Context Volumetric studies have reported relatively decreased cortical thickness and gray matter volumes in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) whose childhood status was retrospectively recalled. We present, to our knowledge, the first prospective study combining cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in adults diagnosed as having ADHD in childhood.
Objectives To test whether adults with combined-type childhood ADHD exhibit cortical thinning and decreased gray matter in regions hypothesized to be related to ADHD and to test whether anatomic differences are associated with a current ADHD diagnosis, including persistent vs remitting ADHD.
Design Cross-sectional analysis embedded in a 33-year prospective follow-up at a mean age of 41.2 years.
Setting Research outpatient center.
Participants We recruited probands with ADHD from a cohort of 207 white boys aged 6 to 12 years. Male comparison participants (n = 178) were free of ADHD in childhood. We obtained magnetic resonance images in 59 probands and 80 comparison participants (28.5% and 44.9% of the original samples, respectively).
Main Outcome Measures Whole-brain voxel-based morphometry and vertexwise cortical thickness analyses.
Results The cortex was significantly thinner in ADHD probands than in comparison participants in the dorsal attentional network and limbic areas (false discovery rate < 0.05, corrected). In addition, gray matter was significantly decreased in probands in the right caudate, right thalamus, and bilateral cerebellar hemispheres. Probands with persistent ADHD (n = 17) did not differ significantly from those with remitting ADHD (n = 26) (false discovery rate < 0.05). At uncorrected P < .05, individuals with remitting ADHD had thicker cortex relative to those with persistent ADHD in the medial occipital cortex, insula, parahippocampus, and prefrontal regions.
Conclusions Anatomic gray matter reductions are observable in adults with childhood ADHD, regardless of the current diagnosis. The most affected regions underpin top-down control of attention and regulation of emotion and motivation. Exploratory analyses suggest that diagnostic remission may result from compensatory maturation of prefrontal, cerebellar, and thalamic circuitry.
Volumetric studies in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have consistently found global reductions of total brain volume, with the prefrontal cortex, anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum, and parietotemporal regions particularly affected relative to typical development in healthy children.1-4 These findings are consistent with a model of ADHD as a disorder of frontal-striatal-cerebellar circuitry. The diagnosis of ADHD requires onset in childhood, but persistence of ADHD into adulthood is now well documented.4,5 This longitudinal course combined with smaller brain volumes in children with ADHD have raised questions about brain development into adulthood.
A sparse literature on brain anatomy in adults with ADHD also reports decreased volumes in the orbitofrontal cortex,6 anterior cingulate cortex (ACC),7,8 dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,7 superior frontal cortex, and cerebellum.9 Complementary analyses of cortical thickness10 reveal overall decreased cortical thickness in children10-13 and adults with ADHD, with reductions in ACC, medial frontal regions, and parietotemporo-occipital cortex.11-13 Recently, Almeida et al14 found cortical thinning in the right frontal lobe of children, adolescents, and adults with ADHD.
Investigations of structural brain abnormalities in adults, for want of a better method, have relied on adults' retrospective recall of their childhood status.6-9,15-19 The documented inaccuracies of such reports20 highlight the advantage of assessing brain anatomy in individuals with established childhood-onset ADHD prospectively followed up into adulthood. In addition, clinical ADHD remits in a substantial proportion of individuals followed up into adulthood21,22; however, to our knowledge, the neurobiological mechanisms of remission have not been previously examined in middle adulthood.
We report cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analyses in the largest sample to date of adults with childhood ADHD diagnoses (mean age, 8.3 years) consistent with DSM-IV. Follow-up assessments occurred at the mean ages of 18.4, 25.0, and 41.2 years (18FU, 25FU, and 41FU, respectively). At the 18FU, a comparison group free of childhood ADHD and matched for age, sex, ethnicity, and childhood social class was recruited.21,23-26 Systematic diagnostic assessments at each follow-up were conducted by interviewers blinded to history and group membership. At the 41FU, we conducted anatomic brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in probands with childhood ADHD and a comparison group. We performed analyses based on childhood diagnosis and current diagnostic status in adulthood. Our primary aims were (1) to test whether adults with a childhood diagnosis of combined-type ADHD (probands), relative to the comparisons group, exhibit cortical thinning and decreased gray matter (GM) volume in regions hypothesized to be related to ADHD11-13,17 and (2) to assess whether anatomic differences are associated with a current ADHD diagnosis.
