[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Flow diagram of participants through the study. *Among the 63 trauma-exposed individuals, we excluded those with a history of pituitary adenoma that had been surgically removed (n = 1), loss of consciousness during escape owing to traumatic head injury or inhalation of toxic fumes or smoke (n = 5), and reported history of previous admission to a psychiatric hospital for alcohol-related problems (n = 1). For ethical reasons, 2 individuals who reported previous claustrophobic experiences during neck scans were also excluded. Sixteen individuals did not meet the diagnostic criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). We enrolled the remaining 38 trauma-exposed individuals who were diagnosed as having PTSD and were eligible for this study on the basis of inclusion and exclusion criteria. †One trauma-exposed individual could not undergo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) at time 1 because of dental braces. Images obtained from this person at times 2 and 3 were included in the analysis. ‡One MRI of a control group member was excluded from the analysis because of inadequate image quality. Images obtained at times 2 and 3 from this person were included in the analysis. Times since trauma are expressed as mean (SD).

Flow diagram of participants through the study. *Among the 63 trauma-exposed individuals, we excluded those with a history of pituitary adenoma that had been surgically removed (n = 1), loss of consciousness during escape owing to traumatic head injury or inhalation of toxic fumes or smoke (n = 5), and reported history of previous admission to a psychiatric hospital for alcohol-related problems (n = 1). For ethical reasons, 2 individuals who reported previous claustrophobic experiences during neck scans were also excluded. Sixteen individuals did not meet the diagnostic criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). We enrolled the remaining 38 trauma-exposed individuals who were diagnosed as having PTSD and were eligible for this study on the basis of inclusion and exclusion criteria. †One trauma-exposed individual could not undergo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) at time 1 because of dental braces. Images obtained from this person at times 2 and 3 were included in the analysis. ‡One MRI of a control group member was excluded from the analysis because of inadequate image quality. Images obtained at times 2 and 3 from this person were included in the analysis. Times since trauma are expressed as mean (SD).

Figure 2. 
Cortical thickness and statistical maps. A, Mean cortical thickness maps of trauma-exposed individuals (top row) and control group members (bottom row). B, Group differences in cortical thickness. Brain regions in orange indicate clusters of significant group differences, greater in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls, adjusting for age and sex and corrected for multiple comparisons at P < .01. Clusters, defined as contiguous vertices with significant group differences at corrected P < .01, were in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the left superior frontal cortex, and the left inferior frontal cortex. No regions were thinner in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls.

Cortical thickness and statistical maps. A, Mean cortical thickness maps of trauma-exposed individuals (top row) and control group members (bottom row). B, Group differences in cortical thickness. Brain regions in orange indicate clusters of significant group differences, greater in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls, adjusting for age and sex and corrected for multiple comparisons at P < .01. Clusters, defined as contiguous vertices with significant group differences at corrected P < .01, were in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the left superior frontal cortex, and the left inferior frontal cortex. No regions were thinner in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls.

Figure 3. 
Relationships between adjusted cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and improvement in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms of trauma-exposed individuals. The 3 panels demonstrate relationships between age- and sex-adjusted cortical thickness of the DLPFC region at time 1 and improvement in Clinician-Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Scale (CAPS) scores at times 1, 2, and 3. The DLPFC region is where trauma-exposed individuals had greater cortical thickness than control group members at time 1, shown in Figure 2 and 
eFigure 3.

Relationships between adjusted cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and improvement in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms of trauma-exposed individuals. The 3 panels demonstrate relationships between age- and sex-adjusted cortical thickness of the DLPFC region at time 1 and improvement in Clinician-Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Scale (CAPS) scores at times 1, 2, and 3. The DLPFC region is where trauma-exposed individuals had greater cortical thickness than control group members at time 1, shown in Figure 2 and eFigure 3.

Figure 4. 
Cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) by the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) genotype. We compared cortical thickness between control group members (n = 35) and trauma-exposed individuals with a methionine/valine (Met/Val) or a methionine/methionine (Met/Met) genotype (n = 19) or a valine/valine (Val/Val) genotype (n = 10). Controls did not differ in cortical thickness in this region by the BDNF genotype. There was a positive linear trend (P < .001 for trend), with trauma-exposed individuals with a Val/Val genotype having the greatest DLPFC cortical thickness, followed by those with a Met genotype and controls. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Cortical thickness is age and sex adjusted. The DLPFC region is where trauma-exposed individuals had greater cortical thickness than controls at time 1, shown in Figure 2 and 
eFigure 3.

Cortical thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) by the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) genotype. We compared cortical thickness between control group members (n = 35) and trauma-exposed individuals with a methionine/valine (Met/Val) or a methionine/methionine (Met/Met) genotype (n = 19) or a valine/valine (Val/Val) genotype (n = 10). Controls did not differ in cortical thickness in this region by the BDNF genotype. There was a positive linear trend (P < .001 for trend), with trauma-exposed individuals with a Val/Val genotype having the greatest DLPFC cortical thickness, followed by those with a Met genotype and controls. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Cortical thickness is age and sex adjusted. The DLPFC region is where trauma-exposed individuals had greater cortical thickness than controls at time 1, shown in Figure 2 and eFigure 3.

Figure 5. 
Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness differences between trauma-exposed individuals and control group members. A, Brain regions in orange indicate clusters of significant group differences, greater in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls, adjusting for age and sex and corrected for multiple comparisons at P < .01 at times 1, 2, and 3. No regions were thinner in trauma-exposed individuals than in controls. B, Estimated cortical thickness changes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) region (Figure 2 and 
eFigure 3), where significant between-group differences were present at time 1, for trauma-exposed individuals (blue line) and controls (gray line) from 156 scans of the cohort, which was followed up for 4.76 years. Regression lines for DLPFC cortical thickness changes from times 1 through 3 were fitted using mixed-effects regression modeling. Heavy blue and gray lines represent the fit of linear modeling in trauma-exposed individuals and controls, respectively; thin lines correspond to their fitted 95% confidence intervals. Blue tick marks above the fitted lines represent times since trauma of trauma-exposed individuals, whereas gray tick marks below the fitted lines represent those of controls. Estimates for cortical thickness of the DLPFC region were age and sex adjusted. Cortical thickness in the DLPFC region decreased in trauma-exposed individuals during the follow-up period of 4.76 years. This pattern was not observed in controls.

Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness differences between trauma-exposed individuals and control group members. A, Brain regions in orange indicate clusters of significant group differences, greater in trauma-exposed individuals relative to controls, adjusting for age and sex and corrected for multiple comparisons at P < .01 at times 1, 2, and 3. No regions were thinner in trauma-exposed individuals than in controls. B, Estimated cortical thickness changes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) region (Figure 2 and eFigure 3), where significant between-group differences were present at time 1, for trauma-exposed individuals (blue line) and controls (gray line) from 156 scans of the cohort, which was followed up for 4.76 years. Regression lines for DLPFC cortical thickness changes from times 1 through 3 were fitted using mixed-effects regression modeling. Heavy blue and gray lines represent the fit of linear modeling in trauma-exposed individuals and controls, respectively; thin lines correspond to their fitted 95% confidence intervals. Blue tick marks above the fitted lines represent times since trauma of trauma-exposed individuals, whereas gray tick marks below the fitted lines represent those of controls. Estimates for cortical thickness of the DLPFC region were age and sex adjusted. Cortical thickness in the DLPFC region decreased in trauma-exposed individuals during the follow-up period of 4.76 years. This pattern was not observed in controls.

