Key PointsQuestion
Does exposure to maternal infection during pregnancy increase the long-term risk for major psychiatric disorders in the child?
Findings
In this Swedish population-based cohort study of children born between 1973 and 2014, exposure to infection in pregnancy significantly increased the risk for autism spectrum disorder and depression.
Meaning
Maternal infection during pregnancy may be responsible for some portion of autism and depression in childhood and adulthood among the exposed offspring.
Importance
The developmental origins of mental illness are incompletely understood. Although the development of autism and schizophrenia are linked to infections during fetal life, it is unknown whether more common psychiatric conditions such as depression might begin in utero.
Objective
To estimate the risk of psychopathologic conditions imparted from fetal exposure to any maternal infection while hospitalized during pregnancy.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A total of 1 791 520 Swedish children born between January 1, 1973, and December 31, 2014, were observed for up to 41 years using linked population-based registries. Children were excluded if they were born too late to contribute person-time, died before being at risk for the outcome, or were missing particular model data. Infection and psychiatric diagnoses were derived using codes from hospitalizations. Directed acyclic graphs were developed from a systematic literature review to determine Cox proportional hazards regression models for risk of psychopathologic conditions in the children. Results were evaluated using probabilistic and simple bias analyses. Statistical analysis was conducted from February 10 to October 17, 2018.
Exposures
Hospitalization during pregnancy with any maternal infection, severe maternal infection, and urinary tract infection.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Inpatient diagnosis of autism, depression, bipolar disorder, or psychosis among offspring.
Results
A total of 1 791 520 Swedish-born children (48.6% females and 51.4% males) were observed from birth up to age 41 years, with a total of 32 125 813 person-years. Within the directed acyclic graph framework of assumptions, fetal exposure to any maternal infection increased the risk of an inpatient diagnosis in the child of autism (hazard ratio [HR], 1.79; 95% CI, 1.34-2.40) or depression (HR, 1.24; 95% CI, 1.08-1.42). Effect estimates for autism and depression were similar following a severe maternal infection (autism: HR, 1.81; 95% CI, 1.18-2.78; depression: HR, 1.24; 95% CI, 0.88-1.73) or urinary tract infection (autism: HR, 1.89; 95% CI, 1.23-2.90; depression: HR, 1.30; 95% CI, 1.04-1.61) and were robust to moderate unknown confounding. Within the directed acyclic graph framework of assumptions, the relationship between infection and depression was vulnerable to bias from loss to follow-up, but separate data from the Swedish Death Registry demonstrated increased risk of suicide among individuals exposed to pregnancy infection. No evidence was found for increased risk of bipolar disorder or psychosis among children exposed to infection in utero.
Conclusions and Relevance
These findings suggest that fetal exposure to a maternal infection while hospitalized increased the risk for autism and depression, but not bipolar or psychosis, during the child’s life. These results emphasize the importance of avoiding infections during pregnancy, which may impart subtle fetal brain injuries contributing to development of autism and depression.
A large body of evidence indicates that particular infections in pregnancy lead to fetal brain injury, neurodevelopmental abnormalities, and an elevated lifelong risk for certain psychiatric disorders in the children.1-4 Early epidemiologic evidence for an association between infection in pregnancy and psychopathologic conditions was evident in Scandinavian studies on the risk of schizophrenia after exposure to influenza in utero3 and subsequent studies have demonstrated similar associations with other viral (measles, rubella, varicella-zoster, polio, and herpes), bacterial, and parasitic infections during pregnancy.5-7 Other studies found associations between maternal infection and development of bipolar disorder and autism.8-10 Although compelling, these studies have focused mainly on linking specific infections with a particular psychopathologic condition, with few exceptions,11 rather than determining a generalized effect of infection and inflammation during pregnancy on a broad spectrum of psychopathologic conditions.
