[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure.  Proportion of Mothers Who Reported Prenatal Vitamin Supplement Intake From 6 Months Before Pregnancy Through the End of Pregnancy
Proportion of Mothers Who Reported Prenatal Vitamin Supplement Intake From 6 Months Before Pregnancy Through the End of Pregnancy

ASD indicates autism spectrum disorder; non-TD, nontypical development; and TD, typical development. Vertical bars represent 95% exact CIs (Clopper-Pearson).

Table 1.  Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Children and Their Mothers in the MARBLES Study
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Children and Their Mothers in the MARBLES Study
Table 2.  Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations of Maternal Prenatal Vitamin Use in the First Month of Pregnancy With Child 36-Month and Outcome Assessment Scores
Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations of Maternal Prenatal Vitamin Use in the First Month of Pregnancy With Child 36-Month and Outcome Assessment Scores
Table 3.  Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations of Maternal Mean Daily Intake of Folic Acid and Iron Supplements in the First Month of Pregnancy With Child Outcome
Unadjusted and Adjusted Associations of Maternal Mean Daily Intake of Folic Acid and Iron Supplements in the First Month of Pregnancy With Child Outcome
1.
Baio  J, Wiggins  L, Christensen  DL,  et al.  Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2014.  MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;67(6):1-23. doi:10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Zuk  O, Hechter  E, Sunyaev  SR, Lander  ES.  The mystery of missing heritability: genetic interactions create phantom heritability.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(4):1193-1198. doi:10.1073/pnas.1119675109PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Hallmayer  J, Cleveland  S, Torres  A,  et al.  Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(11):1095-1102. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Risch  N, Hoffmann  TJ, Anderson  M, Croen  LA, Grether  JK, Windham  GC.  Familial recurrence of autism spectrum disorder: evaluating genetic and environmental contributions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(11):1206-1213. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101359PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Sandin  S, Lichtenstein  P, Kuja-Halkola  R, Larsson  H, Hultman  CM, Reichenberg  A.  The familial risk of autism.  JAMA. 2014;311(17):1770-1777. doi:10.1001/jama.2014.4144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Lyall  K, Schmidt  RJ, Hertz-Picciotto  I.  Maternal lifestyle and environmental risk factors for autism spectrum disorders.  Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(2):443-464. doi:10.1093/ije/dyt282PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Schmidt  RJ, Hansen  RL, Hartiala  J,  et al.  Prenatal vitamins, one-carbon metabolism gene variants, and risk for autism.  Epidemiology. 2011;22(4):476-485. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31821d0e30PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Schmidt  RJ, Tancredi  DJ, Ozonoff  S,  et al.  Maternal periconceptional folic acid intake and risk of autism spectrum disorders and developmental delay in the CHARGE (CHildhood Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment) case-control study.  Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(1):80-89. doi:10.3945/ajcn.110.004416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Surén  P, Roth  C, Bresnahan  M,  et al.  Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children.  JAMA. 2013;309(6):570-577. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.155925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Steenweg-de Graaff  J, Ghassabian  A, Jaddoe  VW, Tiemeier  H, Roza  SJ.  Folate concentrations during pregnancy and autistic traits in the offspring: the Generation R Study.  Eur J Public Health. 2015;25(3):431-433. doi:10.1093/eurpub/cku126PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Braun  JM, Froehlich  T, Kalkbrenner  A,  et al.  Brief report: are autistic-behaviors in children related to prenatal vitamin use and maternal whole blood folate concentrations?  J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2602-2607. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2114-xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Wang  M, Li  K, Zhao  D, Li  L.  The association between maternal use of folic acid supplements during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children: a meta-analysis.  Mol Autism. 2017;8:51. doi:10.1186/s13229-017-0170-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Vázquez  L I, Canals  J, Arija  V.  Review and meta-analysis found that prenatal folic acid was associated with a 58% reduction in autism but had no effect on mental and motor development  [published online November 22, 2018].  Acta Paediatr. doi:10.1111/apa.14657Google Scholar
14.
Levine  SZ, Kodesh  A, Viktorin  A,  et al.  Association of maternal use of folic acid and multivitamin supplements in the periods before and during pregnancy with the risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(2):176-184. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4050PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Virk  J, Liew  Z, Olsen  J, Nohr  EA, Catov  JM, Ritz  B.  Preconceptional and prenatal supplementary folic acid and multivitamin intake and autism spectrum disorders.  Autism. 2016;20(6):710-718. doi:10.1177/1362361315604076PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Hertz-Picciotto  I, Schmidt  RJ, Walker  CK,  et al.  A prospective study of environmental exposures and early biomarkers in autism spectrum disorder: design, protocols, and preliminary data from the MARBLES study.  Environ Health Perspect. 2018;126(11):117004. doi:10.1289/EHP535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Ozonoff  S, Young  GS, Carter  A,  et al.  Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study.  Pediatrics. 2011;128(3):e488-e495. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2825PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Institute of Medicine Standing Committee on the Scientific Evaluation of Dietary Reference Intakes and Its Panel on Folate, Other B Vitamins, and Choline. Dietary Reference Intakes for Thiamin, Riboflavin, Niacin, Vitamin B6, Folate, Vitamin B12, Pantothenic Acid, Biotin, and Choline. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1998.
19.
Institute of Medicine Panel on Micronutrients. Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin A, Vitamin K, Arsenic, Boron, Chromium, Copper, Iodine, Iron, Manganese, Molybdenum, Nickel, Silicon, Vanadium, and Zinc. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2001.
20.
Lord  C, Risi  S, Lambrecht  L,  et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.  J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205-223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Lord  C, Rutter  M, DiLavore  PC, Risi  S.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2000.
22.
Mullen  EM.  Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services Inc; 1995.
23.
Ozonoff  S, Young  GS, Belding  A,  et al.  The broader autism phenotype in infancy: when does it emerge?  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(4):398-407.e2. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Chawarska  K, Shic  F, Macari  S,  et al.  18-Month predictors of later outcomes in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(12):1317-1327.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.09.015 doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.09.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Gotham  K, Pickles  A, Lord  C.  Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(5):693-705. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Lord  C, Rutter  M, DiLavore  PC, Risi  S, Gotham  K, Bishop  SL.  Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2). 2nd ed. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.
27.
Spiegelman  D, Hertzmark  E.  Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences.  Am J Epidemiol. 2005;162(3):199-200. doi:10.1093/aje/kwi188PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
van der Laan  MJ, Rose  S.  Targeted Learning: Causal Inference for Observational and Experimental Data. New York, NY: Springer Series in Statistics; 2011. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-9782-1
29.
Sandin  S, Lichtenstein  P, Kuja-Halkola  R, Hultman  C, Larsson  H, Reichenberg  A.  The heritability of autism spectrum disorder.  JAMA. 2017;318(12):1182-1184. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.12141PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Tordjman  S, Somogyi  E, Coulon  N,  et al.  Gene × environment interactions in autism spectrum disorders: role of epigenetic mechanisms.  Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:53. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Chaste  P, Leboyer  M.  Autism risk factors: genes, environment, and gene-environment interactions.  Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(3):281-292.PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Brian  J, Bryson  SE, Smith  IM,  et al.  Stability and change in autism spectrum disorder diagnosis from age 3 to middle childhood in a high-risk sibling cohort.  Autism. 2016;20(7):888-892. doi:10.1177/1362361315614979PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Shephard  E, Milosavljevic  B, Pasco  G,  et al; BASIS Team.  Mid-childhood outcomes of infant siblings at familial high-risk of autism spectrum disorder.  Autism Res. 2017;10(3):546-557. doi:10.1002/aur.1733PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Schmidt  RJ, Tancredi  DJ, Krakowiak  P, Hansen  RL, Ozonoff  S.  Maternal intake of supplemental iron and risk of autism spectrum disorder.  Am J Epidemiol. 2014;180(9):890-900. doi:10.1093/aje/kwu208PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
February 27, 2019