The ADHD group originally included 207 white boys aged 6 to 12 years who were referred to a research clinic from 1970 to 1977 (mean age, 8.3 years). Briefly, they were referred by schools because of behavioral problems and had elevated parent and teacher ratings of hyperactivity, an IQ of at least 85, and a diagnosis of hyperkinetic reaction of childhood.27,28 Children with a pattern of aggressive or antisocial behavior were excluded to rule out comorbid conduct disorder. Further details of proband characteristics appear in previous publications.26,29 These participants underwent assessment at mean (SD) ages of 18.4 (1.3), 25.0 (1.3), and 41.2 (2.7) years. Comparison male participants (n = 178) were recruited at the 18FU. Medical center pediatric medical records were reviewed for children undergoing routine physical examinations from 1970 through 1977 at 6 through 12 years of age, group matched for the probands' race, childhood socioeconomic status, and geographical residence. Parents of suitable children (by then adolescents) were telephoned, informed of the study, and recruited, conditional on parent interest and no reported teacher complaints about their child's behavior in elementary school. The refusal rate was low (about 5%).
On average, 33 years after the initial childhood diagnosis, clinical data were obtained for 135 male probands (65.5% of the original sample, 70.3% of those living) and 136 male comparison participants (76.4% of those recruited in adolescence, 78.6% of those living). Major DSM-IV disorders and multiple aspects of function were assessed for the interval from the 25FU to the 41FU by trained clinicians blinded to all antecedent data. A special interview, the Assessment of Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, was developed for diagnosing DSM-IV ADHD in adults (the Instrument and a supplementary Description are available at the authors' Web site at http://www.AboutOurKids.org/Research/Research_Publications/Proal_et_al_2011). Current ADHD was defined as meeting DSM-IV criteria during the preceding 6 months. Participants were invited to take part in an anatomic MRI study. Owing to refusals and MRI exclusions (Table 1), we obtained MRIs in 59 ADHD probands and 80 comparison participants. Nearly all probands (57 [97%] of those scanned) were treated with methylphenidate hydrochloride in childhood from ages 6 to 12 years for an average of 2.2 years.30 eTable 1, available at the authors' Web site, gives further details of childhood medication treatment, including thioridazine hydrochloride.26) All participants provided written informed consent as approved by the New York University Langone School of Medicine institutional review board.
To test whether cortical thickness differed as a function of current ADHD, we subdivided probands into the following 3 subgroups: (1) those who met diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV ADHD at the 41FU (persistent ADHD group; 17 participants, including 7 with predominantly inattentive, 6 with predominantly hyperactive/impulsive, and 4 with combined-type ADHD); (2) those who did not (remitting ADHD group; 26 participants); and (3) those diagnosed as having ADHD not otherwise specified (ADHD-NOS group; 16 participants) (see the supplementary Methods available at the authors' Web site). The comparison group was dichotomized into individuals who did not meet criteria for any type of ADHD (non-ADHD comparison group; 57 participants) and those who were diagnosed with ADHD-NOS (comparison group with ADHD; 23 participants). Although all probands and all comparison participants were included in initial vertexwise and VBM analyses, subgroup analyses focused on current diagnostic status. Accordingly, probands and comparison participants with current ADHD-NOS, which is not well-defined and did not differ between groups (16 of 59 [27%] and 23 of 80 [29%], respectively), were excluded from subgroup analyses.
We obtained 41 anatomic T1-weighted images in 20 ADHD probands and 21 comparison participants using a scanner with an 8-channel head coil (3T Siemens Trio; Siemens Medical Solutions USA Inc, Malvern, Pennsylvania) and 98 scans in 39 ADHD probands and 59 comparison participants using a scanner with a single-channel head coil (3T Siemens Allegra; Siemens Medical Solutions USA Inc). Proportions did not differ significantly across scanners, (χ21 = 0.96, P = .33), with the following parameters: repetition time, 2100 milliseconds; flip angle, 12°; slice thickness, 1.5 mm; inversion time, 1100 milliseconds; matrix, 192 × 256; and field of view, 172.5 mm. The only parameter that differed was echo time, which was 3.87 milliseconds on the Trio scanner and 3.90 milliseconds on the Allegra scanner.