Whether maternal infection and inflammation can alter fetal neurodevelopment to a degree that imparts risk for a broad spectrum of psychopathologic conditions across the child’s lifetime is unknown. Several mechanisms of neuronal injury have been postulated to occur in the developing brain as a result of inflammation and glial production due to activation of mast cells, microglia, and astrocytes.7-23 Maternal and fetal inflammatory responses to infection may alter fetal neurodevelopment, as suggested in some children with autism, who display a chronic state of inflammation in the periphery as well as in the brain.10,24-26 Furthermore, changes in placental serotonin production due to maternal inflammation have been suggested to lead to aberrant neurodevelopment.27 Finally, the idea that some psychiatric disorders have a common mechanism of pathogenesis is supported by recent evidence implicating shared molecular pathways of transcriptional dysregulation and a common polygenic origin for autism, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder.28,29
We hypothesized that fetal exposure to maternal infection or associated inflammation increases the future risk for the child of a broad scope of psychopathologic conditions such as autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, depression, and psychosis, including schizophrenia. We further hypothesized that the magnitude of risk for psychopathologic conditions in the child differs by the type and severity of the maternal infection.
We obtained data on all births between January 1, 1973, and December 31, 2014, in the Swedish population-based birth registry, which was linked to hospital inpatient, demographic, education, and death registries by each person’s unique identification number.30 Raw data included some births in 1972 and 2015 that were excluded. Statistics Sweden deidentified the data to maintain confidentiality; therefore, informed consent was not required. Ethical approval was obtained to link birth and registry data from the regional ethical review board of the University of Gothenburg (Gothenburg, Sweden; 437-15) and reciprocally at Seattle Children’s Hospital (Seattle, Washington; STUDY00000634).
Fetal exposure to maternal infection was defined using the Swedish International Classification of Diseases, Eighth Revision (ICD-8), International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision (ICD-9), and International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision (ICD-10) hospitalization codes. Three prespecified exposure categories were used in pregnant women hospitalized with a diagnosis of (1) any maternal infection (eTable 1 in the Supplement); (2) severe infections, which included sepsis, meningitis or encephalitis, pneumonia, influenza, pyelonephritis, or chorioamnionitis (eTable 2 in the Supplement); and/or (3) urinary tract infection (eTable 2 in the Supplement). We used both primary and secondary infection diagnosis codes that occurred during any hospitalization during pregnancy except the admission for delivery, because we could not determine that infection preceded the birth for this admission. There was one exception to this rule: we used a chorioamnionitis diagnosis from the delivery admission because chorioamnionitis could have only occurred before delivery.
Primary study outcomes were autism, bipolar disorder, depression, and psychosis (including schizophrenia), which were defined by Swedish ICD-8, ICD-9, and ICD-10 codes during an inpatient hospitalization (eTable 3 in the Supplement).
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We included all children born in Sweden from January 1, 1973, to December 31, 2014, for our models with depression, bipolar disorder, and psychosis (Figure 1). In the autism model, we included all children born in Sweden from January 1, 1987, to December 31, 2014, because 1987 coincided with the introduction of autism as a new diagnosis and implementation of ICD-9 coding. As diagnoses of psychopathologic conditions are unusual prior to certain ages, children were considered at risk for autism only after age 2 years, depression only after age 6 years, and psychosis and bipolar disorder only after age 10 years.
Statistical analysis was conducted from February 10 to October 17, 2018. All tests were 2-sided. We reviewed 12 264 abstracts and articles to develop a series of directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) to characterize the relationships among maternal infection during pregnancy, diagnosis of psychopathologic conditions in the child, and other important variables. These models were based on the best available evidence and subject area expertise when evidence was not available. We reviewed and approved the final DAG models (eAppendix and eFigures 1-12 in the Supplement).