Association of Maternal Prenatal Vitamin Use With Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder Recurrence in Young Siblings

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Public Health Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, Davis
  • 2MIND Institute, University of California, Davis, Sacramento
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, Sacramento
JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76(4):391-398. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3901
Key Points

Question  Is maternal use of prenatal vitamins associated with decreased risk for autism recurrence in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder?

Findings  In this cohort study of 241 younger siblings of children with autism, the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder was 14.1% in children whose mothers took prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy compared with 32.7% in children whose mothers did not take prenatal vitamins during that time.

Meaning  Maternal daily intake of prenatal vitamins during the first month of pregnancy appears to be associated with reductions in recurrence of autism in high-risk families; additional research is needed to confirm these results, to further investigate specific nutrients, and to inform public health recommendations for autism spectrum disorder prevention.

Abstract

Importance  Maternal use of folic acid supplements has been inconsistently associated with reduced risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the child. No study to date has examined this association in the context of ASD recurrence in high-risk families.

Objective  To examine the association between maternal prenatal vitamin use and ASD recurrence risk in younger siblings of children with ASD.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This prospective cohort study analyzed data from a sample of children (n = 332) and their mothers (n = 305) enrolled in the MARBLES (Markers of Autism Risk in Babies: Learning Early Signs) study. Participants in the MARBLES study were recruited at the MIND Institute of the University of California, Davis and were primarily from families receiving services for children with ASD in the California Department of Developmental Services. In this sample, the younger siblings at high risk for ASD were born between December 1, 2006, and June 30, 2015, and completed a final clinical assessment within 6 months of their third birthday. Prenatal vitamin use during pregnancy was reported by mothers during telephone interviews. Data analysis for this study was conducted from January 1, 2017, to December 3, 2018.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Autism spectrum disorder, other nontypical development (non-TD), and typical development (TD) were algorithmically defined according to Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule and Mullen Scales of Early Learning subscale scores.