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Participants
Table 2. Detailed Information on 3 DLPFC Clusters Where Trauma-Exposed Individuals Had Greater Cortical Thickness Than Controls at Time 1
Detailed Information on 3 DLPFC Clusters Where Trauma-Exposed Individuals Had Greater Cortical Thickness Than Controls at Time 1
1.
Satcher  D, Friel  S, Bell  R.  Natural and manmade disasters and mental health.  JAMA. 2007;298(21):2540-2542. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Galea  S, Nandi  A, Vlahov  D.  The epidemiology of post-traumatic stress disorder after disasters.  Epidemiol Rev. 2005;27:78-91. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
North  CS, Nixon  SJ, Shariat  S, Mallonee  S, McMillen  JC, Spitznagel  EL, Smith  EM.  Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing.  JAMA. 1999;282(8):755-762. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Breslau  N, Davis  GC, Andreski  P, Peterson  E.  Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(3):216-222. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Breslau  N, Kessler  RC, Chilcoat  HD, Schultz  LR, Davis  GC, Andreski  P.  Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: the 1996 Detroit Area Survey of Trauma.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(7):626-632. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Kessler  RC, Sonnega  A, Bromet  E, Hughes  M, Nelson  CB.  Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1048-1060. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Milad  MR, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK, Quirk  GJ.  Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders.  Biol Psychol. 2006;73(1):61-71. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Maren  S, Quirk  GJ.  Neuronal signalling of fear memory.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(11):844-852. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Rauch  SL, Shin  LM, Phelps  EA.  Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research—past, present, and future.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):376-382. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Karl  A, Schaefer  M, Malta  LS, Dörfel  D, Rohleder  N, Werner  A.  A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(7):1004-1031. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Shin  LM, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK.  Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071:67-79. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Milad  MR, Quinn  BT, Pitman  RK, Orr  SP, Fischl  B, Rauch  SL.  Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(30):10706-10711. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Phelps  EA, Delgado  MR, Nearing  KI, LeDoux  JE.  Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC.  Neuron. 2004;43(6):897-905. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Milad  MR, Pitman  RK, Ellis  CB, Gold  AL, Shin  LM, Lasko  NB, Zeidan  MA, Handwerger  K, Orr  SP, Rauch  SL.  Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1075-1082. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Ochsner  KN, Bunge  SA, Gross  JJ, Gabrieli  JD.  Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion.  J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(8):1215-1229. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Bremner  JD.  Functional neuroimaging in post-traumatic stress disorder.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2007;7(4):393-405. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Charney  DS.  Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(2):195-216. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Pitman  RK, Shin  LM, Rauch  SL.  Investigating the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder with neuroimaging.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 17):47-54. PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Yehuda  R, Flory  JD.  Differentiating biological correlates of risk, PTSD, and resilience following trauma exposure.  J Trauma Stress. 2007;20(4):435-447. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Bonne  O, Brandes  D, Gilboa  A, Gomori  JM, Shenton  ME, Pitman  RK, Shalev  AY.  Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(8):1248-1251. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
De Bellis  MD, Hall  J, Boring  AM, Frustaci  K, Moritz  G.  A pilot longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(4):305-309. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yehuda  R, LeDoux  JE.  Response variation following trauma: a translational neuroscience approach to understanding PTSD.  Neuron. 2007;56(1):19-32. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Feder  A, Nestler  EJ, Charney  DS.  Psychobiology and molecular genetics of resilience.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):446-457. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Koenigs  M, Huey  ED, Calamia  M, Raymont  V, Tranel  D, Grafman  J.  Distinct regions of prefrontal cortex mediate resistance and vulnerability to depression.  J Neurosci. 2008;28(47):12341-12348. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Mollica  RF, Lyoo  IK, Chernoff  MC, Bui  HX, Lavelle  J, Yoon  SJ, Kim  JE, Renshaw  PF.  Brain structural abnormalities and mental health sequelae in South Vietnamese ex-political detainees who survived traumatic head injury and torture.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(11):1221-1232. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Hartley  CA, Phelps  EA.  Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):136-146. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Anderson  BJ, Eckburg  PB, Relucio  KI.  Alterations in the thickness of motor cortical subregions after motor-skill learning and exercise.  Learn Mem. 2002;9(1):1-9. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Draganski  B, Gaser  C, Busch  V, Schuierer  G, Bogdahn  U, May  A.  Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training.  Nature. 2004;427(6972):311-312. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Draganski  B, Gaser  C, Kempermann  G, Kuhn  HG, Winkler  J, Büchel  C, May  A.  Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning.  J Neurosci. 2006;26(23):6314-6317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Kleim  JA, Chan  S, Pringle  E, Schallert  K, Procaccio  V, Jimenez  R, Cramer  SC.  BDNF val66met polymorphism is associated with modified experience-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex.  Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(6):735-737. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Cheeran  B, Talelli  P, Mori  F, Koch  G, Suppa  A, Edwards  M, Houlden  H, Bhatia  K, Greenwood  R, Rothwell  JC.  A common polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) modulates human cortical plasticity and the response to rTMS.  J Physiol. 2008;586(pt 23):5717-5725. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ho  BC, Milev  P, O’Leary  DS, Librant  A, Andreasen  NC, Wassink  TH.  Cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometric correlates of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(7):731-740. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Pezawas  L, Verchinski  BA, Mattay  VS, Callicott  JH, Kolachana  BS, Straub  RE, Egan  MF, Meyer-Lindenberg  A, Weinberger  DR.  The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology.  J Neurosci. 2004;24(45):10099-10102. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
McIntosh  AM, Moorhead  TW, McKirdy  J, Sussmann  JE, Hall  J, Johnstone  EC, Lawrie  SM.  Temporal grey matter reductions in bipolar disorder are associated with the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism.  Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(10):902-903. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Soliman  F, Glatt  CE, Bath  KG, Levita  L, Jones  RM, Pattwell  SS, Jing  D, Tottenham  N, Amso  D, Somerville  LH, Voss  HU, Glover  G, Ballon  DJ, Liston  C, Teslovich  T, Van Kempen  T, Lee  FS, Casey  BJ.  A genetic variant BDNF polymorphism alters extinction learning in both mouse and human.  Science. 2010;327(5967):863-866. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Yu  H, Wang  Y, Pattwell  S, Jing  D, Liu  T, Zhang  Y, Bath  KG, Lee  FS, Chen  ZY.  Variant BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects extinction of conditioned aversive memory.  J Neurosci. 2009;29(13):4056-4064. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
American Psychiatric Association.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
38.
First  MB, Gibbon  M, Spitzer  RL, Williams  JBW.  User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Research Version 2.0. New York: Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1996.
39.
Zhao  H, Li  Q, Pei  X, Zhang  Z, Yang  R, Wang  J, Li  Y.  Long-term ginsenoside administration prevents memory impairment in aged C57BL/6J mice by up-regulating the synaptic plasticity-related proteins in hippocampus.  Behav Brain Res. 2009;201(2):311-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Watanabe  CM, Wolffram  S, Ader  P, Rimbach  G, Packer  L, Maguire  JJ, Schultz  PG, Gohil  K.  The in vivo neuromodulatory effects of the herbal medicine ginkgo biloba.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(12):6577-6580. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Lee  TM, Guo  LG, Shi  HZ, Li  YZ, Luo  YJ, Sung  CY, Chan  CC, Lee  TM.  Neural correlates of traditional Chinese medicine induced advantageous risk-taking decision making.  Brain Cogn. 2009;71(3):354-361. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Yamaguchi  S, Matsubara  M, Kobayashi  S.  Event-related brain potential changes after Choto-san administration in stroke patients with mild cognitive impairments.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;171(3):241-249. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Wolfe  JW, Kimerling  R, Brown  PJ, Chrestman  KR, Levin  K.  Psychometric review of the Life Stressor Checklist−Revised.  In: Stamm  BH. ed.  Measurement of Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation. Lutherville, MD: Sidran Press; 1996.Google Scholar
44.
Blake  DD, Weathers  FW, Nagy  LM, Kaloupek  DG, Gusman  FD, Charney  DS, Keane  TM.  The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale.  J Trauma Stress. 1995;8(1):75-90. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Weathers  FW, Keane  TM, Davidson  JR.  Clinician-administered PTSD scale: a review of the first ten years of research.  Depress Anxiety. 2001;13(3):132-156. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hamilton  M.  A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23(1):56-62. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
 FreeSurfer software. FreeSurfer Web site. http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu. Accessed October 8, 2009
48.
Fischl  B, Sereno  MI, Dale  AM.  Cortical surface-based analysis, II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system.  Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):195-207. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Fischl  B, Dale  AM.  Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(20):11050-11055. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Rosas  HD, Liu  AK, Hersch  S, Glessner  M, Ferrante  RJ, Salat  DH, van der Kouwe  A, Jenkins  BG, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington's disease.  Neurology. 2002;58(5):695-701. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Lyoo  IK, Sung  YH, Dager  SR, Friedman  SD, Lee  JY, Kim  SJ, Kim  N, Dunner  DL, Renshaw  PF.  Regional cerebral cortical thinning in bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(1):65-74. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Kuperberg  GR, Broome  MR, McGuire  PK, David  AS, Eddy  M, Ozawa  F, Goff  D, West  WC, Williams  SC, van der Kouwe  AJ, Salat  DH, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(9):878-888. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Salat  DH, Buckner  RL, Snyder  AZ, Greve  DN, Desikan  RS, Busa  E, Morris  JC, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging.  Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(7):721-730. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Han  X, Jovicich  J, Salat  D, van der Kouwe  A, Quinn  B, Czanner  S, Busa  E, Pacheco  J, Albert  M, Killiany  R, Maguire  P, Rosas  D, Makris  N, Dale  A, Dickerson  B, Fischl  B.  Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer.  Neuroimage. 2006;32(1):180-194. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Dickerson  BC, Fenstermacher  E, Salat  DH, Wolk  DA, Maguire  RP, Desikan  R, Pacheco  J, Quinn  BT, Van der Kouwe  A, Greve  DN, Blacker  D, Albert  MS, Killiany  RJ, Fischl  B.  Detection of cortical thickness correlates of cognitive performance: reliability across MRI scan sessions, scanners, and field strengths.  Neuroimage. 2008;39(1):10-18. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Hayasaka  S, Nichols  TE.  Validating cluster size inference: random field and permutation methods.  Neuroimage. 2003;20(4):2343-2356. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Hagler  DJ  Jr, Saygin  AP, Sereno  MI.  Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data.  Neuroimage. 2006;33(4):1093-1103. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Ward  BD.  Simultaneous inference for FMRI data. http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/manual/AlphaSim.pdf. Accessed October 25, 2009
59.
Kirk  RE.  Completely randomized design.  In: Kirk  RE. ed.  Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co; 1995.Google Scholar
60.
Gueorguieva  R, Krystal  JH.  Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the Archives of General Psychiatry Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(3):310-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Hennen  J.  Statistical methods for longitudinal research on bipolar disorders.  Bipolar Disord. 2003;5(3):156-168. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
West  BT, Welch  KB, Galecki  AT.  Linear Mixed Models: A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007.
63.
Rauch  SL, Wright  CI, Martis  B, Busa  E, McMullin  KG, Shin  LM, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  A magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical thickness in animal phobia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(9):946-952. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Lazar  SW, Kerr  CE, Wasserman  RH, Gray  JR, Greve  DN, Treadway  MT, McGarvey  M, Quinn  BT, Dusek  JA, Benson  H, Rauch  SL, Moore  CI, Fischl  B.  Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness.  Neuroreport. 2005;16(17):1893-1897. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Narr  KL, Toga  AW, Szeszko  P, Thompson  PM, Woods  RP, Robinson  D, Sevy  S, Wang  Y, Schrock  K, Bilder  RM.  Cortical thinning in cingulate and occipital cortices in first episode schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(1):32-40. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Shaw  P, Lerch  J, Greenstein  D, Sharp  W, Clasen  L, Evans  A, Giedd  J, Castellanos  FX, Rapoport  J.  Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):540-549. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Schaechter  JD, Moore  CI, Connell  BD, Rosen  BR, Dijkhuizen  RM.  Structural and functional plasticity in the somatosensory cortex of chronic stroke patients.  Brain. 2006;129(pt 10):2722-2733. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Kleim  JA, Barbay  S, Cooper  NR, Hogg  TM, Reidel  CN, Remple  MS, Nudo  RJ.  Motor learning-dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex.  Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;77(1):63-77. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Diamond  MC, Law  F, Rhodes  H, Lindner  B, Rosenzweig  MR, Krech  D, Bennett  EL.  Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to enriched environment.  J Comp Neurol. 1966;128(1):117-126. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Ochsner  KN, Gross  JJ.  The cognitive control of emotion.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(5):242-249. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Anderson  MC, Ochsner  KN, Kuhl  B, Cooper  J, Robertson  E, Gabrieli  SW, Glover  GH, Gabrieli  JD.  Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories.  Science. 2004;303(5655):232-235. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Depue  BE, Curran  T, Banich  MT.  Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process.  Science. 2007;317(5835):215-219. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Goldin  PR, McRae  K, Ramel  W, Gross  JJ.  The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):577-586. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Phan  KL, Fitzgerald  DA, Nathan  PJ, Moore  GJ, Uhde  TW, Tancer  ME.  Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(3):210-219. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Wager  TD, Davidson  ML, Hughes  BL, Lindquist  MA, Ochsner  KN.  Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.  Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037-1050. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Parker  KJ, Buckmaster  CL, Justus  KR, Schatzberg  AF, Lyons  DM.  Mild early life stress enhances prefrontal-dependent response inhibition in monkeys.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(8):848-855. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Parker  KJ, Buckmaster  CL, Schatzberg  AF, Lyons  DM.  Prospective investigation of stress inoculation in young monkeys.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(9):933-941. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Katz  M, Liu  C, Schaer  M, Parker  KJ, Ottet  MC, Epps  A, Buckmaster  CL, Bammer  R, Moseley  ME, Schatzberg  AF, Eliez  S, Lyons  DM.  Prefrontal plasticity and stress inoculation-induced resilience.  Dev Neurosci. 2009;31(4):293-299. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Lyons  DM, Afarian  H, Schatzberg  AF, Sawyer-Glover  A, Moseley  ME.  Experience-dependent asymmetric variation in primate prefrontal morphology.  Behav Brain Res. 2002;136(1):51-59. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Cohen  H, Kaplan  Z, Kotler  M, Kouperman  I, Moisa  R, Grisaru  N.  Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):515-524. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Porto  PR, Oliveira  L, Mari  J, Volchan  E, Figueira  I, Ventura  P.  Does cognitive behavioral therapy change the brain? A systematic review of neuroimaging in anxiety disorders.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;21(2):114-125. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Stein  DJ.  Emotional regulation: implications for the psychobiology of psychotherapy.  CNS Spectr. 2008;13(3):195-198. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Rauch  SL, van der Kolk  BA, Fisler  RE, Alpert  NM, Orr  SP, Savage  CR, Fischman  AJ, Jenike  MA, Pitman  RK.  A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(5):380-387. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
Shin  LM, Kosslyn  SM, McNally  RJ, Alpert  NM, Thompson  WL, Rauch  SL, Macklin  ML, Pitman  RK.  Visual imagery and perception in posttraumatic stress disorder: a positron emission tomographic investigation.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(3):233-241. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
Shin  LM, McNally  RJ, Kosslyn  SM, Thompson  WL, Rauch  SL, Alpert  NM, Metzger  LJ, Lasko  NB, Orr  SP, Pitman  RK.  Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: A PET investigation.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):575-584. PubMedGoogle Scholar
86.
Siegle  GJ, Thompson  W, Carter  CS, Steinhauer  SR, Thase  ME.  Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features.  Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(2):198-209. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Phillips  ML, Drevets  WC, Rauch  SL, Lane  R.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception.  Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(5):504-514. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
LeDoux  JE.  The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala.  Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2003;23(4-5):727-738. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Banks  SJ, Eddy  KT, Angstadt  M, Nathan  PJ, Phan  KL.  Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(4):303-312. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Gorski  JA, Zeiler  SR, Tamowski  S, Jones  KR.  Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is required for the maintenance of cortical dendrites.  J Neurosci. 2003;23(17):6856-6865. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Hua  JY, Smith  SJ.  Neural activity and the dynamics of central nervous system development.  Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(4):327-332. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Huang  ZJ, Kirkwood  A, Pizzorusso  T, Porciatti  V, Morales  B, Bear  MF, Maffei  L, Tonegawa  S.  BDNF regulates the maturation of inhibition and the critical period of plasticity in mouse visual cortex.  Cell. 1999;98(6):739-755. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Egan  MF, Kojima  M, Callicott  JH, Goldberg  TE, Kolachana  BS, Bertolino  A, Zaitsev  E, Gold  B, Goldman  D, Dean  M, Lu  B, Weinberger  DR.  The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function.  Cell. 2003;112(2):257-269. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Chen  ZY, Patel  PD, Sant  G, Meng  CX, Teng  KK, Hempstead  BL, Lee  FS.  Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons.  J Neurosci. 2004;24(18):4401-4411. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Chen  ZY, Ieraci  A, Teng  H, Dall  H, Meng  CX, Herrera  DG, Nykjaer  A, Hempstead  BL, Lee  FS.  Sortilin controls intracellular sorting of brain-derived neurotrophic factor to the regulated secretory pathway.  J Neurosci. 2005;25(26):6156-6166. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Ninan  I, Bath  KG, Dagar  K, Perez-Castro  R, Plummer  MR, Lee  FS, Chao  MV.  The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impairs NMDA receptor–dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus.  J Neurosci. 2010;30(26):8866-8870. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Ganzel  BL, Kim  P, Glover  GH, Temple  E.  Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain.  Neuroimage. 2008;40(2):788-795. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Matthews  SC, Strigo  IA, Simmons  AN, O'Connell  RM, Reinhardt  LE, Moseley  SA.  A multimodal imaging study in U.S. veterans of Operations Iraqi and Enduring Freedom with and without major depression after blast-related concussion.  Neuroimage. 2011;54(suppl 1):S69-S75. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
99.
Pezawas  L, Meyer-Lindenberg  A, Drabant  EM, Verchinski  BA, Munoz  KE, Kolachana  BS, Egan  MF, Mattay  VS, Hariri  AR, Weinberger  DR.  5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(6):828-834. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
100.
Golarai  G, Ghahremani  DG, Whitfield-Gabrieli  S, Reiss  A, Eberhardt  JL, Gabrieli  JD, Grill-Spector  K.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory.  Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(4):512-522. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
101.
Kato  N, Kawata  M, Pitman  R.  PTSD: Brain Mechanisms and Clinical Implications. Tokyo, Japan: Springer Verlag; 2006.
102.
Rauch  SL, Whalen  PJ, Shin  LM, McInerney  SC, Macklin  ML, Lasko  NB, Orr  SP, Pitman  RK.  Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(9):769-776. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
103.
Armony  JL, Corbo  V, Clément  MH, Brunet  A.  Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1961-1963. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
104.
Bremner  JD, Vermetten  E, Vythilingam  M, Afzal  N, Schmahl  C, Elzinga  B, Charney  DS.  Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(6):612-620. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
105.
Hedges  DW, Woon  FLM.  Structural magnetic resonance imaging findings in posttraumatic stress disorder and their response to treatment: a systematic review.  Curr Psychiatry Rev. 2007;3(2):85-93.Google ScholarCrossref
106.
Landré  L, Destrieux  C, Baudry  M, Barantin  L, Cottier  JP, Martineau  J, Hommet  C, Isingrini  M, Belzung  C, Gaillard  P, Camus  V, El Hage  W.  Preserved subcortical volumes and cortical thickness in women with sexual abuse-related PTSD.  Psychiatry Res. 2010;183(3):181-186. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
107.
Woodward  SH, Schaer  M, Kaloupek  DG, Cediel  L, Eliez  S.  Smaller global and regional cortical volume in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1373-1382. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
108.
Geuze  E, Westenberg  HG, Heinecke  A, de Kloet  CS, Goebel  R, Vermetten  E.  Thinner prefrontal cortex in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder.  Neuroimage. 2008;41(3):675-681. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
109.
Rauch  SL, Shin  LM, Segal  E, Pitman  RK, Carson  MA, McMullin  K, Whalen  PJ, Makris  N.  Selectively reduced regional cortical volumes in post-traumatic stress disorder.  Neuroreport. 2003;14(7):913-916. PubMedGoogle Scholar
110.
Yamasue  H, Kasai  K, Iwanami  A, Ohtani  T, Yamada  H, Abe  O, Kuroki  N, Fukuda  R, Tochigi  M, Furukawa  S, Sadamatsu  M, Sasaki  T, Aoki  S, Ohtomo  K, Asukai  N, Kato  N.  Voxel-based analysis of MRI reveals anterior cingulate gray-matter volume reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder due to terrorism.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(15):9039-9043. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
111.
Corbo  V, Clément  MH, Armony  JL, Pruessner  JC, Brunet  A.  Size versus shape differences: contrasting voxel-based and volumetric analyses of the anterior cingulate cortex in individuals with acute posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(2):119-124. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
112.
Araki  T, Kasai  K, Yamasue  H, Kato  N, Kudo  N, Ohtani  T, Nakagome  K, Kirihara  K, Yamada  H, Abe  O, Iwanami  A.  Association between lower P300 amplitude and smaller anterior cingulate cortex volume in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: a study of victims of Tokyo subway sarin attack.  Neuroimage. 2005;25(1):43-50. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
113.
Chen  S, Xia  W, Li  L, Liu  J, He  Z, Zhang  Z, Yan  L, Zhang  J, Hu  D.  Gray matter density reduction in the insula in fire survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder: a voxel-based morphometric study.  Psychiatry Res. 2006;146(1):65-72. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
114.
Woodward  SH, Kaloupek  DG, Streeter  CC, Martinez  C, Schaer  M, Eliez  S.  Decreased anterior cingulate volume in combat-related PTSD.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(7):582-587. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
115.
Kitayama  N, Quinn  S, Bremner  JD.  Smaller volume of anterior cingulate cortex in abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2006;90(2-3):171-174. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
116.
Hakamata  Y, Matsuoka  Y, Inagaki  M, Nagamine  M, Hara  E, Imoto  S, Murakami  K, Kim  Y, Uchitomi  Y.  Structure of orbitofrontal cortex and its longitudinal course in cancer-related post-traumatic stress disorder.  Neurosci Res. 2007;59(4):383-389. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
117.
Kasai  K, Yamasue  H, Gilbertson  MW, Shenton  ME, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK.  Evidence for acquired pregenual anterior cingulate gray matter loss from a twin study of combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):550-556. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
118.
Koenigs  M, Huey  ED, Raymont  V, Cheon  B, Solomon  J, Wassermann  EM, Grafman  J.  Focal brain damage protects against post-traumatic stress disorder in combat veterans.  Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(2):232-237. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
119.
Chen  S, Li  L, Xu  B, Liu  J.  Insular cortex involvement in declarative memory deficits in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder.  BMC Psychiatry. 2009;9:39PubMed10.1186/1471-244X-9-39Google ScholarCrossref
120.
Rogers  MA, Yamasue  H, Abe  O, Yamada  H, Ohtani  T, Iwanami  A, Aoki  S, Kato  N, Kasai  K.  Smaller amygdala volume and reduced anterior cingulate gray matter density associated with history of post-traumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatry Res. 2009;174(3):210-216. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
121.
Nardo  D, Högberg  G, Looi  JC, Larsson  S, Hällström  T, Pagani  M.  Gray matter density in limbic and paralimbic cortices is associated with trauma load and EMDR outcome in PTSD patients.  J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(7):477-485. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
122.
Fornito  A, Whittle  S, Wood  SJ, Velakoulis  D, Pantelis  C, Yücel  M.  The influence of sulcal variability on morphometry of the human anterior cingulate and paracingulate cortex.  Neuroimage. 2006;33(3):843-854. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
123.
Winter  H, Irle  E.  Hippocampal volume in adult burn patients with and without posttraumatic stress disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(12):2194-2200. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
124.
Yamasue  H, Abe  O, Kasai  K, Suga  M, Iwanami  A, Yamada  H, Tochigi  M, Ohtani  T, Rogers  MA, Sasaki  T, Aoki  S, Kato  T, Kato  N.  Human brain structural change related to acute single exposure to sarin.  Ann Neurol. 2007;61(1):37-46. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
125.
Solomon  Z, Mikulincer  M.  Trajectories of PTSD: a 20-year longitudinal study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):659-666. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
126.
Galea  S, Vlahov  D, Resnick  H, Ahern  J, Susser  E, Gold  J, Bucuvalas  M, Kilpatrick  D.  Trends of probable post-traumatic stress disorder in New York City after the September 11 terrorist attacks.  Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(6):514-524. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
127.
Tsai  KY, Chou  P, Chou  FH, Su  TT, Lin  SC, Lu  MK, Ou-Yang  WC, Su  CY, Chao  SS, Huang  MW, Wu  HC, Sun  WJ, Su  SF, Chen  MC.  Three-year follow-up study of the relationship between posttraumatic stress symptoms and quality of life among earthquake survivors in Yu-Chi, Taiwan.  J Psychiatr Res. 2007;41(1-2):90-96. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
128.
Wu  HC, Chou  P, Chou  FH, Su  CY, Tsai  KY, Ou-Yang  WC, Su  TT, Chao  SS, Sun  WJ, Chen  MC.  Survey of quality of life and related risk factors for a Taiwanese village population 3 years post-earthquake.  Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(4):355-361. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
129.
North  CS, McCutcheon  V, Spitznagel  EL, Smith  EM.  Three-year follow-up of survivors of a mass shooting episode.  J Urban Health. 2002;79(3):383-391. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
130.
Breslau  N.  Outcomes of posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 17):55-59. PubMedGoogle Scholar
131.
LeDoux  JE.  Emotion circuits in the brain.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155-184. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
132.
Phelps  EA, LeDoux  JE.  Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior.  Neuron. 2005;48(2):175-187. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
133.
Sotres-Bayon  F, Cain  CK, LeDoux  JE.  Brain mechanisms of fear extinction: historical perspectives on the contribution of prefrontal cortex.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):329-336. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
134.
Preuss  TM.  Evolutionary specializations of primate brain systems.  In: Ravoso  MJ, Dagosto  M. eds.  Primate Origins: Adaptations and Evolution. New York, NY: Springer; 2007.Google Scholar
135.
Nieder  A.  Prefrontal cortex and the evolution of symbolic reference.  Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19(1):99-108. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
July 2011