On the basis of these DAGs, we used established analytic rules with the Dagitty web application31 and the R package (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) to determine whether a given variable should be considered as a confounder, collider, or neither a confounder nor collider. These tools were also used to establish the minimally sufficient adjustment sets of variables for regression to estimate the total effects of exposure to infection during fetal life on subsequent development of psychopathologic conditions in childhood and adulthood.32 If there was disagreement about a variable’s status, we performed the analysis with and without the variable to determine how the effect estimate changed (eg, maternal mental health; eTable 4 in the Supplement). Statistically equivalent DAG models were also evaluated for alternate frameworks.
According to best practice and consistent with study power, we split the data in half; one half was used for preliminary power analyses to determine whether there was sufficient power to examine infections by trimester as well as to examine asymptomatic bacteriuria and vaginal tract infections as separate categories. The second half of the data was used for 8 prespecified analyses with Bonferroni correction (type I error rate of 0.05 adjusted after correction to 0.0063). In the first 4 analyses, we investigated the relationship between any maternal infection and bipolar disorder, psychosis (including schizophrenia), autism, or depression. The final 4 analyses studied the effect of a severe maternal infection with the diagnosis of autism and depression or the effect of a maternal urinary tract infection (UTI) on the diagnosis of autism or depression. Based on the DAG models, we used Cox proportional hazards regression to generate hazard ratios (HRs). We ensured that the proportional hazards assumption was met using individual and global χ2 tests and examination of Schoenfeld residual plots. In most models, delivery year and maternal tobacco use violated the proportional hazards assumption; therefore, we estimated HRs with maternal tobacco use and 10-year birth epoch as strata. We also adjusted for maternal age, maternal asthma, maternal diabetes, and premature rupture of membranes. Other variables examined are shown in eFigures 1 to 12 in the Supplement. To account for nonindependence of siblings, we used robust sandwich variance estimators. Unadjusted cumulative hazards were also plotted for each exposure and outcome.
We used the following 2 methods of bias analysis to interrogate our results: probabilistic bias analysis to assess for an unknown confounder and simple bias analysis to assess for outcome misclassification (differential loss to follow-up) and exposure misclassification (inaccurate infection coding).33
Study Population and Cumulative Hazard Curves
We analyzed linked Swedish pregnancy and birth records for 4 278 146 neonates, as well as subsequent hospitalization records for the children up to 41 years after birth. We used half the data for preliminary power analyses and the second half (2 138 012 records) for the main analysis (Figure 1). Study population characteristics are reported in Table 1.34
To evaluate the risk of psychopathologic conditions in the child after fetal exposure to any maternal infection during hospitalization, unadjusted cumulative hazard curves were generated by infection status for each outcome (Figure 2). The risk for hospital admission with psychosis and bipolar disorder appeared to be similar between children exposed and not exposed in utero to any maternal infection during hospitalization (Figure 2A and B). However, compared with children not exposed in utero to maternal infection during hospitalization, the cumulative hazard for hospital admission with autism was significantly greater by age 7 years and the cumulative hazard for hospital admission with depression was significantly greater by age 21 years (Figure 2C and D). The number of outcomes, person-years, and unadjusted rate ratios for any maternal infection and risk for a neuropsychiatric disorder are presented in eTables 5 to 8 in the Supplement.
To determine if the type of maternal infection changed the risk of autism or depression, we modeled the risk of the psychopathologic condition imparted by a composite of severe maternal infections (sepsis, pneumonia, pyelonephritis, meningitis or encephalitis, influenza, and chorioamnionitis) vs UTI, which may represent a more limited infection. In unadjusted cumulative hazard curves, children or adults exposed to severe maternal infections during fetal life had similar magnitudes of increased risk for autism and depression compared with children exposed to a maternal UTI (Figure 3). The number of outcomes, person-years, and unadjusted rate ratios for severe maternal infections or UTI and risk for a neuropsychiatric disorder are presented in eTables 9 to 12 in the Supplement.