Structural MRIs were preprocessed through the fully automated CIVET–Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) pipeline.31-34 The initial preprocessing step was to mask MRI native images using an automated brain extraction method.35 Data were corrected for nonuniformity artifacts and registered to stereotactic space (MNI152) using a 9-parameter linear transformation. Voxelwise tissue type classification was performed using a neural network classifier followed by a partial volume estimation step.33,36
For VBM, the classified tissue maps were blurred with a gaussian kernel of 10 mm full width at half maximum. Cortical thickness measures were assessed using a fully automated algorithm that defines the distances between a set of vertices at the white matter (WM) surface and then expands outward to find the intersection with GM to generate surface meshes that represent WM and GM interfaces.37 A total of 40 962 linked vertices were calculated per hemisphere. Each individual cortical thickness map was blurred using a 30-mm surface-based diffusion-smoothing kernel to reduce noise while preserving anatomic location, as this method produces less volumetric blurring than the equivalent gaussian kernel.38
Global Cortical Thickness
We obtained a single global cortical thickness value for each participant by averaging across all 81 924 vertices. Linear regression models controlled for age at the time of imaging and for the scanner model (Trio vs Allegra).
Vertexwise and VBM Analyses
Following the study aims, group analyses tested for regional differences in cortical thickness and GM density between (1) all adults with a childhood diagnosis of combined-type ADHD and all comparison groups; (2) persistent ADHD vs the non-ADHD comparison group; (3) remitting ADHD vs the non-ADHD comparison group; and (4) participants with persistent vs those with remitting ADHD. For each comparison, we regressed cortical thickness at each of 81 924 vertices or whole-brain GM density on group, controlling for age at the time of imaging and for the scanner model. The software package mni.cortical.statistics (Brain Imaging Centre, MNI; http://www.bic.mni.mcgill.ca) for the R environment39 was used for cortical thickness analyses, and the FMRIB Software Library (available at http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk) tool Feat, for VBM. Results were thresholded using a false discovery rate (FDR) of 0.05.40,41 Maps of t statistics for group effects on cortical thickness at each vertex or GM density at each voxel were projected onto an average brain template revealing clusters that differed significantly between groups. We retained clusters comprising at least 50 contiguous vertices for cortical thickness42 and 5 voxels for VBM.
Region-Based Analyses of Cortical Thickness and VBM
To test whether childhood or current ADHD was associated with significant differences in specific regions, we performed post hoc region-of-interest–based analyses. For each participant, we computed the mean cortical thickness or GM density within each cluster exhibiting significant (FDR < 0.05) group differences in primary analyses by averaging across all vertices or voxels within each cluster. We then compared the diagnostic subgroups of probands (participants with persistent and those with remitting ADHD) and the comparison group without current ADHD, Bonferroni corrected for the number of clusters. For completeness, eTable 2 (available on the authors' Web site) contains means and SDs for the subgroups with current ADHD-NOS.
Exploratory Analyses of Cortical Thickness
To further investigate primary hypotheses for which no vertices with FDR of less than 0.05 were found, we reexamined subgroup differences heuristically using an uncorrected threshold of P < .05 with a cluster threshold of 50 vertices.42 Because of significant between-group differences in IQ, we confirmed cortical thickness results by also adjusting for IQ.
Table 1 summarizes the derivation of the sample. A larger proportion of comparison participants (80 of 178 [44.9%] originally enrolled participants) than probands (59 of 207 [28.5%]) had analyzable MRIs. This discrepancy reflects a significantly higher rate of unavoidable factors in probands (55 of 207 [26.6%]) (ie, deaths, incarcerations, and MRI exclusions) than in comparison participants (22 of 178 [12.4%]) (χ21 = 12.08; P < .001). By contrast, rates of refusal and failure to schedule or to locate study participants did not differ significantly (93 of 207 probands [44.9%] vs 76 of 178 comparison participants [42.7%]). Accordingly, results are based on anatomic images from 59 ADHD probands and 80 comparison participants.
We compared diagnoses and demographic information at the 18FU of participants who underwent scanning and those who did not (data were available for 57 of 59 probands and for all 80 comparison participants; see eTable 3, available at the authors' Web site). Within the proband and comparison groups, individuals who did and did not undergo scanning did not differ significantly on prevalence of ADHD, antisocial personality disorder, mood or anxiety disorders, any DSM-III disorders, age at referral, IQ, socioeconomic status, or Teacher Conners Hyperactivity Factor score. However, probands undergoing scanning had significantly higher rates of alcohol and nonalcohol substance use disorder and any substance use disorder than did probands who did not undergo scanning (see eTable 3, available at the authors' Web site).