The Neurobiological Role of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Recovery From Trauma: Longitudinal Brain Imaging Study Among Survivors of the South Korean Subway Disaster

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Psychiatry and Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Seoul National University College of Medicine and College of Natural Sciences (Drs Lyoo, Kim, and Hwang, Mr Bae, and Ms Kim), Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Ewha Womans University (Dr Kim), Department of Psychiatry, Catholic University of Korea School of Medicine (Dr Yoon), and Department of Psychiatry, Soonchunhyang University College of Medicine (Dr Hwang), Seoul, South Korea.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(7):701-713. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.70
Abstract

Context  A multiwave longitudinal neuroimaging study in a cohort of direct survivors of a South Korean subway disaster, most of whom recovered from posttraumatic stress disorder 5 years after trauma, provided a unique opportunity to investigate the brain correlates of recovery from a severe psychological trauma.

Objectives  To investigate region-specific brain mobilization during successful recovery from posttraumatic stress disorder by assessing cortical thickness multiple times from early after trauma to recovery, and to examine whether a brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene polymorphism was associated with this brain mobilization.

Design  Five-year follow-up case-control study conducted from 2003-2007.

Setting  Seoul National University and Hospital.

Participants  Thirty psychologically traumatized disaster survivors and 36 age- and sex-matched control group members recruited from the disaster registry and local community, respectively, who contributed 156 high-resolution brain magnetic resonance images during 3 waves of assessments.