Cox Proportional Hazards Regression Cumulative
Directed acyclic graph models informed a minimally sufficient adjustment set for estimating the effect of infections during pregnancy with future risk of psychopathologic conditions in the child, which included 10-year epoch of delivery and specific maternal exposure variables (age, tobacco use, asthma, diabetes, and premature rupture of membranes). In Cox proportional hazards regression models, there was a 79% increased risk of an autism diagnosis (HR, 1.79; 95% CI, 1.34-2.40) and a 24% increased risk of a depression diagnosis (HR, 1.24; 95% CI, 1.08-1.42) among children and adults exposed to any maternal infection during pregnancy (Table 2 and eFigures 7-13 in the Supplement). There was no apparent increased risk of bipolar disorder (HR, 0.99; 95% CI, 0.71-1.38) or diagnoses of psychosis, including schizophrenia (HR, 1.14; 95% CI, 0.83-1.57), in childhood or adulthood after fetal exposure to maternal infection (eFigures 1-6 in the Supplement). Additional analyses with maternal mental health as a confounder did not yield significantly different effect estimates (eTable 4 in the Supplement). When we analyzed the risk for psychopathologic conditions by type of infection, we found similar magnitudes of increased risk for autism and depression regardless of whether the exposure was a severe maternal infection or UTI (Table 2). For example, the HR for hospital admission with an autism diagnosis in childhood or adulthood after exposure to infection during fetal life was 1.81 (95% CI, 1.18-2.78) for a maternal severe infection and 1.89 (95% CI, 1.23-2.90) for a maternal UTI.
We used 2 methods of bias analysis to evaluate result sensitivities to introduction of an unknown confounder and loss to follow-up bias. First, we used probabilistic bias analysis to introduce a hypothetical moderate confounder to determine the sensitivity of the results (eFigure 14 in the Supplement).33 However, measures of effect remained significant and only slightly decreased for all statistically significant results for the development of autism or depression.
Second, we used simple bias analysis33 to determine the effect of misclassification of outcome bias from loss to follow-up, which would occur when a child received a diagnosis of a psychopathologic condition (ie, autism) but was never admitted to the hospital during the study period. Inpatient admission was required in our study to capture diagnoses of psychopathologic conditions through the Swedish Inpatient Health Registry. Our findings suggest that a population prevalence of autism spectrum disorder between 2% and 3% would be required to negate these results based on a misclassification of outcome bias (eTable 13 in the Supplement). Although estimates of the prevalence of autism have increased over time, with the highest estimates of prevalence in Stockholm published in 2012 at 2.5%,35 prior studies in Sweden encompassing most of the study period estimated the prevalence of autism between 0.2% and 1%.36
For the outcome of depression, when we assumed a population prevalence of only 5%, there was no increased risk of depression among children and adults who were exposed to any maternal infection or UTI during fetal life (eTable 14 in the Supplement). Estimates of depression in Sweden are higher than 5%, suggesting that this analysis is vulnerable to a misclassification of outcome bias from loss to follow-up.37 To interrogate these results with external data, we used the Swedish National Death Registry to examine cumulative hazard curves for suicide among individuals who were exposed during fetal life to a maternal infection during hospitalization. As the National Death Registry is inclusive of the entire Swedish population, it is not vulnerable to bias from loss to follow-up like the inpatient registry. The cumulative hazard for death by suicide among adults exposed to infection during fetal life was significantly greater compared with unexposed individuals starting at age 21 years, which mirrored the results from the inpatient registry for depression (Figure 2E). Although the Cox proportional hazards regression models for depression using inpatient data were vulnerable to a misclassification of outcome bias, descriptive suicide data supported the results and were not subject to the same bias.
Finally, we did a series of analyses to examine whether our results were sensitive to a misclassification of exposure bias, which may occur when maternal infection is misdiagnosed or miscoded during pregnancy. We found that the risk ratios corrected for misdiagnosis or miscoding were larger than the original effect estimates for maternal infection on autism and depression, severe maternal infection (autism only), or maternal UTI (eTables 15-19 in the Supplement). This finding suggests that our results are robust to possible bias from misclassification of exposure.
