confounded our observations. Last and most important, leisurely venues involving games of chance are distributed more densely in more impoverished districts.54 In fact, neighborhood disadvantage is positively related to frequency of gambling and problem or pathological gambling.55 These two findings alone make a strong argument that our sample characterizes an important ecological laboratory in which to examine the developmental precursors of gambling in youngsters.

Although we resolve the question of directionality in the life course relationship between 2 individual characteristics, our design does not help resolve causal issues. It is plausible that children with impulsivity problems in early childhood (such as ADHD) might be marginalized by peers. This in turn could play a role in a causal pathway toward subsequent involvement in games of chance to win money or prizes. Another limit is that the data did not include a professional diagnosis of ADHD or, even better, more phenotypic measures of impulsivity (given that it is not a unitary construct). Large population-based studies like this one provide a portrait of interindividual variations in typical development by using associated diagnostic symptoms. In fact, even low levels of symptoms associated with deficits in attention and inhibitory control in typically developing children risk limiting their individual potential and contribution to society.56 At this time, we know of no other longitudinal study that comprises early childhood diagnoses and later measures of gambling behavior. We encourage others to extend our findings with more rigorous, population-based, prospective analyses.

Be it a consequence or correlate, youth gambling can easily unravel into a public heath issue. This study builds upon previous research on youthful risk behaviors in several ways. Its prospective nature and the age at which we assessed child gambling involvement are unprecedented. Because the past focus has been on prevalence issues, previous studies have surveyed older youth. We estimate a carefully specified model with intuitively little overlap between our key variables. That is, children were unlikely to be gambling in kindergarten, when teachers reported on inattentive, distractible, and hyperactive behaviors in the classroom. Last, by controlling for characteristics that are often comorbid in both clinical pictures, our predictive link suggests how the nature and course of impulsivity translate into risks of problematic gambling beyond childhood.

Correspondence: Linda S. Pagani, MD, École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal, CP 6128, succursale Centre-ville, Montréal, QC H3C 3J7, Canada (linda.s.pagani@umontreal.ca).

Accepted for Publication: October 14, 2008.

Author Contributions: All of the authors had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for its integrity and the accuracy of its analysis. Study concept and design: Pagani and Derevensky. Acquisition of data: Pagani and Derevensky. Analysis and interpretation of data: Pagani and Japel. Drafting of the manuscript: Pagani and Derevensky. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Pagani, Derevensky, and Japel. Statistical analysis: Pagani. Obtained funding: Pagani. Administrative, technical, and material support: Pagani and Derevensky. Study supervision: Pagani.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This work was funded by Canada's Social Science and Humanities Research Council Standard Research Grants Program.

Additional Contributions: Statistical analyses were conducted by Mylène Baptista, BSc, under the guidance of Dr Pagani. The staff at the Research Unit on Children's Psycho-Social Maladjustment, Centre de Recherche de l’Hôpital Sainte-Justine, Université de Montréal provided support with data collection and management.