Main Outcome Measures  Cerebral cortical thickness measured in high-resolution anatomic magnetic resonance images using a validated cortical thickness analysis tool and its prospective changes from early after trauma to recovery in trauma-exposed individuals and controls.

Results  Trauma-exposed individuals had greater dorsolateral prefrontal cortical (DLPFC) thickness 1.42 years after trauma (right DLPFC, 5.4%; left superior frontal cortex, 5.8%; and left inferior frontal cortex, 5.3% [all clusters, P  .01]) relative to controls. Thicknesses gradually normalized over time during recovery. We found a positive linear trend, with trauma-exposed individuals with a valine/valine genotype having the greatest DLPFC cortical thickness, followed by those with a methionine genotype and controls (P < .001 for trend). Greater DLPFC thickness was associated with greater posttraumatic stress disorder symptom reductions and better recovery.

Conclusion  The DLPFC region might play an important role in psychological recovery from a severely traumatic event in humans.

On February 18, 2003, a metropolitan subway train was set on fire by an arsonist at the Jungangno Station in Daegu, South Korea. One hundred ninety-two people perished and hundreds more were injured. After this tragedy, a substantial number of survivors have struggled to overcome the traumatic memories and to return to their normal lives. Five years after the disaster, most survivors showed significant improvement in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, echoing our innate mechanism to promote psychological recovery from a severely traumatic event.

An increased risk of exposure to extreme traumatic events, including terrorist attacks, natural disasters, accidents, physical assaults, torture, or military combats, has become a concern of modern life.1 A substantial number of trauma-exposed individuals develop PTSD. Core symptoms of PTSD include reexperience of trauma, avoidance of trauma-related stimuli, and hypervigilence.2-6 In a nationwide epidemiological study, individuals with PTSD showed varying clinical courses ranging from successful recovery to chronic PTSD, with recovery in about half the persons.6

Studies in rats have indicated the crucial role of the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex and their functional relationships in fear extinction learning.7,8 Results of functional and structural neuroimaging studies in patients with PTSD and studies in healthy volunteers with fear extinction paradigms reflect this model of PTSD neurocircuitry.9-14 More recently, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) have been implicated as having a role in fear extinction learning.7 Additional studies13,15 have suggested that the DLPFC could be pivotal in the conscious regulation of emotion to reduce fear responses,9 suggesting the role of the DLPFC in recovery from PTSD. However, to the best of our knowledge, no longitudinal imaging study, from trauma to recovery, has studied individuals with PTSD to identify brain correlates related to recovery.11,16-21

Understanding how the trauma-exposed brain responds and changes during recovery from trauma and identifying specific brain regions that are linked to efficient recovery will provide valuable information regarding the development of preventive measures for trauma-exposed individuals and more treatment options for patients with PTSD.22,23 A longitudinal brain imaging study in which trauma-exposed individuals undergo multiple assessments from trauma to recovery would be an efficient way to accomplish this goal.

Two recent studies24,25 in combat veterans and torture survivors retrospectively assessed trauma and head injury and evaluated current psychiatric symptoms with neuroimaging data acquisition. The findings suggested that lesions and deficits in the DLPFC of the brain may be related to unfavorable mental health outcomes, including depression and PTSD. The DLPFC has a wide array of reciprocal connections to the limbic structures, including the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex,26 allowing it to exert control over these structures. The altered structure and function of these areas have consistently been reported in individuals with PTSD.11,16-19 Therefore, the DLPFC may cautiously be hypothesized as playing an important role in recovery from PTSD by providing cognitive control over the dysregulated fear and anxiety circuit among the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus.23,26

More studies have shown that brain structure and function measurably change in response to external stimuli and internal requirements in adults and children.27-30 Among trauma-exposed individuals, a potential for greater cortical plasticity of the DLPFC in response to emotional distress may be an important factor in mobilizing the DLPFC function for strong and efficient emotional regulation. This plasticity thus provides resilience to or recovery from PTSD.23,26

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is a key molecule that mediates experience-dependent cortical plasticity30-34 and is abundantly expressed in the prefrontal cortex.31 Increasing evidence indicates that a single nucleotide polymorphism of the BDNF gene (rs6265) prodomain involving substitution of valine (Val) for methionine (Met) influences experience-dependent plasticity, with Val homozygotes having the greater plasticity. This evidence suggested that capacity for mobilizing the DLPFC in an effort to overcome trauma and subsequent PTSD potentially differs by BDNF polymorphism. Recent animal and human studies35,36 have also shown that efficiency in the extinction learning is associated with BDNF polymorphism.

To investigate the brain regions that were mobilized during individuals' active efforts to overcome trauma and following mental health sequelae including PTSD, we followed up direct survivors of the Daegu subway disaster soon after trauma through the course of recovery using multiple waves of clinical, neuropsychological, and high-resolution brain imaging measurements with acquisition of the genetic information.

We hypothesized that there would be early increases in the cortical thickness of the DLPFC in trauma-exposed individuals corresponding with successful improvement in PTSD symptoms, with greater increases associated with later recovery as well. We also hypothesized that the extent of greater thickness of the DLPFC early after trauma would differ by the BDNF genotype, with greater increases in Val homozygotes than in Met carriers.

METHODS
DISASTER

On February 18, 2003, a fire was started on one subway train and spread to another that was entering the station from the opposite direction. Twelve subway coaches from both trains were burned completely. Of about 450 passengers aboard these trains, 192 died and approximately 148 had various levels of injuries.

PARTICIPANTS AND TIME FRAME OF SERIAL ASSESSMENTS

A registry of the Daegu subway disaster survivors (n = 83) was established to monitor their mental and physical health problems. From these survivors, a longitudinal cohort of trauma-exposed individuals was derived. A detailed flow diagram of the progress of participants from the registry through study participation is presented in Author Figure 1 (available on the authors' Web site [http://bic.snu.ac.kr/supplement/2011/AuthorContent_Trauma.pdf]).

Baseline clinical assessment (time 0) for diagnosis and symptom severity of PTSD was conducted from 1.0 to 3.5 (mean [SD], 1.65 [0.74]) months after the disaster (Figure 1). We included trauma-exposed individuals aged 18 to 50 years who were passengers in the fire-caught coaches; who escaped without head trauma, smoke inhalation injury, loss of consciousness, or any burns and organ damage; and who met the DSM-IV criteria for PTSD37 because of this traumatic event as confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV.38 At study entry, we excluded individuals with any of the following: lifetime (before the subway disaster) Axis I psychiatric disorders, including PTSD and alcohol and other drug abuse; Axis II antisocial and borderline personality disorders; a history of or current major medical conditions, including hypertension and diabetes mellitus; clinically significant abnormal findings in physical examinations, routine laboratory tests, and electrocardiography; urinalysis findings positive for psychoactive drugs; a history of alternative and complementary medical treatments with herbal mixtures; contraindications to brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), including claustrophobia, pacemakers, and metal implants; and previous exposure to traumatic events that may potentially cause PTSD.

Exclusion criteria during the study period included the use of complementary and alternative medical treatments with herbal mixtures because some ingredients may exert unknown effects on brain structure and function (a detailed explanation is available in Author Methods 1 [available on the authors' Web site]).39-42 All trauma-exposed individuals were psychotropic naive at the time of trauma exposure. Four trauma-exposed individuals took anxiolytics and antidepressants during the early months after the trauma. Five trauma-exposed individuals were referred for treatment on the basis of predefined clinical referral criteria but did not take psychotropics or have regular psychotherapeutic sessions primarily owing to fear of being stigmatized. Consequently, 26 of 30 trauma-exposed individuals were psychotropic naive for their lifetimes, including the study period. Four trauma-exposed individuals who had taken psychotropics early after the trauma were psychotropic free for 6 months before the first wave of MRI scans and thereafter.

No control met clinical referral criteria or used psychotropics before or during the study period.

Among 63 candidates undergoing the initial screening, 38 trauma-exposed individuals (16 men and 22 women) were enrolled according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria and underwent assessment at time 0 (Figure 1). We excluded 8 individuals owing to the use of herbal mixtures between time 0 and time 1, leaving 30 trauma-exposed individuals at time 1.

Thirty-six age- and sex-matched healthy controls (14 men and 22 women) were recruited from the local community by time 1 for matching the time 1, 2, and 3 evaluations in the trauma-exposed individuals, with the same set of exclusion criteria as that for trauma-exposed individuals. This control cohort included 6 surplus individuals (3 men and 3 women) who were enrolled in consideration of the potential difficulties of following up healthy volunteers. Previous lifetime stressors were documented for all individuals using the Life Stressor Checklist–Revised43 (Author Methods 2 [available on the authors' Web site]).

Three serial sets of clinical and neuropsychological assessments and brain MRI scans were performed at approximately 1.3-year intervals during the 5-year study for trauma-exposed individuals and controls. Figure 1 provides a detailed timeline of individual assessments and the number of participants.

Symptom severity for PTSD and comorbid Axis I disorders were determined using the Clinician-Administered Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Scale (CAPS)44,45 and the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV, respectively. Reliability measures for all structured interviews are described in Author Methods 3 (available on the authors' Web site).

This study was approved by the Seoul National University Hospital institutional review board. All individuals provided written informed consent to participate in this longitudinal follow-up study at study entry and at each subsequent assessment.

Subject protection policies included clinical referrals for further evaluation and treatment if participants scored higher than 14 on the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale46 or showed suicidal ideation or clinical psychiatric syndromes. These evaluations were performed at each time point by independent psychiatrists who were not involved in the research project. Clinical referral during the study was not an exclusion criterion.

MRI ACQUISITION

Brain MRI scans were obtained on the same 3-T whole-body imaging system (Signa EXCITE; General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin). Sagittal T1-weighted images were acquired with the 3-dimensional inversion recovery spoiled gradient-echo pulse sequence (echo time [TE], 1.4 milliseconds; repetition time [TR], 5.7 milliseconds; inversion time [TI], 400 milliseconds; matrix, 256 × 256; field of view [FOV], 22 cm; flip angle, 20°; number of excitations [NEX], 1; section thickness, 0.7 mm; no skip). Axial T2-weighted images (TE, 118 milliseconds; TR, 3500 milliseconds; matrix, 256 × 192; FOV, 22 cm; flip angle, 90°; NEX, 3; section thickness, 5 mm; 1.5-mm skip) and fluid-attenuated inversion recovery axial images (TE, 145 milliseconds; TR, 9900 milliseconds; TI, 2250 milliseconds; matrix, 256 × 192; FOV, 22 cm; flip angle, 90°; NEX, 1; section thickness, 5 mm; 1.5-mm skip) were also obtained.

We used the same scanner with the same software version and initial hardware settings throughout the 5-year study period. Detailed scanner maintenance information and measures for securing consistent head positions throughout serial MRI scans are described in Author Methods 4 (available on authors' Web site).

Experienced neuroradiologists confirmed that no subject had any gross brain abnormality. Potential image acquisition errors including motion artifacts were also assessed and rated (Author Methods 4).

CORTICAL THICKNESS MEASUREMENT

We performed cortical surface reconstruction and cortical thickness measurement with cortical surface-based analysis. We used FreeSurfer tools,47 an automated procedure involving a series of steps that consisted of intensity normalization, skull stripping, segmentation of the cortical white matter, subsequent tessellation of gray/white matter boundaries, and smoothing and inflation of the surface.48,49 The deformable surface algorithm allows for detection of the gray/white matter boundaries and surfaces with submillimeter precision.49,50

At several junctures through the processing stream, data from each individual were visually inspected, manually corrected, and reinspected to ensure the accuracy by an experienced doctoral-level rater (S.J.Y.) who was blinded to the participant's identity. Each person's data were smoothed onto the surface tessellation using a kernel of 20 mm full-width at half-maximum and then resampled into a common spherical coordinate system.48 The detailed process has been described elsewhere.25,51

This technique used to measure cerebral cortical thickness was validated via histological50 and manual measurements,52,53 establishing excellent test-retest reproducibility.54,55 Reliability of the cortical thickness measurement was assessed in the following 2 ways: (1) interrater and intrarater reliabilities of measuring cortical thickness using the same set of brain images and (2) measuring cortical thickness of controls who underwent repeated scans at 2-week intervals (Author Methods 5 [available on the authors' Web site]).