Results  After exclusions, the final sample comprised 241 younger siblings, of which 140 (58.1%) were male and 101 (41.9%) were female, with a mean (SD) age of 36.5 (1.6) months. Most mothers (231 [95.9%]) reported taking prenatal vitamins during pregnancy, but only 87 mothers (36.1%) met the recommendations to take prenatal vitamins in the 6 months before pregnancy. The prevalence of ASD was 14.1% (18) in children whose mothers took prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy compared with 32.7% (37) in children whose mothers did not take prenatal vitamins during that time. Children whose mothers reported taking prenatal vitamins during the first month of pregnancy were less likely to receive an ASD diagnosis (adjusted relative risk [RR], 0.50; 95% CI, 0.30-0.81) but not a non-TD 36-month outcome (adjusted RR, 1.14; 95% CI, 0.75-1.75) compared with children whose mothers reported not taking prenatal vitamins. Children in the former maternal prenatal vitamin group also had statistically significantly lower autism symptom severity (adjusted estimated difference, –0.60; 95% CI, –0.97 to –0.23) and higher cognitive scores (adjusted estimated difference, 7.1; 95% CI, 1.2-13.1).

Conclusions and Relevance  Maternal prenatal vitamin intake during the first month of pregnancy may reduce ASD recurrence in siblings of children with ASD in high-risk families. Additional research is needed to confirm these results; to investigate dose thresholds, contributing nutrients, and biologic mechanisms of prenatal vitamins; and to inform public health recommendations for ASD prevention in affected families.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a growing concern, with 1 in every 59 children affected in the United States.1 Complex interactions between genetic and environmental factors likely contribute to ASD risk.2-4 Evidence is accumulating that the in utero environment,3,5,6 including gestational nutrition,6-9 has a potentially large role in the pathologic development of autism. A previous study found that maternal prenatal vitamin intake near conception was associated with an approximately 40% reduction in the risk of ASD in a population-based case-control study.7 This association was replicated in a prospective birth cohort study of 85 176 Norwegian children.9 Additional population-based cohort or nested case-control studies10-13 found that multivitamin or folic acid supplement intake was associated with reductions in the risk for ASD or autistic traits, but for some studies the association was not limited to use near conception,10,14 and another study showed no association.15

A limitation of these previous studies is the potential for confounding by mothers who are more healthy in many ways and thus more likely to follow guidelines to take prenatal vitamins at the start of pregnancy. This study limitation could be overcome by conducting a randomized clinical trial of folic acid prevention; however, given that ASD is still relatively uncommon and randomizing to placebo is unethical, the comparison of higher folic acid dose with the recommended dose would require a large sample size. Furthermore, the critical period for intervention might begin before pregnancy; thus, for a reliable diagnosis, a large number of women would need to be enrolled, and the subset who became pregnant would need to be followed until their child is 3 years of age, which would be expensive. Examining prenatal vitamin use within families already affected by autism could partially overcome this limitation by focusing on mothers already at high risk for having another child with ASD; the younger sibling shares environmental, genetic, and demographic factors with the affected older sibling. To our knowledge, no study to date has examined maternal prenatal vitamin use in the context of ASD recurrence in families affected by ASD.

This study examined whether maternal prenatal vitamin use is associated with reduced risk for ASD in high-risk younger siblings of children who have ASD. Siblings of children with ASD are approximately 12 times more likely to develop ASD than the general population, with a recurrence rate of nearly 1 in 5 siblings (19%-24%).16 They are also at higher risk for other behavioral or developmental diagnoses, including language delay, attention deficit, intellectual disability, and broader autism phenotype.17 These higher risks are attributed to shared genetic and environmental factors. This study focused on participants at high risk for developing ASD and other neurodevelopmental outcomes. With this prospective design, we could more efficiently assess prenatal vitamin use as a potentially protective factor while controlling for a wide range of family characteristics.

Methods
Study Population and Design

Children in this study were enrolled from MARBLES (Markers of Autism Risk in Babies: Learning Early Signs), a study that recruits children from families who receive services through the California Department of Developmental Services16 and that contacts Northern California mothers who have a child with confirmed ASD and who are either planning a pregnancy or pregnant with another child. Participants were recruited at the MIND Institute of the University of California, Davis. The MARBLES team prospectively collected demographic, lifestyle, environmental, and medical information (including supplementation habits) through telephone-assisted interviews and mailed questionnaires. Infants received standardized neurodevelopmental assessments beginning at 6 months and concluding at 3 years of age. The study sample was derived from younger siblings (n = 332) who were born between December 1, 2006, and June 30, 2015, and completed a final clinical assessment within 6 months of their third birthday. The University of California, Davis Institutional Review Board and the California Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects approved this study and the MARBLES study protocols. Neither data nor specimens were collected from children until written informed consent was obtained from their parents. Data analysis for this study was conducted from January 1, 2017, to December 3, 2018.