References
1.
Potenza  MNKosten  TRRounsaville  BJ Pathological gambling.  JAMA 2001;286 (2) 141- 144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Proimos  JDuRant  RHPierce  JDGoodman  E Gambling and other risk behaviors among 8th- to 12th-grade students.  Pediatrics 1998;102 (2) e23PubMed10.1542/peds.102.2.e23Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Shaffer  HJHall  MNVander Bilt  J Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: a research synthesis.  Am J Public Health 1999;89 (9) 1369- 1376PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Shaffer  HJKorn  DA Gambling and related mental disorders: a public health analysis.  Annu Rev Public Health 2002;23171- 212PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Shaw  MCForbush  KTSchlinder  JRosenman  EBlack  DW The effect of pathological gambling on families, marriages, and children.  CNS Spectr 2007;12 (8) 615- 622PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Potenza  MN A perspective on adolescent gambling: relationship to other risk behaviors and implications for prevention strategies. Romer  D Reducing Adolescent Risk An Integrated Approach. Thousand Oaks, CA Sage Publications2003;247- 255Google Scholar
7.
Nower  LGupta  RBlaszczynski  ADerevensky  J Suicidality ideation and depression among youth gamblers: a preliminary examination of three studies.  Int Gambl Stud 2004;4 (1) 69- 80Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Lynch  WJMaciejewki  PKPotenza  MN Psychiatric correlates of gambling in adolescents and young adults grouped by age at gambling onset.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61 (11) 1116- 1122PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Volberg  RA The prevalence and demographics of pathological gamblers: implications for public health.  Am J Public Health 1994;84 (2) 237- 241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Jessor  R New Perspectives on Adolescent Risk Behavior.  New York, NY Cambridge University Press1998;
11.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
12.
Sadock  BJSadock  VA Kaplan and Sadock's Synopsis of Psychiatry: Behavioral Sciences/Clinical Psychiatry. 10th ed. New York, NY Lippincott Williams & Wilkins2007;
13.
Derevensky  JLPratt  LMHardoon  KKGupta  R Gambling problems and features of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children and adolescents.  J Addict Med 2007;1 (3) 165- 172Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Barkley  RAFischer  MSmallish  LFletcher  K Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: antisocial activities and drug use.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45 (2) 195- 211PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Boonstra  AMOosterlaan  JSergeant  JABuitelaar  JK Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review.  Psychol Med 2005;35 (8) 1097- 1108PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Zouk  HTousignant  MSeguin  MLesage  ATurecki  G Characterization of impulsivity in suicide completers: clinical, behavioral and psychosocial dimensions.  J Affect Disord 2006;92 (2-3) 195- 204PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Carlton  PLManowitz  P McBride  HNora  RSwartzburg  MGoldstein  L Attention deficit disorder and pathological gambling.  J Clin Psychiatry 1987;48 (12) 487- 488PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Rugle  LMelamed  L Neuropsychological assessment of attention problems in pathological gamblers.  J Nerv Ment Dis 1993;181 (2) 107- 112PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Specker  SMCarlson  GAChristenson  GAMarcotte  M Impulse control disorders and attention deficit disorder in pathological gamblers.  Ann Clin Psychiatry 1995;7 (4) 175- 179PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Malloy-Diniz  LFuentes  DLeite  WBCorrea  HBechara  A Impulsive behavior in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: characterization of attentional, motor and cognitive impulsiveness.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13 (4) 693- 698PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Vitaro  FArseneault  LTremblay  RE Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males.  Addiction 1999;94 (4) 565- 575PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Vitaro  FBrendgen  MLadouceur  RTremblay  RE Gambling, delinquency, and drug use during adolescence: mutual influences and common risk factors.  J Gambl Stud 2001;17 (3) 171- 190PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Vitaro  FWanner  BLadouceur  RBrendgen  MTremblay  RE Trajectories of gambling during adolescence.  J Gambl Stud 2004;20 (1) 47- 69PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Barnett  WSMasse  LN Early childhood program design and economic returns: comparative benefit-cost analysis of the Abecedarian program and policy implications.  Econ Educ Rev 2007;26 (1) 113- 125Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Belfield  CNores  MBarnett  WSSchweinhart  L Updating the benefit-cost analysis of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program through age 40.  Educ Eval Policy Anal 2005;27 (3) 245- 262Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Boisjoli  RVitaro  FLacourse  ÉBarker  EDTremblay  RE Impact and clinical significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys: 15-year follow-up.  Br J Psychiatry 2007;191 (5) 415- 419PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Greenwood  PW Costs and Benefits of Early Childhood Intervention.  Washington, DC Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Justice Programs, US Dept of Criminal Justice1999;
28.