SURFACE-BASED ANALYSIS OF CORTICAL THICKNESS

Surface-based between-group comparisons used general linear models adjusted for age and sex. Mean cerebral cortical thickness and intracranial volume were initially considered as potential covariates but were not included in the main analysis because models with or without these covariates produced similar results (eFigure 1A-C).

To correct for multiple comparisons, we used the cluster size inference by means of z Monte Carlo simulations implemented in the FreeSurfer software.56,57 This program is based on the AFNI program AphaSim.58 The Monte Carlo simulation creates multiple simulated z statistic maps and from them creates a distribution of cluster sizes. We tested clusters against an empirical null distribution of maximum cluster size across 10 000 iterations created using z-distributed data. For this study, the initial cluster-forming threshold was P < .01 (2-tailed). This determines the likelihood that a cluster of a certain size, observed for the main effects of group, would be found by chance.56,57

GENOTYPING OF BDNF GENE

All blood samples for genotyping were drawn after participants provided written informed consent for the procedure. We analyzed the BDNF polymorphism in duplicate. Genomic DNA was extracted from whole blood using the high-salt extraction technique. We performed polymerase chain reaction amplification of a 274–base pair fragment containing the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism (rs6265) using the forward primer 5′-TGATGACCATCCTTTTCCTT-3′ and the reverse primer 5′- CACTGGGAGTTCCAATGC-3′. Genotyping of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism used the single base primer extension assay with a multiplex kit (PRISM SnaPShot; Applied Biosystems, Inc, Foster City, California). Analysis was performed using the identification software (GeneMapper, version 4.0; Applied Biosystems, Inc).

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

We performed independent t tests and χ2 tests to compare continuous and categorical demographic or psychometric variables, respectively. Thickness in clusters at time 1 was adjusted for age and sex for further correlational analyses. Pearson correlation analyses were used to measure associations between the CAPS score changes and adjusted cortical thickness in clusters at time 1. Associations between the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and cortical thickness in clusters of group differences were examined using the test for linear trend.59

We evaluated changes in PTSD symptoms over time using the mixed-effects regression model to minimize the effects of missing data.60-62 The CAPS score was the dependent variable. Fixed factors were the time effect as the predictor of interest and age, sex, and baseline CAPS scores as covariates. Subject variable was included as a random effect, meaning that the individual time trajectories could vary randomly with the subject-specific coefficients.

The main effect of time on changes in the thickness of the DLPFC cluster was evaluated in the mixed-effects regression model, which included age and sex as fixed effects and subject as a random effect. We added a group × time interaction term to test whether the slopes of the DLPFC thickness changes differed by groups.

We constructed linear and quadratic models for time effects on the CAPS scores and the DLPFC thickness and compared these using the likelihood ratio test. The best-fitting model with the smallest number of terms was selected.

Two-tailed significance of P < .05 was considered as statistically significant. We analyzed data using commercially available software (STATA SE, version 11.0; StataCorp LC, College Station, Texas).

RESULTS
CHARACTERISTICS OF PARTICIPANTS

There were no significant differences in any of the baseline demographic characteristics between trauma-exposed and control groups (Table 1).

There were no significant differences in age, sex, handedness, or educational level between trauma-exposed and control groups at each time point or between subjects who completed all assessments and those who did part of the assessments. Details are provided in Author Table 1 (available on the authors' Web site).

Table 1 includes detailed demographic and clinical information, including CAPS scores and comorbid diagnoses at study entry. Further information on demographics and comorbidity during the study period is provided in Author Table 1 (available on the authors' Web site).

TRAJECTORIES OF PTSD SYMPTOMS

Of trauma-exposed individuals who met all the diagnostic criteria for PTSD at time 0, 23 of 30 (77%), 12 of 25 (48%), and 2 of 17 (12%) met the diagnostic criteria for PTSD at times 1, 2, and 3, respectively (Table 1). Reductions in PTSD symptoms, as assessed by changes in CAPS total scores, are shown in Table 1 and Author Figure 2 (available on the authors' Web site).

Mixed-model regression analyses showed that trauma-exposed individuals had overall and continuous reductions in CAPS scores throughout the study period. For the first model with a linear time term, the time effect was significant (linear time effect, z = −16.8 [P < .001]). For the second model with linear and quadratic time terms, the linear (z = −13.7 [P < .001]) and quadratic effects (z = 7.24 [P < .001]) were significant with improvement in model fitness.

CORTICAL THICKNESS GROUP DIFFERENCES AT TIME 1 AND PTSD SYMPTOM IMPROVEMENT

Maps showing mean cortical thicknesses of the trauma-exposed and control groups are provided in Figure 2A. Trauma-exposed individuals had greater cortical thickness in the following 3 DLPFC clusters (Figure 2B) relative to controls (corrected P < .01): the right DLPFC (5.4%; Cohen d effect size [ES] for group differences, 1.01), the left superior frontal cortex (5.8%; ES, 0.93), and the left inferior frontal cortex (5.3%; ES, 0.91). Information on cluster size, number of vertices, Talairach coordinates, cortical thickness, and cluster-level P values are shown in Table 2.

To ensure the robustness of our findings, we conducted 4 sensitivity analyses in reduced data sets, excluding individuals with prior sub–PTSD-level life stressors, trauma-exposed individuals who had a comorbid psychiatric diagnosis, those who had taken psychotropic medications, or individuals who were not able to complete all 3 assessments. Similar results of these analyses showed that the present results were not likely to be modulated by potential confounding factors (eFigure 1A and E-H [see also Author Results 1 on the authors' Web site]).

Functional significance of this greater DLPFC thickness was measured by testing the association between cortical thickness and performance levels on a set of relevant neuropsychological tests that measure executive function (Author Results 2 and Author Table 2 [available on the authors' Web site]). In brief, correlation analysis revealed that greater thickness of the DLPFC at time 1 was associated with better performance in relevant neuropsychological tests at time 1 (greater executive function domain score) among the trauma-exposed individuals (ρ = 0.45 [P = .02]) (eFigure 2).

For further analyses, the mean thickness of DLPFC clusters of significant group differences at time 1 was calculated by cluster size–weighted averaging of the thickness of 3 clusters where group differences were noted at corrected P < .01 (eFigure 3). This mean thickness of DLPFC clusters was then adjusted for age and sex. Auxiliary post hoc analyses were repeated with 3 individual DLPFC clusters and produced similar results (eFigure 4 and eFigure 5 [see also Author Figure 3 on the authors' Web site]). Analysis results with nonadjusted raw thickness of the DLPFC clusters are also presented in Author Figure 4 (available on the authors' Web site).

Thickness of the DLPFC region at time 1 was correlated with CAPS score decreases from time 0 to time 1 (r = −0.54 [P = .002]) (Figure 3) but not with CAPS scores at time 0 or at time 1 (r = 0.24 [P = .22] and r = −0.12 [P = .52], respectively) in trauma-exposed individuals.

Thickness of the DLPFC region at time 1 was also correlated with CAPS score decreases from time 0 to time 2 (r = −0.53 [P = .007]) and those from time 0 to time 3 (r = −0.72 [P = .002]) (Figure 3). Adjusted cortical thickness of the DLPFC region at time 1 was also correlated with CAPS score changes from time 1 to time 3 (r = −0.55 [P = .03]) (Author Results 3, Author Figure 5, and Author Figure 6 [available on the authors' Web site]).

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BDNF POLYMORPHISM AND DLPFC CORTICAL THICKNESS AT TIME 1

Genotype distribution for the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism did not deviate from the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium in trauma-exposed individuals and controls (trauma-exposed individuals, χ2 = 0.02 [P = .88]; controls, χ2 = 0.25 [P = .62]).

We tested the association between the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and DLPFC cortical thickness using the trend analyses. There was a significant linear trend toward greater cortical thickness at time 1; trauma-exposed individuals with a Val/Val genotype in the DLPFC region had greater thickness than those with a Met/Val or a Met/Met genotype, who had greater thickness than controls (< .001 for trend) (Figure 4). This linear trend remained unchanged when the depressive symptom severity as measured by the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale at time 1 was included as an additional covariate (P = .001 for trend).

In an effort to examine potential functional significance of the BDNF polymorphism, we explored how changes in CAPS scores and cortical thickness of the DLPFC region vary with the BDNF polymorphism (Author Results 4 [available on the authors' Web site]).

TRAJECTORIES OF CORTICAL THICKNESS CHANGES FROM TIMES 1 TO 3

The size of DLPFC clusters of group differences decreased over time, and there was no significant group difference in cortical thickness in the prefrontal cortical region at time 3 (Figure 5A). Estimated trajectories of mean cortical thickness in the DLPFC region during the study period are plotted in Figure 5B, Author Figure 3, and Author Figure 7 (available on the authors' Web site).

Linear mixed-model regression analyses have shown that trauma-exposed individuals had linear decreases in cortical thickness over time in the DLPFC region (z = −3.92 [P < .001]), whereas controls had no thickness changes in this region (z = −0.64 [P = .52]). A significant group × time interaction was noted in this DLPFC region (z = −2.07 [P = .04]). The extent of cortical thinning of the DLPFC over time was correlated with changes in CAPS scores (r = −0.66 [P = .006]).

COMMENT

This first longitudinal multiwave neuroimaging study of disaster survivors, who initially met the criteria for a PTSD diagnosis and had significant improvement in PTSD symptoms several years later, provides a unique opportunity to identify brain correlates that promote psychological recovery from severe trauma.

Our study demonstrated that disaster survivors early in the course of PTSD had greater cortical thickness in the DLPFC regions relative to controls and that, among survivors, Val homozygotes had greater DLPFC cortical thickness than Met carriers. This greater DLPFC thickness early after the trauma, which was associated with earlier improvement and later recovery, has gradually normalized to the level of controls. Taken together, greater DLPFC thickness early after the trauma in trauma-exposed individuals is likely to reflect disaster survivors' active use of DLPFC function to overcome trauma-related distress.

Although the cellular basis of changes in cortical thickness has yet to be established, increased cortical thickness may be attributed to the greater arborization of neurons, increased glial volume, or increased vacularization.12,63-66 Changes in cortical thickness during adulthood may also reflect experience-dependent cortical plasticity.27,64,67-69 The DLPFC might be the region that undergoes plastic changes during trauma-exposed individuals' active efforts to overcome the distress from the trauma.

DLPFC FUNCTION IN CONTROLLING NEGATIVE EMOTIONS

The role of the DLPFC in controlling negative emotions has been well documented in healthy individuals.23,26,70 Emotion-regulating cognitive strategies, such as reappraisal of negative events and suppression of unpleasant memories, are associated with increased DLPFC and decreased limbic activities.15,71-75 In nonhuman primates, mild and intermittent stress exposure helps develop resilience to severe stressors76,77 and increase the volume and thickness of the prefrontal cortex.78,79 In patients with PTSD, cognitive behavioral treatment and transcranial magnetic stimulations induce functional and structural changes in the prefrontal cortex.80-82 Thus, changes in DLPFC regions in disaster survivors are likely to reflect active use of cognitive resources of the DLPFC to control the maladaptive and overactive fear circuit.

When faced with traumatic reminders or in provoked conditions, exaggerated deactivation in the superior and inferior frontal gyrus has been noted in patients with PTSD,83-85 which is in line with our findings that greater cortical thickness in these cortical structures was associated with better control of the symptoms.