Maternal Prenatal Vitamin Use

Vitamin and supplement information, including brand, frequency, dose, and timing of maternal intake, for the 6 months before and each month during the pregnancy of the younger sibling was obtained through telephone interviews conducted for the first and second halves of pregnancy and again after birth. The interviews included questions about supplement intake throughout the pregnancy since the previous interview (eMethods in the Supplement). Information from multiple questionnaires was merged to determine prenatal vitamin use for each month before and during the pregnancy.

Positive responses to vitamin use and maximum frequency or dose were recorded for months that overlapped across the questionnaires. Self-reported brand names for vitamins and supplements were used to confirm or correctly classify prenatal vitamins. Adjustments in the denominator for each of the later months were made to account for shorter gestational ages at the time of birth; premature pregnancies were excluded from calculations for the months the mother was not pregnant. Total mean daily folic acid and iron intake from supplements was calculated according to the brands, frequencies, and doses reported for the first month of pregnancy. Categories of mean maternal folic acid and iron intake were based on tertiles of intake for the entire study population and on compliance with the Institute of Medicine–recommended dietary reference intakes for pregnancy of 600 μg folic acid18 and 27 mg iron.19

Outcome Measures and Classification

Trained, reliable examiners assessed the children’s development, including conducting diagnostic assessments for ASD at 3 years using the criterion standard Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), a semistructured, standardized assessment tool in which the examiner observes the child’s social interaction, communication, play, and imaginative use of materials.20,21 The ADOS module 2 algorithm score range is 0 to 28, with higher total scores (7 or higher) indicating evidence for ASD. Cognitive function was measured with the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL),22 which generates 4 subscale scores (fine motor, expressive language, receptive language, and visual reception) and an overall early learning composite score. The MSEL composite score range is 49 to 155, and the T-score range is 20 to 80, with lower composite scores (below 70) indicating developmental delays in cognitive functioning.

Participants were classified into 1 of 3 outcome groups (ASD, typical development [TD], or nontypical development [non-TD]) according to an algorithm that uses ADOS and MSEL scores.17,23,24 Children in the ASD outcome group had scores that were higher than the ADOS cutoff scores and met the DSM-5 criteria for ASD. Children in the non-TD group had low MSEL scores (ie, 2 or more MSEL subscale scores more than 1.5 SD below the normative mean or at least 1 MSEL subscale score more than 2 SD below the normative mean), elevated ADOS scores (ie, within 3 points of the ASD cutoff scores), or both. Children in the TD outcome group had all MSEL scores within 2.0 SD and no more than 1 MSEL subscale score that was 1.5 SD below the normative mean, and their ADOS scores were at least 3 or more points below the ASD cutoff scores. Standardized ADOS-calibrated severity scores (known as comparison scores in the ADOS-2 scale) were calculated as indicators of ASD symptom severity on the ADOS scale depending on age and language level.25 The ADOS-2 comparison scores range from 1 to 10, with a score of 3 or higher indicative of ASD and 6 or higher indicating severe symptoms.26

Statistical Analysis

We calculated P values for group comparisons using 2-sided χ2 or Fisher exact test for categorical variables and independent 2-tailed t tests or 1-way analysis of variance for continuous variables. We used multinomial logistic regression to examine the association between prenatal vitamin use and diagnosis of ASD or non-TD outcome compared with TD. The following variables were examined as potential upstream confounders: maternal age, educational level, prepregnancy body mass index, birthplace (eg, United States or other country), and pregnancy intent; paternal age; child sex, year of birth, and race/ethnicity; home ownership; and type of payment for the delivery. Bivariate analyses examined unadjusted associations of potential confounders with both outcomes (ASD and non-TD) and vitamin use. Next, we built multivariable models, separate for ASD and non-TD risk, by adding 1 variable at a time to the multinomial logistic model and retaining only those variables that caused a 10% or larger change in the exposure or the odds ratio estimates. After the final models were selected, we calculated ASD and non-TD multivariable-adjusted relative risks (RRs) directly using a SAS macro, %RELRISK8 (SAS Institute Inc).27 Stratified analyses and corresponding RRs (when numbers allowed) were used to examine effect modification of prenatal vitamin use by child sex, race (white or non-white), ethnicity (non-Hispanic or Hispanic), intellectual disability (defined as an MSEL composite score under 70 points), and advanced maternal age (≥35 years or <35 years).