Probands and comparison participants did not differ significantly in age at the time of imaging or in lifetime prevalence of substance abuse or dependence (Table 2). As expected, probands and comparison participants differed significantly in IQ in childhood and 41FU assessments (see eTable 5, available at the authors' Web site, for demographics of subgroups based on current diagnosis and for current substance use and comorbid diagnoses).
Global cortical thickness
Surfacewide mean cortical thickness was significantly lower in probands (n = 59) than in comparison participants (n = 80) (mean [SD], 3.18 [0.11] and 3.24 [0.11] mm, respectively; P < .001 in regression controlling for age and scanner; Cohen d = 0.54). At the 41FU, probands with persistent ADHD differed significantly from the non-ADHD comparison group (3.14 [0.13] and 3.25 [0.10] mm, respectively; P < .001; Cohen d = 1.02). The remitting ADHD group (3.20 [0.11] mm) also differed from the non-ADHD comparison group in overall cortical thickness (P = .04; Cohen d = 0.48). However, participants with persistent ADHD and those with remitting ADHD did not differ significantly (P = .10; Cohen d = 0.51).
Vertexwise analyses of cortical thickness
Figure 1A displays the multiple clusters of vertices (detailed in Table 3) for which the cortex was significantly thinner (surfacewide FDR < 0.05) in ADHD probands; the largest cluster extended from the right precuneus to the precentral gyrus. Other right hemisphere clusters were located in the inferior parietal lobe, temporal pole, and insula. Left hemisphere clusters were located in the superior frontal gyrus/frontal pole, precentral gyrus, insula, temporal pole, and cuneus. There was no instance in which cortical thickness was significantly increased in probands. As shown in eFigure 1 and eTable 6 (available at the authors' Web site), after covarying for IQ (in addition to scanner and age), significant cluster centers remained largely unchanged in location, but the clusters were less extensive.
To assess associations with current ADHD diagnosis, we performed vertexwise comparisons among the different diagnostic subgroups. The 17 individuals with persistent ADHD differed significantly from the 57 non-ADHD comparison participants in most but not all the regions identified in the initial inclusive analyses (Table 3 and Figure 1B). In addition, this analysis revealed thinner cortex related to persistent ADHD in the left medial occipital cortex and right subgenual ACC. When we used FDR < 0.05 as a threshold, participants with remitting ADHD (n = 26) did not differ significantly from the non-ADHD comparison group; participants with persistent ADHD and those with remitting ADHD also did not differ in any region at this threshold. There were no vertices at which cortical thickness was significantly associated with lifetime or current substance abuse diagnoses, dimensional measures of substance abuse, lifetime smoking history, or thioridazine treatment, and there were no significant interactions between group and scanner for any cortical or VBM measures.
Region-based analyses of cortical thickness
To examine potential differences associated with remission from childhood ADHD, we focused on the clusters in which ADHD probands exhibited significantly thinner cortex than comparison participants (FDR < 0.05). Participants with remitting ADHD and those with persistent ADHD had a thinner cortex than did those in the non-ADHD comparison group, with medium to large effect sizes. Average effect sizes between individuals with persistent ADHD and the non-ADHD comparison group (Cohen d = 0.73) were larger than for participants with remitting ADHD (Cohen d = 0.52), although all confidence intervals overlapped (data not shown); individuals with persistent ADHD and those with remitting ADHD did not differ significantly from each other in any cluster at FDR < 0.05 (Table 3).
Exploratory vertexwise analyses
When vertexwise results were thresholded at P < .05 (uncorrected), we observed a thinner cortex for participants with persistent ADHD vs those with remitting ADHD in the insula, bilateral temporal cortex including the right temporal pole, left occipital Brodmann area (BA) 19, orbitofrontal cortex, and medial ACC (Figure 2; see also eTable 7, available at the authors' Web site). There were no regions exceeding our cluster size threshold of 50 vertices in which participants with remitting ADHD exhibited thinner cortex than those with persistent ADHD.
Exploratory region-based analyses
In the clusters that differentiated individuals with persistent ADHD from those with remitting ADHD in exploratory vertexwise analyses, participants with persistent ADHD differed markedly from the non-ADHD comparison group (average Cohen d = 0.75), whereas individuals with remitting ADHD did not (average Cohen d = 0.03; t9 = 8.26; P < .001). Relative to comparison participants, those with remitting ADHD had (nonsignificantly) greater cortical thickness in the left superior temporal gyrus extending to the insula and orbitofrontal cortex, left parahippocampus, left ACC, and left medial occipital cortex (see eTable 7, available at the authors' Web site).