Recent functional neuroimaging studies have also suggested the potential role of the DLPFC in diminishing the exaggerated fear response in concert with the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which have consistently been implicated in fear extinction learning.9,13,15 Studies have suggested that the DLPFC has functional connections with the amygdala via the extensive connections between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala.86-89

BDNF VAL66MET POLYMORPHISM AND DLPFC PLASTICITY

In general, BDNF modulates experience-dependent synaptic plasticity and neuronal development.33,90-92 Homozygous Val carriers with the Val66Met BDNF polymorphism have greater plasticity relative to Met carriers via greater efficiency in activity-dependent BDNF release and intracellular trafficking.93,94 This common polymorphism influences the structure and function of the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, where BDNF is abundantly expressed.33,93

Our findings that Val homozygotes had greater DLPFC cortical thickness than Met carriers approximately 1 year after trauma and that greater cortical thickness was associated with greater CAPS score changes indicate that the Val66Met BDNF polymorphism may play an important role in mobilizing the DLPFC that controls negative emotions and facilitates extinction learning. As a result, Val homozygotes have more efficient recovery from PTSD.35,36

The DLPFC thickness did not differ between controls according to BDNF Val66Met gene polymorphism but, in trauma-exposed individuals, those with a Val/Val genotype had greater thickness than those carrying the Met allele. This finding may be understood in that this common genetic variation of BDNF can modulate experience-dependent synaptic plasticity and may become more important when there is a need for the cortical plasticity.30,31,35,94-96

GREATER DLPFC THICKNESS AND SUCCESSFUL RECOVERY FROM PTSD

Greater DLPFC thickness early after trauma strongly related to successful reductions in PTSD symptoms but was not associated with PTSD symptom severity at a specific time point. This finding indirectly supports the hypothesis that greater DLPFC thickness has an important role in the recovery of PTSD rather than demonstrating the consequences of the distress due to the disorder. This finding may also serve to help predict mental health outcomes among trauma-exposed individuals.

With gradual and substantial recovery from PTSD, trauma-exposed individuals were likely to have a gradually lessening need for DLPFC use. This finding may have been reflected in the gradual normalization of early greater DLPFC thickness. Moreover, the greater the extent of normalization of DLPFC thickness (ie, the extent of cortical thinning of the DLPFC), the greater the changes in PTSD symptoms. This finding supports the theory that active DLPFC recruitment, which normalized later in the course of recovery, may have induced greater symptom improvement.

STUDY LIMITATIONS

The present study focused on cortical structures and their association with recovery from PTSD. Thus, it does not provide information regarding the interplay between the DLPFC and subcortical structures of the amygdala and hippocampus. Functional neuroimaging data in addition to the structural neuroimaging data would have provided more comprehensive information97-100 on the potential role of the DLPFC in facilitating extinction learning and modulating the ventromedial prefrontal cortex–amygdala dynamics over time during recovery from PTSD.11,101-104

This longitudinal cohort of disaster survivors can be considered unique from other PTSD samples studied in most prior brain imaging studies.10,105 Most of the trauma survivors recovered during the 5 years after trauma in the present study compared with PTSD diagnoses confirmed in studies typically conducted many years after trauma. The interval from trauma to brain imaging (about 1 year after trauma for the first imaging session) was also shorter compared with previous studies (mostly several years after trauma). These differences can be important moderator variables that account for sources of seemingly dissimilar findings.

To our knowledge, 3 previous reports have analyzed cortical thickness in adult patients with PTSD.106-108 In those studies, sample characteristics were different from ours in that more than a decade had passed since the index trauma event. In addition, some of the participants had a history of alcohol or substance abuse, and trauma types were combat-related repeated trauma or sexual abuse.106-108 Sample heterogeneity across studies may also explain the inconsistency in findings among studies that examined cortical structures using voxel-based morphometry or volumetry,107,109-121 in which most commonly reported findings are ventromedial prefrontal cortical alterations. Corbo and colleagues111 raised the possibility that these observations stemmed from differences in the shape of the anterior cingulate cortex rather than from differences in gray matter volume,9 considering the high sulcal variabilities of the anterior cingulate and paracingulate cortex that are likely to undermine the reliability of morphometric analysis, especially when automated. 122

Brain imaging before trauma exposure or during the early weeks after trauma would have enabled us to rule out the possibility that greater thickness in the DLPFC may be a preexisting vulnerability factor for PTSD development. The time 1 neuroimaging assessment, however, was the earliest possible time for neuro imaging owing to administrative, legal, and subject protection issues in the present study. Greater cortical thickness at time 1 was correlated with earlier improvement and later recovery in PTSD symptoms and this greater cortical thickness has gradually decreased to the level of healthy controls during the recovery course from PTSD. Together, this may suggest that DLPFC thickness increased via the experience-dependent cortical plasticity during active use of DLPFC function to help overcome trauma-related distress.

Factors other than trauma exposure or subsequent PTSD may mimic some of the cortical thickness differences between the trauma-exposed and control groups. We tried to control or covary these potential confounding factors by excluding trauma-exposed individuals with head trauma, smoke inhalation injury, loss of consciousness, or any burns and organ damage (ie, potential toxic effects of fire) in addition to psychological trauma. We conducted extensive sensitivity analyses to covary out the potential confounders.123,124

A high attrition rate is a common and well-recognized issue in cohort studies of disaster victims.2 Attrition may undermine the credibility of longitudinal study findings, especially when it occurs in a biased way.125 In addition to using an appropriate statistical method to minimize the effects of the missing data,60,61 we conducted sensitivity analyses replacing missing data with last observations and produced similar results (eFigures 1A and H) and Author Results 5 (available on the authors' Web site). These findings indicate that missing observations are likely to have randomly occurred in the present study.61

IMPLICATIONS AND SUMMARY

The present findings have potential implications for people who are commonly exposed to serious levels of trauma, including firefighters, emergency rescuers, and combat personnel. Preemptive strategies to bolster DLPFC function of successful cognitive mobilization over traumatic experiences, including preventive stress inoculation measures, could prove beneficial for these personnel.

A large proportion of survivors from the subway disaster in this study had recovered from PTSD 5 years after the trauma. At follow-up, we did not estimate the prevalence of PTSD among all survivors from the disaster but only examined the individuals who were diagnosed as having PTSD at baseline and enrolled in the cohort. Therefore, our sample does not include patients who later developed PTSD, which can be very common in survivors of mass disasters. The present sample does not represent individuals with chronic PTSD, in which symptoms persist for several years or for whom multiple traumatic events were linked to the development of PTSD,126-130 but rather provided opportunities to observe changes in the brain and behavior in a situation in which most traumatized persons recovered from acute PTSD after a single traumatic event.

Fear acquisition, retention, and extinction models involving the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventromedial cortex are well established in mammals.131-133 Humans have the most developed DLPFC, where a substantial part of higher cognitive functions, including reappraisal of negative events and suppression of unpleasant memories, arises.134,135 The present findings concerning the role of the DLPFC in recovery from PTSD likely will provide complementary knowledge for translational research.

In summary, this 5-year study that followed up disaster survivors during the natural course of recovery from PTSD has shown that trauma-exposed individuals develop greater thickness in the DLPFC region and that this cortical plasticity reflects active mobilization of the DLPFC function during individuals' efforts to overcome trauma and predicts successful recovery. Interventions that enhance the DLPFC mobilization or increase the DLPFC cortical thickness may merit attention as potential preventive or therapeutic options in trauma-exposed individuals and PTSD patients.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: In Kyoon Lyoo, MD, PhD, MMS, Department of Psychiatry and Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Seoul National University College of Medicine and College of Natural Sciences, 28 Yongon-dong, Jongno-gu, 110-744 Seoul, South Korea (inkylyoo@gmail.com).

Submitted for Publication: September 5, 2010; final revision received and accepted January 5, 2011.

Author Contributions: Drs Lyoo and Kim contributed equally to this work. Drs Lyoo, Kim, Yoon, and Hwang had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Financial Disclosure: Dr Lyoo reports receiving research support from Eli Lilly and Company and Lundbeck.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant 2010K000814 from the Brain Research Center of the 21st Century Frontier Research Program funded by the Korean Ministry of Education, Science and Technology; by grant KRF-2008-220-E00021 funded by the National Research Foundation of Korea; and by an independent investigator award from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression.

Role of the Sponsors: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Previous Presentation: This study was presented in part at the Frontier Tech Fair of the Korean Ministry of Education, Science and Technology; November 2, 2009; Seoul, South Korea.

Additional Contributions: We acknowledge the invaluable contributions of the participants of this 5-year longitudinal follow-up study. Hyeonseok S. Jeong, BS, and Han Byul Cho, BS, provided technical assistance; Emily Kim, English language editing as a native speaker; and Perry F. Renshaw, MD, PhD, thoughtful insights and valuable discussions.