A generalized linear model framework with standard choices for link functions, according to the type of outcome, was used to examine ADOS and MSEL scores. The MSEL composite scores were analyzed with normality assumption and a linear link. The distribution of the ADOS severity scores was skewed, with many children scoring at the minimum. Thus, ADOS scores were rescaled (by subtracting 1, the lowest score) and analyzed using negative binomial distribution with a log link.

We conducted additional univariable and multivariable nominal logistic regression analyses to find the association between maternal intake of folic acid and iron (categorized into tertiles and dichotomized according to the dietary reference intake for pregnancy) and outcome. We performed sensitivity analyses with targeted maximum likelihood estimation (TMLE)28 to allow for the possibility that the effect of the exposure may vary across the levels of confounders. Two-sided P < .05 was considered statistically significant. Primary analyses were conducted with SAS software, version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc), and TMLE analyses were implemented with the tmle package in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing).

Results

Of the 332 eligible younger siblings born to 305 mothers in the MARBLES study, 281 (84.6%) completed a final clinical visit and 272 (81.9%) received an algorithmic outcome classification within 6 months of their third birthday. We excluded from analysis 4 children because their outcome assessment was received after they were 42 months of age, 3 children because they had incomplete MSEL scores, and 2 children because their algorithm diagnosis data were received after analyses were conducted. Furthermore, we excluded 9 participants because they were missing prenatal vitamin data and 22 children because they were younger siblings of other children in the sample.

Of the 241 MARBLES siblings retained in the final sample, 140 (58.1%) were male and 101 (41.9%) were female, with a mean (SD) age of 36.5 (1.6) months. Fifty-five children (22.8%) met the algorithmic criteria for ASD, 60 (24.9%) had algorithmic outcomes of non-TD, and 126 (52.3%) were confirmed with TD. Children in the ASD group were statistically significantly more likely to be male compared with children in the TD group (38 of 55 [69.1%] vs 65 of 126 [51.6%]; P = .03). Mothers of children with ASD and non-TD compared with mothers of children with TD were less likely to have private insurance (39 of 55 [70.9%] and 41 of 59 [69.5%] vs 104 of 124 [83.9%]) and education of a bachelor’s degree or higher (21 [38.2%] and 27 [45.0%] vs 72 [57.1%]) (Table 1).

Most mothers (231 [95.9%]) reported taking prenatal vitamins at some point during pregnancy, but most (154 [63.9%]) did not comply with the recommendation to start taking prenatal vitamins in the 6 months before pregnancy; only 87 mothers (36.1%) met the recommendations (Figure). Mothers who took prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy (n = 128) compared with those who did not do so (n = 113) were more educated (76 [59.4%] vs 44 [38.9%]; P = .002), more likely to own their homes (88 [68.8%] vs 55 [49.1%]; P = .002), more likely to have private health insurance (109 [85.8%] vs 75 [67.6%]; P < .001), and more likely to have had an intentional pregnancy (101 [78.9%] vs 50 [44.2%]; P < .001) (Table 1). The prevalence of ASD was 14.1% (18) in children whose mothers took prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy compared with 32.7% (37) in children whose mothers did not take prenatal vitamins during that time.

After statistical adjustment for confounders, we included the following in the final multivariable models: maternal education for ASD risk and child ethnicity, maternal age and education, maternal intention to become pregnant, and type of insurance for non-TD risk. After adjusting for maternal education, we found that children whose mothers took prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy were half as likely to receive an ASD diagnosis (adjusted RR, 0.50; 95% CI, 0.30-0.81) (Table 2 and eFigures 1 and 2 in the Supplement) but not a non-TD 36-month outcome (adjusted RR, 1.14; 95% CI, 0.75-1.75), and they had statistically significantly lower autism symptom severity (adjusted estimated difference, –0.60; 95% CI, –0.97 to –0.23) than children whose mothers did not take prenatal vitamins in the first month. No association was found between prenatal vitamin use and non-TD compared with TD (Table 2). A statistically significant RR for ASD was associated with prenatal vitamin use during the first month when compared with children with non-TD before (RR, 0.57; 95% CI, 0.37-0.87; P = .01) and after (RR, 0.52; 95% CI, 0.33-0.81; P = .004) adjusting for maternal education, age, type of insurance, and pregnancy intention and child white race. The difference in cognitive score across prenatal vitamin intake was statistically significant but was attenuated after adjustment (estimated difference, 7.1; 95% CI, 1.2-13.1) (Table 2). Sensitivity analyses using a TMLE framework confirmed these results (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

The effect estimates for the associations between prenatal vitamin use in the first month of pregnancy and reduced ASD risk did not differ substantially by child race or ethnicity or by maternal age but did differ across child sex with reduced RRs in females, although the number of females with ASD was limited (eTable 2 in the Supplement). The associations between use of prenatal vitamins in the first month of pregnancy and both ASD and non-TD did not appear to differ substantially across child cognitive functioning. Thirty-three mothers (13.7%), including 12 in the ASD, 4 in the non-TD, and 17 in the TD groups, reported taking other multivitamins during the first month of pregnancy.