The effect of infection during pregnancy on the fetal brain and risk for subsequent development of neuropsychiatric disorders is understudied. In the Swedish population, we found compelling evidence that fetal exposure to infection (or inflammation) when the mother was hospitalized increased the risk for the child of hospital admission with autism during childhood and adulthood. These effects were observed irrespective of whether the exposure was a maternal severe infection (sepsis, influenza, pneumonia, meningitis or encephalitis, chorioamnionitis, and pyelonephritis) or UTI during pregnancy. Bias analyses revealed that autism results were robust to adjustment for a moderate unknown confounder, but that the depression results were vulnerable to bias from our inability to capture diagnoses among those who were never admitted to the hospital after birth (loss to follow-up). However, separate descriptive data from the National Death Registry, which was not subject to loss to follow-up, supported the depression results by demonstrating an increased risk of suicide among adults who were exposed to infection during fetal life. We did not find convincing evidence that maternal infection during gestation increased the lifetime risk of bipolar disorder or psychosis, including schizophrenia. Overall, our findings provide evidence for a fetal origin of some portion of autism and depression across a spectrum of maternal infections in pregnancy.
The earliest evidence for a fetal origin of psychiatric disease came from correlations of birth season with the incidence of schizophrenia and later through associations with maternal influenza infection.3 Subsequent studies have yielded mixed results38,39 and recent evidence suggests that the relationship with inflammatory exposures in pregnancy may be complicated by genetic susceptibility for both schizophrenia and autism.29,40-42 Unlike prior epidemiologic studies, our work used a literature-based variable framework (DAG models) and descriptive cumulative hazard curves to demonstrate that maternal infection during pregnancy increased the risk not only for autism but also possibly for depression. A few studies have investigated the lifetime risk of depression for the child after exposure to particular infections, but they have yielded mixed results.43,44 Although vulnerable to possible loss to follow-up bias, our study provides suggestive evidence for a fetal origin for depression, with separate support using suicide data from the Swedish National Death Registry. Although little is known about the scientific basis to link aberrant fetal neurodevelopment with subsequent risk for depression, infection and inflammation in the pregnant mouse lead to alterations in placental serotonin production and dysgenesis of serotonergic neurons in the fetal brain.27 These new findings suggest an important possible biological basis for a fetal origin for depression and suicide.
Our results of increased risk of autism after fetal exposure to infection are consistent with other epidemiologic and animal studies, which suggest that inflammation during gestation alters brain architecture or transcriptional programs.1,8,11 Similar to results from another study from Sweden, we found no evidence that maternal infection increased the lifetime risk of psychosis or bipolar disorder.45 However, the descriptive cumulative hazard curve for admission with psychosis suggests that infection may increase the risk earlier, but not later, in the child’s lifetime. Our results cannot exclude the possibility of increased risk for psychopathologic conditions as a result of a dual “hit”: an inflammatory fetal brain injury on a background of genetic susceptibility.
Although we expected that fetal exposure to severe infection would increase the risk for psychopathologic conditions compared with limited infections such as UTIs, we did not find a difference in these models. The effect of UTIs on uterine and placental inflammation during pregnancy is unknown but may be sufficient to alter neuropsychiatric risk for the fetus. A few studies of UTIs in pregnancy have found increased risk of fetal morbidity and developmental delay.46 Although it is possible that the diagnosis of a UTI in hospitalized women may have reflected a more severe infection (eg, pyelonephritis), our findings suggest that further study is warranted to quantify the inflammatory effects of a UTI in pregnancy on the fetus.