References
1.
Satcher  D, Friel  S, Bell  R.  Natural and manmade disasters and mental health.  JAMA. 2007;298(21):2540-2542. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Galea  S, Nandi  A, Vlahov  D.  The epidemiology of post-traumatic stress disorder after disasters.  Epidemiol Rev. 2005;27:78-91. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
North  CS, Nixon  SJ, Shariat  S, Mallonee  S, McMillen  JC, Spitznagel  EL, Smith  EM.  Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the Oklahoma City bombing.  JAMA. 1999;282(8):755-762. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Breslau  N, Davis  GC, Andreski  P, Peterson  E.  Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(3):216-222. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Breslau  N, Kessler  RC, Chilcoat  HD, Schultz  LR, Davis  GC, Andreski  P.  Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: the 1996 Detroit Area Survey of Trauma.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(7):626-632. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Kessler  RC, Sonnega  A, Bromet  E, Hughes  M, Nelson  CB.  Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1048-1060. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Milad  MR, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK, Quirk  GJ.  Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders.  Biol Psychol. 2006;73(1):61-71. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Maren  S, Quirk  GJ.  Neuronal signalling of fear memory.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(11):844-852. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Rauch  SL, Shin  LM, Phelps  EA.  Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research—past, present, and future.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):376-382. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Karl  A, Schaefer  M, Malta  LS, Dörfel  D, Rohleder  N, Werner  A.  A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(7):1004-1031. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Shin  LM, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK.  Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071:67-79. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Milad  MR, Quinn  BT, Pitman  RK, Orr  SP, Fischl  B, Rauch  SL.  Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(30):10706-10711. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Phelps  EA, Delgado  MR, Nearing  KI, LeDoux  JE.  Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC.  Neuron. 2004;43(6):897-905. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Milad  MR, Pitman  RK, Ellis  CB, Gold  AL, Shin  LM, Lasko  NB, Zeidan  MA, Handwerger  K, Orr  SP, Rauch  SL.  Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1075-1082. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Ochsner  KN, Bunge  SA, Gross  JJ, Gabrieli  JD.  Rethinking feelings: an fMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion.  J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(8):1215-1229. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Bremner  JD.  Functional neuroimaging in post-traumatic stress disorder.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2007;7(4):393-405. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Charney  DS.  Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(2):195-216. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Pitman  RK, Shin  LM, Rauch  SL.  Investigating the pathogenesis of posttraumatic stress disorder with neuroimaging.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 17):47-54. PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Yehuda  R, Flory  JD.  Differentiating biological correlates of risk, PTSD, and resilience following trauma exposure.  J Trauma Stress. 2007;20(4):435-447. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Bonne  O, Brandes  D, Gilboa  A, Gomori  JM, Shenton  ME, Pitman  RK, Shalev  AY.  Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(8):1248-1251. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
De Bellis  MD, Hall  J, Boring  AM, Frustaci  K, Moritz  G.  A pilot longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(4):305-309. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Yehuda  R, LeDoux  JE.  Response variation following trauma: a translational neuroscience approach to understanding PTSD.  Neuron. 2007;56(1):19-32. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Feder  A, Nestler  EJ, Charney  DS.  Psychobiology and molecular genetics of resilience.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):446-457. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Koenigs  M, Huey  ED, Calamia  M, Raymont  V, Tranel  D, Grafman  J.  Distinct regions of prefrontal cortex mediate resistance and vulnerability to depression.  J Neurosci. 2008;28(47):12341-12348. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Mollica  RF, Lyoo  IK, Chernoff  MC, Bui  HX, Lavelle  J, Yoon  SJ, Kim  JE, Renshaw  PF.  Brain structural abnormalities and mental health sequelae in South Vietnamese ex-political detainees who survived traumatic head injury and torture.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(11):1221-1232. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Hartley  CA, Phelps  EA.  Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):136-146. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Anderson  BJ, Eckburg  PB, Relucio  KI.  Alterations in the thickness of motor cortical subregions after motor-skill learning and exercise.  Learn Mem. 2002;9(1):1-9. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Draganski  B, Gaser  C, Busch  V, Schuierer  G, Bogdahn  U, May  A.  Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training.  Nature. 2004;427(6972):311-312. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Draganski  B, Gaser  C, Kempermann  G, Kuhn  HG, Winkler  J, Büchel  C, May  A.  Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning.  J Neurosci. 2006;26(23):6314-6317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Kleim  JA, Chan  S, Pringle  E, Schallert  K, Procaccio  V, Jimenez  R, Cramer  SC.  BDNF val66met polymorphism is associated with modified experience-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex.  Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(6):735-737. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Cheeran  B, Talelli  P, Mori  F, Koch  G, Suppa  A, Edwards  M, Houlden  H, Bhatia  K, Greenwood  R, Rothwell  JC.  A common polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene (BDNF) modulates human cortical plasticity and the response to rTMS.  J Physiol. 2008;586(pt 23):5717-5725. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ho  BC, Milev  P, O’Leary  DS, Librant  A, Andreasen  NC, Wassink  TH.  Cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometric correlates of brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met gene polymorphism in patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(7):731-740. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Pezawas  L, Verchinski  BA, Mattay  VS, Callicott  JH, Kolachana  BS, Straub  RE, Egan  MF, Meyer-Lindenberg  A, Weinberger  DR.  The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology.  J Neurosci. 2004;24(45):10099-10102. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
McIntosh  AM, Moorhead  TW, McKirdy  J, Sussmann  JE, Hall  J, Johnstone  EC, Lawrie  SM.  Temporal grey matter reductions in bipolar disorder are associated with the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism.  Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(10):902-903. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Soliman  F, Glatt  CE, Bath  KG, Levita  L, Jones  RM, Pattwell  SS, Jing  D, Tottenham  N, Amso  D, Somerville  LH, Voss  HU, Glover  G, Ballon  DJ, Liston  C, Teslovich  T, Van Kempen  T, Lee  FS, Casey  BJ.  A genetic variant BDNF polymorphism alters extinction learning in both mouse and human.  Science. 2010;327(5967):863-866. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Yu  H, Wang  Y, Pattwell  S, Jing  D, Liu  T, Zhang  Y, Bath  KG, Lee  FS, Chen  ZY.  Variant BDNF Val66Met polymorphism affects extinction of conditioned aversive memory.  J Neurosci. 2009;29(13):4056-4064. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
American Psychiatric Association.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
38.
First  MB, Gibbon  M, Spitzer  RL, Williams  JBW.  User's Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Research Version 2.0. New York: Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1996.
39.
Zhao  H, Li  Q, Pei  X, Zhang  Z, Yang  R, Wang  J, Li  Y.  Long-term ginsenoside administration prevents memory impairment in aged C57BL/6J mice by up-regulating the synaptic plasticity-related proteins in hippocampus.  Behav Brain Res. 2009;201(2):311-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Watanabe  CM, Wolffram  S, Ader  P, Rimbach  G, Packer  L, Maguire  JJ, Schultz  PG, Gohil  K.  The in vivo neuromodulatory effects of the herbal medicine ginkgo biloba.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(12):6577-6580. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Lee  TM, Guo  LG, Shi  HZ, Li  YZ, Luo  YJ, Sung  CY, Chan  CC, Lee  TM.  Neural correlates of traditional Chinese medicine induced advantageous risk-taking decision making.  Brain Cogn. 2009;71(3):354-361. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Yamaguchi  S, Matsubara  M, Kobayashi  S.  Event-related brain potential changes after Choto-san administration in stroke patients with mild cognitive impairments.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;171(3):241-249. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Wolfe  JW, Kimerling  R, Brown  PJ, Chrestman  KR, Levin  K.  Psychometric review of the Life Stressor Checklist−Revised.  In: Stamm  BH. ed.  Measurement of Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation. Lutherville, MD: Sidran Press; 1996.Google Scholar
44.
Blake  DD, Weathers  FW, Nagy  LM, Kaloupek  DG, Gusman  FD, Charney  DS, Keane  TM.  The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale.  J Trauma Stress. 1995;8(1):75-90. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Weathers  FW, Keane  TM, Davidson  JR.  Clinician-administered PTSD scale: a review of the first ten years of research.  Depress Anxiety. 2001;13(3):132-156. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hamilton  M.  A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23(1):56-62. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
 FreeSurfer software. FreeSurfer Web site. http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu. Accessed October 8, 2009
48.
Fischl  B, Sereno  MI, Dale  AM.  Cortical surface-based analysis, II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system.  Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):195-207. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Fischl  B, Dale  AM.  Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(20):11050-11055. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Rosas  HD, Liu  AK, Hersch  S, Glessner  M, Ferrante  RJ, Salat  DH, van der Kouwe  A, Jenkins  BG, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington's disease.  Neurology. 2002;58(5):695-701. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Lyoo  IK, Sung  YH, Dager  SR, Friedman  SD, Lee  JY, Kim  SJ, Kim  N, Dunner  DL, Renshaw  PF.  Regional cerebral cortical thinning in bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2006;8(1):65-74. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Kuperberg  GR, Broome  MR, McGuire  PK, David  AS, Eddy  M, Ozawa  F, Goff  D, West  WC, Williams  SC, van der Kouwe  AJ, Salat  DH, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(9):878-888. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Salat  DH, Buckner  RL, Snyder  AZ, Greve  DN, Desikan  RS, Busa  E, Morris  JC, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging.  Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(7):721-730. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Han  X, Jovicich  J, Salat  D, van der Kouwe  A, Quinn  B, Czanner  S, Busa  E, Pacheco  J, Albert  M, Killiany  R, Maguire  P, Rosas  D, Makris  N, Dale  A, Dickerson  B, Fischl  B.  Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer.  Neuroimage. 2006;32(1):180-194. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Dickerson  BC, Fenstermacher  E, Salat  DH, Wolk  DA, Maguire  RP, Desikan  R, Pacheco  J, Quinn  BT, Van der Kouwe  A, Greve  DN, Blacker  D, Albert  MS, Killiany  RJ, Fischl  B.  Detection of cortical thickness correlates of cognitive performance: reliability across MRI scan sessions, scanners, and field strengths.  Neuroimage. 2008;39(1):10-18. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Hayasaka  S, Nichols  TE.  Validating cluster size inference: random field and permutation methods.  Neuroimage. 2003;20(4):2343-2356. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Hagler  DJ  Jr, Saygin  AP, Sereno  MI.  Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data.  Neuroimage. 2006;33(4):1093-1103. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Ward  BD.  Simultaneous inference for FMRI data. http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/manual/AlphaSim.pdf. Accessed October 25, 2009
59.
Kirk  RE.  Completely randomized design.  In: Kirk  RE. ed.  Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co; 1995.Google Scholar
60.
Gueorguieva  R, Krystal  JH.  Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the Archives of General Psychiatry Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(3):310-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Hennen  J.  Statistical methods for longitudinal research on bipolar disorders.  Bipolar Disord. 2003;5(3):156-168. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
West  BT, Welch  KB, Galecki  AT.  Linear Mixed Models: A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2007.
63.