The highest tertile of total mean folic acid supplemental intake during the first month of pregnancy was associated with the greatest reduction in estimated ASD risk, and the P value for trend was statistically significant before and after adjustment for maternal education (unadjusted RR, 0.36; 95% CI, 0.15-0.84; P = .02 vs adjusted RR, 0.42; 95% CI, 0.17-0.99; P = .048; Table 3). No evidence was found for reduced non-TD risk associated with the highest tertile of folic acid intake or for a dose trend (unadjusted RR, 1.00; 95% CI, 0.57-1.77; P = .99 and adjusted RR, 1.34; 95% CI, 0.72-2.49; P = .36; Table 3). Folic acid intake at or above 600 μg was also associated with statistically significantly lower ASD risk (adjusted RR, 0.51; 95% CI, 0.31-0.82) but no association with non-TD (adjusted RR, 1.19; 95% CI, 0.77-1.84; Table 3). The top 2 tertiles of total mean daily iron intake during the first month of pregnancy were associated with reduced estimated ASD risk compared with the bottom tertile, and evidence of a dose trend was found (tertile 2, adjusted RR, 0.57; 95% CI, 0.33-0.98;tertile 3, adjusted RR, 0.52; 95% CI, 0.30-0.92; adjusted P = .02; Table 3). Iron intake at or above 27 mg was not statistically significantly associated with ASD (adjusted RR, 0.66; 95% CI, 0.40- 1.09). No evidence of an association or a dose trend was observed between iron intake and non-TD (adjusted RR, 0.93; 95% CI, 0.62-1.41; Table 3). Sensitivity analyses using the TMLE approach showed similar effect estimates between ASD and the second and third tertiles of folic acid and iron compared with the first; TMLE findings for non-TD were similar for tertiles of iron, but the highest tertile of folic acid was associated with increased risk for non-TD in all TMLE models (eTable 3 in the Supplement).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study is the first to evaluate the association between risk of ASD recurrence and maternal prenatal vitamin use. We found that in this high-risk cohort, maternal prenatal vitamin use near conception was associated with a reduction in ASD risk in younger siblings similar to the decreased risk observed in the general population. Considering the potential for greater genetic susceptibility in these families, these findings, if replicated, imply that susceptibility could potentially be overcome by environmental manipulation. Twin studies3,5,29 suggest that the early environment contributes to the development of ASD in addition to shared genetics, and gene-environment interaction effects are likely.30,31 Furthermore, this association appeared to be specific to ASD and was not observed for other non-TD outcomes, which may be because the non-TD group was heterogeneous and, according to previous studies of other high-risk samples,32,33 was likely composed of multiple small subgroups with diverse outcomes, not all of which were detectable at 3 years of age.

MARBLES collected information on vitamin use by month, which allows for greater characterization of timing than the information collected in most ASD studies. The timing implicated in this study is congruent with previous studies showing that the periconceptional period could be particularly critical in ASD origin and prenatal vitamin use.6,7,9 The association between prenatal vitamin use and reduced ASD risk could be owing to any of the many nutrients contained in these vitamins. As suggested in previous studies, iron and especially folic acid are likely contributors given their high content in prenatal vitamins (compared with multivitamins, which were not associated with lower ASD risk), their importance in neurodevelopment, their depletion during pregnancy, and (especially for folic acid) the timing of their effect.7-9,34

We observed modest evidence of a dose trend in which the highest tertile of mean daily folic acid supplementation was associated with a slightly greater reduction in ASD RR. Folic acid intake above the amount recommended for pregnancy (≥600 μg) was associated with lower ASD risk. Multivitamins, which typically contain less than 400 μg, were not associated with this decreased ASD risk, which implies that the association between prenatal vitamin use and ASD in high-risk siblings could, at least partly, be attributed to the high folic acid content in prenatal vitamins, as has been previously observed in general population studies,8,9,14 but other nutrients like iron could also be playing a role.34 The inverse association with folic acid intake compared with iron intake appeared more specific to ASD, which could suggest differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes by nutrient content in prenatal vitamin supplements. Because intake of folic acid and iron was highly correlated, our ability to examine their independent effects was limited; however, when we examined the intake of these 2 nutrients above the recommended dose for the first pregnancy month in the same model, the folic acid association remained statistically significant. Additional studies are needed to more rigorously investigate which nutrient might be driving the association with reduced ASD risk.

Limitations

This study has limitations. Even though the proportion of younger siblings with ASD was consistent with that of the much larger sample in the Baby Siblings Research Consortium,17 the sample size in this study provided limited power to detect differences across strata; however, to our knowledge, this research is the largest study to date that enrolled high-risk siblings before or during pregnancy and that prospectively collected supplement intake. This study assessed many potential confounding factors; however, the possibility of residual confounding or confounding by unmeasured factors cannot be ruled out. Prenatal vitamin use starting before pregnancy as recommended is a potential marker of good health literacy and is associated with many health-conscious behaviors, such as maternal exercise and avoidance of toxicant exposures that could decrease background ASD risk in children.