Strengths and Limitations
A clear strength of the study was the conservative nature of our analysis and use of mechanistic DAG models to specify a priori the complicated relationships among maternal, paternal, perinatal, and psychiatric outcome variables. We also interrogated the sensitivity of our results to potential biases. Directed acyclic graph modeling is limited by the degree to which variables are included and relationships appropriately specified. Although the DAGs did not indicate that adjustment for socioeconomic status was required, further analysis adjusting for occupation, educational level, or income would have enriched the study; however, income variables were incomplete in our data set. As our study was restricted to pregnancies in Swedish women, it is possible that findings may not be generalizable to other populations. Finally, we acknowledge that data on maternal infection and psychiatric outcomes from birth and health registries were derived only from inpatient hospitalizations. Our results may not translate to infections diagnosed in the outpatient setting. Bias analyses and suicide data from the death registry suggest that our results are somewhat robust to the probable underestimate of psychiatric disorders in our study, which occurred when participants were not admitted to the hospital after birth (lost to follow-up).
Overall, we found evidence that exposure to maternal infection during fetal life increased the risk of autism and possibly of depression in the child within our DAG model assumptions. Although the individual risk appears to be small, the population effects are potentially large. Our findings amplify the urgency to better understand the role of maternal infection during pregnancy on fetal brain development and suggest that prevention of infection (eg, influenza vaccination) or anti-inflammatory therapies47,48 may be important strategies for the primary prevention of some portion of autism and depression.
Accepted for Publication: December 3, 2018.
Corresponding Author: Kristina M. Adams Waldorf, MD, Center for Innate Immunity and Immune Disease, Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Washington, PO Box 356460, Seattle, WA 91985 (adamsk@uw.edu).
Published Online: March 6, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0029
Author Contributions: Dr al-Haddad had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Drs Adams Waldorf and Sengpiel contributed equally to this work.
Concept and design: al-Haddad, Jacobsson, Chabra, Olson, Bernier, Adams Waldorf, Sengpiel.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: al-Haddad, Jacobsson, Modzelewska, Bernier, Enquobahrie, Hagberg, Östling, Rajagopal, Adams Waldorf, Sengpiel.
Drafting of the manuscript: al-Haddad, Hagberg, Rajagopal, Adams Waldorf.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: al-Haddad, Jacobsson, Chabra, Modzelewska, Olson, Bernier, Enquobahrie, Hagberg, Östling, Sengpiel.
Statistical analysis: al-Haddad, Modzelewska.
Obtained funding: Jacobsson, Chabra, Adams Waldorf, Sengpiel.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Jacobsson, Bernier, Enquobahrie, Hagberg, Östling, Rajagopal, Sengpiel.
Supervision: Jacobsson, Adams Waldorf, Sengpiel.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by Goljes minnesfond stiftelsen Sigurd och Elsa (LA2015-0121, Dr Sengpiel) and Fru Mary von Sydows, född Wijk, donationsfond (nr3615, Dr Sengpiel), the Agreement concerning research and education of doctors (ALFGBG-717501, ALFGBG-426411, Ms Modzelewska and Dr Jacobsson; Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Sahlgrenska Academy, Gothenburg, Sweden), the University of Washington Neonatal Biology Research Fund (Drs al-Haddad and Chabra), grant T32 GM008244 from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (Dr al-Haddad), grant R01 AI33976 from the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (Drs Rajagopal and Adams Waldorf), and the Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology at the University of Washington (Dr Adams Waldorf).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: Ylva Folkesson, Gothenberg University, Geena Gallardo, BA, University of Washington, and Sylvia Lago, MBA, Seattle Children’s Hospital, provided administrative assistance. Peggy Cruse, MLIS, Seattle Children’s Hospital, provided invaluable assistance with the literature review. Jan Hamanishi, University of Washington, provided technical assistance with preparation of the figures. Ann Vander Stoep, PhD, University of Washington, provided expertise and advice related to psychiatric epidemiology. None of the additional contributors were specifically compensated for their contributions, but they receive salaries from affiliated institutions for administrative and graphic support to the departments in which study investigators are employed.
12.Pratt
L, Ni
L, Ponzio
NM, Jonakait
GM. Maternal inflammation promotes fetal microglial activation and increased cholinergic expression in the fetal basal forebrain: role of interleukin-6.