Rauch  SL, Wright  CI, Martis  B, Busa  E, McMullin  KG, Shin  LM, Dale  AM, Fischl  B.  A magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical thickness in animal phobia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(9):946-952. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Lazar  SW, Kerr  CE, Wasserman  RH, Gray  JR, Greve  DN, Treadway  MT, McGarvey  M, Quinn  BT, Dusek  JA, Benson  H, Rauch  SL, Moore  CI, Fischl  B.  Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness.  Neuroreport. 2005;16(17):1893-1897. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Narr  KL, Toga  AW, Szeszko  P, Thompson  PM, Woods  RP, Robinson  D, Sevy  S, Wang  Y, Schrock  K, Bilder  RM.  Cortical thinning in cingulate and occipital cortices in first episode schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(1):32-40. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Shaw  P, Lerch  J, Greenstein  D, Sharp  W, Clasen  L, Evans  A, Giedd  J, Castellanos  FX, Rapoport  J.  Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):540-549. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Schaechter  JD, Moore  CI, Connell  BD, Rosen  BR, Dijkhuizen  RM.  Structural and functional plasticity in the somatosensory cortex of chronic stroke patients.  Brain. 2006;129(pt 10):2722-2733. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Kleim  JA, Barbay  S, Cooper  NR, Hogg  TM, Reidel  CN, Remple  MS, Nudo  RJ.  Motor learning-dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex.  Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;77(1):63-77. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Diamond  MC, Law  F, Rhodes  H, Lindner  B, Rosenzweig  MR, Krech  D, Bennett  EL.  Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to enriched environment.  J Comp Neurol. 1966;128(1):117-126. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Ochsner  KN, Gross  JJ.  The cognitive control of emotion.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(5):242-249. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Anderson  MC, Ochsner  KN, Kuhl  B, Cooper  J, Robertson  E, Gabrieli  SW, Glover  GH, Gabrieli  JD.  Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories.  Science. 2004;303(5655):232-235. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Depue  BE, Curran  T, Banich  MT.  Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process.  Science. 2007;317(5835):215-219. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Goldin  PR, McRae  K, Ramel  W, Gross  JJ.  The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):577-586. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Phan  KL, Fitzgerald  DA, Nathan  PJ, Moore  GJ, Uhde  TW, Tancer  ME.  Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(3):210-219. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Wager  TD, Davidson  ML, Hughes  BL, Lindquist  MA, Ochsner  KN.  Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.  Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037-1050. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Parker  KJ, Buckmaster  CL, Justus  KR, Schatzberg  AF, Lyons  DM.  Mild early life stress enhances prefrontal-dependent response inhibition in monkeys.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(8):848-855. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Parker  KJ, Buckmaster  CL, Schatzberg  AF, Lyons  DM.  Prospective investigation of stress inoculation in young monkeys.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(9):933-941. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Katz  M, Liu  C, Schaer  M, Parker  KJ, Ottet  MC, Epps  A, Buckmaster  CL, Bammer  R, Moseley  ME, Schatzberg  AF, Eliez  S, Lyons  DM.  Prefrontal plasticity and stress inoculation-induced resilience.  Dev Neurosci. 2009;31(4):293-299. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Lyons  DM, Afarian  H, Schatzberg  AF, Sawyer-Glover  A, Moseley  ME.  Experience-dependent asymmetric variation in primate prefrontal morphology.  Behav Brain Res. 2002;136(1):51-59. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Cohen  H, Kaplan  Z, Kotler  M, Kouperman  I, Moisa  R, Grisaru  N.  Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):515-524. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Porto  PR, Oliveira  L, Mari  J, Volchan  E, Figueira  I, Ventura  P.  Does cognitive behavioral therapy change the brain? A systematic review of neuroimaging in anxiety disorders.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;21(2):114-125. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Stein  DJ.  Emotional regulation: implications for the psychobiology of psychotherapy.  CNS Spectr. 2008;13(3):195-198. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Rauch  SL, van der Kolk  BA, Fisler  RE, Alpert  NM, Orr  SP, Savage  CR, Fischman  AJ, Jenike  MA, Pitman  RK.  A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(5):380-387. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
Shin  LM, Kosslyn  SM, McNally  RJ, Alpert  NM, Thompson  WL, Rauch  SL, Macklin  ML, Pitman  RK.  Visual imagery and perception in posttraumatic stress disorder: a positron emission tomographic investigation.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(3):233-241. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
Shin  LM, McNally  RJ, Kosslyn  SM, Thompson  WL, Rauch  SL, Alpert  NM, Metzger  LJ, Lasko  NB, Orr  SP, Pitman  RK.  Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: A PET investigation.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(4):575-584. PubMedGoogle Scholar
86.
Siegle  GJ, Thompson  W, Carter  CS, Steinhauer  SR, Thase  ME.  Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: related and independent features.  Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(2):198-209. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Phillips  ML, Drevets  WC, Rauch  SL, Lane  R.  Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception.  Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(5):504-514. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
LeDoux  JE.  The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala.  Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2003;23(4-5):727-738. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Banks  SJ, Eddy  KT, Angstadt  M, Nathan  PJ, Phan  KL.  Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(4):303-312. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Gorski  JA, Zeiler  SR, Tamowski  S, Jones  KR.  Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is required for the maintenance of cortical dendrites.  J Neurosci. 2003;23(17):6856-6865. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Hua  JY, Smith  SJ.  Neural activity and the dynamics of central nervous system development.  Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(4):327-332. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Huang  ZJ, Kirkwood  A, Pizzorusso  T, Porciatti  V, Morales  B, Bear  MF, Maffei  L, Tonegawa  S.  BDNF regulates the maturation of inhibition and the critical period of plasticity in mouse visual cortex.  Cell. 1999;98(6):739-755. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Egan  MF, Kojima  M, Callicott  JH, Goldberg  TE, Kolachana  BS, Bertolino  A, Zaitsev  E, Gold  B, Goldman  D, Dean  M, Lu  B, Weinberger  DR.  The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function.  Cell. 2003;112(2):257-269. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Chen  ZY, Patel  PD, Sant  G, Meng  CX, Teng  KK, Hempstead  BL, Lee  FS.  Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons.  J Neurosci. 2004;24(18):4401-4411. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Chen  ZY, Ieraci  A, Teng  H, Dall  H, Meng  CX, Herrera  DG, Nykjaer  A, Hempstead  BL, Lee  FS.  Sortilin controls intracellular sorting of brain-derived neurotrophic factor to the regulated secretory pathway.  J Neurosci. 2005;25(26):6156-6166. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Ninan  I, Bath  KG, Dagar  K, Perez-Castro  R, Plummer  MR, Lee  FS, Chao  MV.  The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism impairs NMDA receptor–dependent synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus.  J Neurosci. 2010;30(26):8866-8870. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Ganzel  BL, Kim  P, Glover  GH, Temple  E.  Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain.  Neuroimage. 2008;40(2):788-795. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Matthews  SC, Strigo  IA, Simmons  AN, O'Connell  RM, Reinhardt  LE, Moseley  SA.  A multimodal imaging study in U.S. veterans of Operations Iraqi and Enduring Freedom with and without major depression after blast-related concussion.  Neuroimage. 2011;54(suppl 1):S69-S75. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
99.
Pezawas  L, Meyer-Lindenberg  A, Drabant  EM, Verchinski  BA, Munoz  KE, Kolachana  BS, Egan  MF, Mattay  VS, Hariri  AR, Weinberger  DR.  5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(6):828-834. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
100.
Golarai  G, Ghahremani  DG, Whitfield-Gabrieli  S, Reiss  A, Eberhardt  JL, Gabrieli  JD, Grill-Spector  K.  Differential development of high-level visual cortex correlates with category-specific recognition memory.  Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(4):512-522. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
101.
Kato  N, Kawata  M, Pitman  R.  PTSD: Brain Mechanisms and Clinical Implications. Tokyo, Japan: Springer Verlag; 2006.
102.
Rauch  SL, Whalen  PJ, Shin  LM, McInerney  SC, Macklin  ML, Lasko  NB, Orr  SP, Pitman  RK.  Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(9):769-776. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
103.
Armony  JL, Corbo  V, Clément  MH, Brunet  A.  Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1961-1963. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
104.
Bremner  JD, Vermetten  E, Vythilingam  M, Afzal  N, Schmahl  C, Elzinga  B, Charney  DS.  Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(6):612-620. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
105.
Hedges  DW, Woon  FLM.  Structural magnetic resonance imaging findings in posttraumatic stress disorder and their response to treatment: a systematic review.  Curr Psychiatry Rev. 2007;3(2):85-93.Google ScholarCrossref
106.
Landré  L, Destrieux  C, Baudry  M, Barantin  L, Cottier  JP, Martineau  J, Hommet  C, Isingrini  M, Belzung  C, Gaillard  P, Camus  V, El Hage  W.  Preserved subcortical volumes and cortical thickness in women with sexual abuse-related PTSD.  Psychiatry Res. 2010;183(3):181-186. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
107.
Woodward  SH, Schaer  M, Kaloupek  DG, Cediel  L, Eliez  S.  Smaller global and regional cortical volume in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1373-1382. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
108.
Geuze  E, Westenberg  HG, Heinecke  A, de Kloet  CS, Goebel  R, Vermetten  E.  Thinner prefrontal cortex in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder.  Neuroimage. 2008;41(3):675-681. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
109.
Rauch  SL, Shin  LM, Segal  E, Pitman  RK, Carson  MA, McMullin  K, Whalen  PJ, Makris  N.  Selectively reduced regional cortical volumes in post-traumatic stress disorder.  Neuroreport. 2003;14(7):913-916. PubMedGoogle Scholar
110.
Yamasue  H, Kasai  K, Iwanami  A, Ohtani  T, Yamada  H, Abe  O, Kuroki  N, Fukuda  R, Tochigi  M, Furukawa  S, Sadamatsu  M, Sasaki  T, Aoki  S, Ohtomo  K, Asukai  N, Kato  N.  Voxel-based analysis of MRI reveals anterior cingulate gray-matter volume reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder due to terrorism.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(15):9039-9043. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
111.
Corbo  V, Clément  MH, Armony  JL, Pruessner  JC, Brunet  A.  Size versus shape differences: contrasting voxel-based and volumetric analyses of the anterior cingulate cortex in individuals with acute posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(2):119-124. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
112.
Araki  T, Kasai  K, Yamasue  H, Kato  N, Kudo  N, Ohtani  T, Nakagome  K, Kirihara  K, Yamada  H, Abe  O, Iwanami  A.  Association between lower P300 amplitude and smaller anterior cingulate cortex volume in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: a study of victims of Tokyo subway sarin attack.  Neuroimage. 2005;25(1):43-50. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
113.
Chen  S, Xia  W, Li  L, Liu  J, He  Z, Zhang  Z, Yan  L, Zhang  J, Hu  D.  Gray matter density reduction in the insula in fire survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder: a voxel-based morphometric study.  Psychiatry Res. 2006;146(1):65-72. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
114.
Woodward  SH, Kaloupek  DG, Streeter  CC, Martinez  C, Schaer  M, Eliez  S.  Decreased anterior cingulate volume in combat-related PTSD.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(7):582-587. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
115.
Kitayama  N, Quinn  S, Bremner  JD.  Smaller volume of anterior cingulate cortex in abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2006;90(2-3):171-174. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
116.
Hakamata  Y, Matsuoka  Y, Inagaki  M, Nagamine  M, Hara  E, Imoto  S, Murakami  K, Kim  Y, Uchitomi  Y.  Structure of orbitofrontal cortex and its longitudinal course in cancer-related post-traumatic stress disorder.  Neurosci Res. 2007;59(4):383-389. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
117.
Kasai  K, Yamasue  H, Gilbertson  MW, Shenton  ME, Rauch  SL, Pitman  RK.  Evidence for acquired pregenual anterior cingulate gray matter loss from a twin study of combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(6):550-556. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
118.
Koenigs  M, Huey  ED, Raymont  V, Cheon  B, Solomon  J, Wassermann  EM, Grafman  J.  Focal brain damage protects against post-traumatic stress disorder in combat veterans.  Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(2):232-237. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
119.
Chen  S, Li  L, Xu  B, Liu  J.  Insular cortex involvement in declarative memory deficits in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder.  BMC Psychiatry. 2009;9:39PubMed10.1186/1471-244X-9-39Google ScholarCrossref
120.
Rogers  MA, Yamasue  H, Abe  O, Yamada  H, Ohtani  T, Iwanami  A, Aoki  S, Kato  N, Kasai  K.  Smaller amygdala volume and reduced anterior cingulate gray matter density associated with history of post-traumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatry Res. 2009;174(3):210-216. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
121.
Nardo  D, Högberg  G, Looi  JC, Larsson  S, Hällström  T, Pagani  M.  Gray matter density in limbic and paralimbic cortices is associated with trauma load and EMDR outcome in PTSD patients.  J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(7):477-485. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
122.
Fornito  A, Whittle  S, Wood  SJ, Velakoulis  D, Pantelis  C, Yücel  M.  The influence of sulcal variability on morphometry of the human anterior cingulate and paracingulate cortex.  Neuroimage. 2006;33(3):843-854. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
123.
Winter  H, Irle  E.  Hippocampal volume in adult burn patients with and without posttraumatic stress disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(12):2194-2200. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
124.
Yamasue  H, Abe  O, Kasai  K, Suga  M, Iwanami  A, Yamada  H, Tochigi  M, Ohtani  T, Rogers  MA, Sasaki  T, Aoki  S, Kato  T, Kato  N.  Human brain structural change related to acute single exposure to sarin.  Ann Neurol. 2007;61(1):37-46. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
125.
Solomon  Z, Mikulincer  M.  Trajectories of PTSD: a 20-year longitudinal study.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):659-666. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
126.
Galea  S, Vlahov  D, Resnick  H, Ahern  J, Susser  E, Gold  J, Bucuvalas  M, Kilpatrick  D.  Trends of probable post-traumatic stress disorder in New York City after the September 11 terrorist attacks.  Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158(6):514-524. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
127.
Tsai  KY, Chou  P, Chou  FH, Su  TT, Lin  SC, Lu  MK, Ou-Yang  WC, Su  CY, Chao  SS, Huang  MW, Wu  HC, Sun  WJ, Su  SF, Chen  MC.  Three-year follow-up study of the relationship between posttraumatic stress symptoms and quality of life among earthquake survivors in Yu-Chi, Taiwan.  J Psychiatr Res. 2007;41(1-2):90-96. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
128.
Wu  HC, Chou  P, Chou  FH, Su  CY, Tsai  KY, Ou-Yang  WC, Su  TT, Chao  SS, Sun  WJ, Chen  MC.  Survey of quality of life and related risk factors for a Taiwanese village population 3 years post-earthquake.  Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(4):355-361. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
129.
North  CS, McCutcheon  V, Spitznagel  EL, Smith  EM.  Three-year follow-up of survivors of a mass shooting episode.  J Urban Health. 2002;79(3):383-391. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
130.
Breslau  N.  Outcomes of posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 17):55-59. PubMedGoogle Scholar
131.
LeDoux  JE.  Emotion circuits in the brain.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155-184. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
132.
Phelps  EA, LeDoux  JE.  Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior.  Neuron. 2005;48(2):175-187. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
133.
Sotres-Bayon  F, Cain  CK, LeDoux  JE.  Brain mechanisms of fear extinction: historical perspectives on the contribution of prefrontal cortex.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):329-336. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
134.
Preuss  TM.  Evolutionary specializations of primate brain systems.  In: Ravoso  MJ, Dagosto  M. eds.  Primate Origins: Adaptations and Evolution. New York, NY: Springer; 2007.Google Scholar
135.
Nieder  A.  Prefrontal cortex and the evolution of symbolic reference.  Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19(1):99-108. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×