Conclusions

This study is the first to our knowledge to suggest that maternal prenatal vitamin intake during the first month of pregnancy may reduce ASD recurrence by half in younger siblings of children with ASD in high-risk families. These findings, if replicated, could have important public health implications for affected families. Future work should examine the contributions of specific nutrients from supplements as well as food sources, overall diet quality, and biologic measurements of nutrient status, as well as investigate dose thresholds, interactions with genetic variants, and potential mechanisms.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: October 18, 2018.

Corresponding Author: Rebecca J. Schmidt, PhD, Department of Public Health Sciences, School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, One Shields Ave, 123 Medical Sciences 1C, Davis, CA 95616-8638 (rjschmidt@ucdavis.edu).

Published Online: February 27, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3901

Author Contributions: Dr Iosif and Ms Guerrero Angel had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Schmidt, Ozonoff.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: All authors.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Schmidt, Iosif.

Obtained funding: Schmidt, Ozonoff.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Schmidt, Guerrero Angel.

Supervision: Schmidt, Ozonoff.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest. Dr Schmidt reported receiving lodging support for the Autism Speaks Baby Siblings Research Consortium meeting; travel and lodging support for invited talks at the University of Sherbrooke and at the University of California, Santa Cruz; lodging support for the Epigenomics 2016 meeting, the Neurotoxicity Society and International Neurotoxicology Association meeting, and the RISE (Rencontres internationales santé et environnement) 2017 Second International Meeting on Environmental Health; and grant funding to develop an online autism environmental questionnaire from Autism Speaks. Dr Iosif reported receiving honoraria as a peer reviewer for the US Department of Defense, as a member of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) ad hoc study section, and as a reviewer for Lancet Psychiatry. Dr Ozonoff has received research grant funding from the National Institutes of Health and Autism Speaks, travel reimbursement and honoraria for editorial activities from Autism Speaks, Autism Science Foundation, and Wiley, and book royalties from Guilford Press and American Psychiatric Press, Inc. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This study was funded by a grant from the Allen Foundation; pilot funding from the MIND Institute; STAR grant RD-83329201 from the US Environmental Protection Agency (EPA); and grants R24ES028533, R01ES028089, R01ES020392, R01ES025574, P01ES011269, and K12HD051958 from the NIH.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Disclaimer: The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official policy or position of the NIH or EPA.

Additional Contributions: We thank Irva Hertz-Picciotto, MPH, PhD, University of California, Davis, the original principal investigator of the Markers of Autism Risk in Babies: Learning Early Signs (MARBLES) study, for allowing access to the data used in this study.