Duncan  GJDowsett  CJClaessens  A  et al.  School readiness and later achievement [published correction appears in Dev Psychol. 2008;44(1):232].  Dev Psychol 2007;43 (6) 1428- 1446PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Dickson  LDerevensky  JGupta  R Youth gambling problems: examining of risk and protective factors.  Int Gambl Stud 2008;8 (1) 25- 47Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Kerr  MTremblay  REPagani  LSVitaro  F Boys' behavioral inhibition and the risk of later delinquency.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54 (9) 809- 816PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Lacourse  ENagin  DSVitaro  FCôté  SArseneault  LTremblay  RE Prediction of early onset deviant peer group affiliation: a 12-year longitudinal study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (5) 562- 568PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Tremblay  REPihl  ROVitaro  FDobkin  PL Predicting early onset of male antisocial behavior from preschool behavior: a test of two personality theories.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51 (9) 732- 739PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Desai  RAMaciejewski  PKPantalon  MVPotenza  MN Gender differences in adolescent gambling.  Ann Clin Psychiatry 2005;17 (4) 249- 258PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
DuPaul  GJ McGuuey  KEEckert  TLVanBrackle  J Preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: impairments in behavioral, social, and school functioning.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40 (5) 508- 515PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Ellenbogen  SDerevensky  JGupta  R Gender differences among adolescents with gambling-related problems.  J Gambl Stud 2007;23 (2) 133- 143PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Langhinrichsen-Rohling  JRohde  PSeeley  JRRohling  ML Individual, family, and peer correlates of adolescent gambling.  J Gambl Stud 2004;20 (1) 23- 46PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Byles  JByrne  CBoyle  MOfford  D Reliability and validity of the general functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device.  Fam Process 1988;27 (1) 97- 104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Epstein  NBBaldwin  LMBishop  DS The McMaster Family Assessment Device.  J Marital Fam Ther 1983;9 (2) 171- 180Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Brook  JSBalka  EBWhiteman  MZheng  L Intergenerational transmission of risks for problem behavior.  J Abnorm Child Psychol 2002;30 (1) 65- 76PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Minde  KEakin  LHechtman  L  et al.  The psychosocial functioning of children and spouses of adults with ADHD.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003;44 (4) 637- 646PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Oei  TPRaylu  N Familial influence on offspring gambling: a cognitive mechanism for transmission of gambling behavior in families.  Psychol Med 2004;34 (7) 1279- 1288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Fischer  MBarkley  RAFletcher  KESmallish  L The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children: predictors of psychiatric, academic, social, and emotional adjustment.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993;32 (2) 324- 332PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Goudriaan  AEOosterlaan  Jde Beurs  EVan den Brink  W Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28 (2) 123- 141PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
van Leijenhorst  LCrone  EABunge  SA Neural correlates of developmental differences in risk estimation and feedback processing.  Neuropsychologia 2006;44 (11) 2158- 2170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
van Meel  CSOosterlaan  JHeslenfeld  DJSergeant  JA Telling good from bad news: ADHD differentially affects processing of positive and negative feedback during guessing.  Neuropsychologia 2005;43 (13) 1946- 1954PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Rodriguez-Jimenez  RAvila  CJimenez-Arriero  MA  et al.  Impulsivity and sustained attention in pathological gamblers: influence of childhood ADHD history.  J Gambl Stud 2006;22 (4) 451- 461PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Kerr  AZelazo  PD Development of “hot” executive function: the children's gambling task.  Brain Cogn 2004;55 (1) 148- 157PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Shonkoff  JPedPhillips  DAed From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Child Development.  Washington, DC National Academies Press2000;
49.
Rueda  MRPosner  MIRothbart  MK Attentional control and self-regulation. Baumeister  RFVohs  KD Handbook of Self-regulation Research, Theory, and Applications. New York, NY Guilford2004;283- 300Google Scholar
50.
Kerns  KAEso  KThomson  J Investigation of a direct intervention for improving attention in young children with ADHD.  Dev Neuropsychol 1999;16 (2) 273- 295Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Klingberg  TForssberg  HWesterberg  H Training of working memory in children with ADHD.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24 (6) 781- 791PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Diamond  ABarnett  WSThomas  JMunro  S Preschool program improves cognitive control.  Science 2007;318 (5855) 1387- 1388PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Pagani  LBoulerice  BVitaro  FTremblay  RE Effects of poverty on academic failure and delinquency in boys: a change and process model approach.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40 (8) 1209- 1219PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Gilliland  JARoss  NA Opportunities for video lottery gambling in Montréal: an environmental analysis.  Can J Public Health 2005;96 (1) 55- 59PubMedGoogle Scholar
55.
Welte  JWWieczorek  WFBarnes  GMTidwell  MCHoffman  JH The relationship of ecological and geographic factors to gambling behavior and pathology.  J Gambl Stud 2004;20 (4) 405- 423PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Currie  JStabile  M Child mental health and human capital accumulation: the case of ADHD.  J Health Econ 2006;25 (6) 1094- 1118PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×