Pediatr Res. 2013;74(4):393-401. doi:
10.1038/pr.2013.126PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Ni
L, Acevedo
G, Muralidharan
B, Padala
N, To
J, Jonakait
GM. Toll-like receptor ligands and CD154 stimulate microglia to produce a factor(s) that promotes excess cholinergic differentiation in the developing rat basal forebrain: implications for neurodevelopmental disorders.
Pediatr Res. 2007;61(1):15-20. doi:
10.1203/01.pdr.0000249981.70618.18PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Oskvig
DB, Elkahloun
AG, Johnson
KR, Phillips
TM, Herkenham
M. Maternal immune activation by LPS selectively alters specific gene expression profiles of interneuron migration and oxidative stress in the fetus without triggering a fetal immune response.
Brain Behav Immun. 2012;26(4):623-634. doi:
10.1016/j.bbi.2012.01.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Wu
WL, Adams
CE, Stevens
KE, Chow
KH, Freedman
R, Patterson
PH. The interaction between maternal immune activation and alpha 7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in regulating behaviors in the offspring.
Brain Behav Immun. 2015;46:192-202. doi:
10.1016/j.bbi.2015.02.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Hagberg
H, Gressens
P, Mallard
C. Inflammation during fetal and neonatal life: implications for neurologic and neuropsychiatric disease in children and adults.
Ann Neurol. 2012;71(4):444-457. doi:
10.1002/ana.22620PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Meyer
U, Murray
PJ, Urwyler
A, Yee
BK, Schedlowski
M, Feldon
J. Adult behavioral and pharmacological dysfunctions following disruption of the fetal brain balance between pro-inflammatory and IL-10–mediated anti-inflammatory signaling.
Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(2):208-221. doi:
10.1038/sj.mp.4002042PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Jyonouchi
H, Sun
S, Le
H. Proinflammatory and regulatory cytokine production associated with innate and adaptive immune responses in children with autism spectrum disorders and developmental regression.
J Neuroimmunol. 2001;120(1-2):170-179. doi:
10.1016/S0165-5728(01)00421-0PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Gandal
MJ, Haney
JR, Parikshak
NN,
et al; CommonMind Consortium; PsychENCODE Consortium; iPSYCH-BROAD Working Group. Shared molecular neuropathology across major psychiatric disorders parallels polygenic overlap.
Science. 2018;359(6376):693-697. doi:
10.1126/science.aad6469PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Textor
J, van der Zander
B, Gilthorpe
MS, Liskiewicz
M, Ellison
GT. Robust causal inference using directed acyclic graphs: the R package ‘dagitty’.
Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(6):1887-1894. doi:
10.1093/ije/dyw341PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Lash
T, Fox
MP, Fink
AK.
Applying Quantitative Bias Analysis to Epidemiologic Data. New York, NY: Springer US; 2009. doi:
10.1007/978-0-387-87959-8 36.Lundström
S, Reichenberg
A, Anckarsäter
H, Lichtenstein
P, Gillberg
C. Autism phenotype versus registered diagnosis in Swedish children: prevalence trends over 10 years in general population samples.
BMJ. 2015;350:h1961. doi:
10.1136/bmj.h1961PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Johansson
R, Carlbring
P, Heedman
Å, Paxling
B, Andersson
G. Depression, anxiety and their comorbidity in the Swedish general population: point prevalence and the effect on health-related quality of life.
PeerJ. 2013;1:e98. doi:
10.7717/peerj.98PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Blomström
Å, Karlsson
H, Gardner
R, Jörgensen
L, Magnusson
C, Dalman
C. Associations between maternal infection during pregnancy, childhood infections, and the risk of subsequent psychotic disorder—a Swedish cohort study of nearly 2 million individuals.
Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(1):125-133. doi:
10.1093/schbul/sbv112PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.McDermott
S, Daguise
V, Mann
H, Szwejbka
L, Callaghan
W. Perinatal risk for mortality and mental retardation associated with maternal urinary-tract infections.
J Fam Pract. 2001;50(5):433-437.
PubMedGoogle Scholar