References
1.
Baio  J, Wiggins  L, Christensen  DL,  et al.  Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2014.  MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;67(6):1-23. doi:10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Zuk  O, Hechter  E, Sunyaev  SR, Lander  ES.  The mystery of missing heritability: genetic interactions create phantom heritability.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(4):1193-1198. doi:10.1073/pnas.1119675109PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Hallmayer  J, Cleveland  S, Torres  A,  et al.  Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(11):1095-1102. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Risch  N, Hoffmann  TJ, Anderson  M, Croen  LA, Grether  JK, Windham  GC.  Familial recurrence of autism spectrum disorder: evaluating genetic and environmental contributions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171(11):1206-1213. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13101359PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Sandin  S, Lichtenstein  P, Kuja-Halkola  R, Larsson  H, Hultman  CM, Reichenberg  A.  The familial risk of autism.  JAMA. 2014;311(17):1770-1777. doi:10.1001/jama.2014.4144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Lyall  K, Schmidt  RJ, Hertz-Picciotto  I.  Maternal lifestyle and environmental risk factors for autism spectrum disorders.  Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(2):443-464. doi:10.1093/ije/dyt282PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Schmidt  RJ, Hansen  RL, Hartiala  J,  et al.  Prenatal vitamins, one-carbon metabolism gene variants, and risk for autism.  Epidemiology. 2011;22(4):476-485. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31821d0e30PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Schmidt  RJ, Tancredi  DJ, Ozonoff  S,  et al.  Maternal periconceptional folic acid intake and risk of autism spectrum disorders and developmental delay in the CHARGE (CHildhood Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment) case-control study.  Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(1):80-89. doi:10.3945/ajcn.110.004416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Surén  P, Roth  C, Bresnahan  M,  et al.  Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children.  JAMA. 2013;309(6):570-577. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.155925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Steenweg-de Graaff  J, Ghassabian  A, Jaddoe  VW, Tiemeier  H, Roza  SJ.  Folate concentrations during pregnancy and autistic traits in the offspring: the Generation R Study.  Eur J Public Health. 2015;25(3):431-433. doi:10.1093/eurpub/cku126PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Braun  JM, Froehlich  T, Kalkbrenner  A,  et al.  Brief report: are autistic-behaviors in children related to prenatal vitamin use and maternal whole blood folate concentrations?  J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2602-2607. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2114-xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Wang  M, Li  K, Zhao  D, Li  L.  The association between maternal use of folic acid supplements during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children: a meta-analysis.  Mol Autism. 2017;8:51. doi:10.1186/s13229-017-0170-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Vázquez  L I, Canals  J, Arija  V.  Review and meta-analysis found that prenatal folic acid was associated with a 58% reduction in autism but had no effect on mental and motor development  [published online November 22, 2018].  Acta Paediatr. doi:10.1111/apa.14657Google Scholar
14.
Levine  SZ, Kodesh  A, Viktorin  A,  et al.  Association of maternal use of folic acid and multivitamin supplements in the periods before and during pregnancy with the risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(2):176-184. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4050PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Virk  J, Liew  Z, Olsen  J, Nohr  EA, Catov  JM, Ritz  B.  Preconceptional and prenatal supplementary folic acid and multivitamin intake and autism spectrum disorders.  Autism. 2016;20(6):710-718. doi:10.1177/1362361315604076PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Hertz-Picciotto  I, Schmidt  RJ, Walker  CK,  et al.  A prospective study of environmental exposures and early biomarkers in autism spectrum disorder: design, protocols, and preliminary data from the MARBLES study.  Environ Health Perspect. 2018;126(11):117004. doi:10.1289/EHP535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Ozonoff  S, Young  GS, Carter  A,  et al.  Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study.  Pediatrics. 2011;128(3):e488-e495. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-2825PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Institute of Medicine Standing Committee on the Scientific Evaluation of Dietary Reference Intakes and Its Panel on Folate, Other B Vitamins, and Choline. Dietary Reference Intakes for Thiamin, Riboflavin, Niacin, Vitamin B6, Folate, Vitamin B12, Pantothenic Acid, Biotin, and Choline. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1998.
19.
Institute of Medicine Panel on Micronutrients. Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin A, Vitamin K, Arsenic, Boron, Chromium, Copper, Iodine, Iron, Manganese, Molybdenum, Nickel, Silicon, Vanadium, and Zinc. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2001.
20.
Lord  C, Risi  S, Lambrecht  L,  et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.  J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205-223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Lord  C, Rutter  M, DiLavore  PC, Risi  S.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2000.
22.
Mullen  EM.  Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services Inc; 1995.
23.
Ozonoff  S, Young  GS, Belding  A,  et al.  The broader autism phenotype in infancy: when does it emerge?  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(4):398-407.e2. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Chawarska  K, Shic  F, Macari  S,  et al.  18-Month predictors of later outcomes in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(12):1317-1327.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.09.015 doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.09.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Gotham  K, Pickles  A, Lord  C.  Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(5):693-705. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Lord  C, Rutter  M, DiLavore  PC, Risi  S, Gotham  K, Bishop  SL.  Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2). 2nd ed. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.
27.
Spiegelman  D, Hertzmark  E.  Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences.  Am J Epidemiol. 2005;162(3):199-200. doi:10.1093/aje/kwi188PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
van der Laan  MJ, Rose  S.  Targeted Learning: Causal Inference for Observational and Experimental Data. New York, NY: Springer Series in Statistics; 2011. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-9782-1
29.
Sandin  S, Lichtenstein  P, Kuja-Halkola  R, Hultman  C, Larsson  H, Reichenberg  A.  The heritability of autism spectrum disorder.  JAMA. 2017;318(12):1182-1184. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.12141PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Tordjman  S, Somogyi  E, Coulon  N,  et al.  Gene × environment interactions in autism spectrum disorders: role of epigenetic mechanisms.  Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:53. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Chaste  P, Leboyer  M.  Autism risk factors: genes, environment, and gene-environment interactions.  Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(3):281-292.PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Brian  J, Bryson  SE, Smith  IM,  et al.  Stability and change in autism spectrum disorder diagnosis from age 3 to middle childhood in a high-risk sibling cohort.  Autism. 2016;20(7):888-892. doi:10.1177/1362361315614979PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Shephard  E, Milosavljevic  B, Pasco  G,  et al; BASIS Team.  Mid-childhood outcomes of infant siblings at familial high-risk of autism spectrum disorder.  Autism Res. 2017;10(3):546-557. doi:10.1002/aur.1733PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Schmidt  RJ, Tancredi  DJ, Krakowiak  P, Hansen  RL, Ozonoff  S.  Maternal intake of supplemental iron and risk of autism spectrum disorder.  Am J Epidemiol. 2014;180(9):890-900. doi:10.1093/aje/kwu208PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×