As shown in Table 4 and Figure 3, GM density was significantly greater (FDR < 0.05) for comparison participants than for the ADHD probands in many of the same regions identified through cortical thickness analyses, as well as in subcortical regions inaccessible to cortex-based measures. Figure 4 displays decreased GM in probands in the right caudate, right thalamus, and bilateral cerebellar hemispheres. Voxel-based morphometric analyses of diagnostic subgroups or of medication treatment in childhood with methylphenidate or thioridazine did not yield significant results, even with more lenient thresholds (FDR ≤ 0.2).
In a prospective 33-year longitudinal follow-up of 59 probands (mean age, 41.2 years) with established ADHD in childhood and 80 prospectively enrolled non-ADHD comparison participants, we found an overall significant reduction in mean cortical thickness in the probands. Beyond this global difference, the greatest cortical thinning associated with childhood ADHD was located in the bilateral parietal lobes, temporal poles, insula, precentral gyri, frontal poles, and right precuneus. No cortical region was significantly thicker in probands than in comparison participants. Although less sensitive,44 VBM also revealed significantly decreased GM in probands vs comparison participants in the right precentral, bilateral parietal, left temporal, and right cuneus. In addition, VBM detected decreased GM in probands in the caudate, thalamus, and cerebellar hemispheres.
With respect to current adult diagnosis, probands with persistent ADHD differed the most from those in the non-ADHD comparison group in the same cortical regions identified in our primary analyses, as well as in additional clusters in the left medial occipital cortex and subgenual ACC. Probands with remitting ADHD did not differ significantly from those with persistent ADHD when analyses were corrected for full-brain comparisons. In exploratory uncorrected analyses, probands with persistent ADHD exhibited reduced cortical thickness relative to those with remitting ADHD in the bilateral medial occipital lobes, temporal lobes extending to the insula, and left parahippocampus.
Our results extend previous volumetric and cortical thickness findings in ADHD. First, consistent with the decreased total cerebral volume in ADHD,2-4 our observation of reduced global cortical thickness in probands with ADHD confirms previous reports.12,13,17 Furthermore, although we found less frontal and prefrontal cortical thinning in ADHD than were found in previous studies,11-14,17,45 we confirmed a thinner cortical mantle in the occipitoparietal,11,12,17 temporal cortex, and precentral regions12,13 in ADHD. In subcortical analyses, we also confirmed anatomic abnormalities in the caudate,3,46,47 thalamus,48,49 and cerebellum3 in ADHD.
Studies of cortical thickness in adults with ADHD have focused on specific regions associated with executive function and attentional control.50,51 Makris et al8 selected 9 parcellation units (from 48) per hemisphere and found thinner cortex related to ADHD in the prefrontal and cingulate cortex and inferior parietal lobe, albeit without correcting for multiple comparisons. A cross-sectional study of children, adolescents, and adults found that individuals with ADHD, regardless of age, had a significantly thinner right superior frontal cortex than controls.14 In the adults with ADHD, the specific reduction, with correction for multiple comparisons limited to the frontal lobe, was localized to BA9. In contrast, we did not find group differences in much of the prefrontal cortex but found widespread cortical thinning in the bilateral parietal-temporal cortex. We found similar results in analyses that included all participants, as well as in those limited to probands with persistent ADHD vs the non-ADHD comparison group. The latter contrasts are comparable to those of studies in adults that define group membership by current diagnostic status.14,17
Studies of cortical thickness in children with ADHD are more numerous than those in adults11-13,28,42,52,53 and typically have examined the entire cerebrum, although nearly all (except Shaw et al13) report results uncorrected for multiple comparisons. Thinner cortex has been reported in children with ADHD in the prefrontal and precentral regions,11,13 parietal and temporal lobes,11,12 and inferior frontal gyrus bilaterally.54 In our main analyses, we applied FDR full-brain correction for multiple comparisons and observed significant differences whether groups were defined by the initial childhood history or by the current adult diagnoses. We speculate that the robustness of our results reflects having established the diagnosis of ADHD in childhood as well as our medium to large sample sizes.
Broadly, our results implicate disruptions in large-scale neural systems involved in the regulation of both attention and emotion in adults with childhood ADHD. We found convincing converging anatomic evidence implicating the dorsal attentional network51 and distributed regions within limbic circuits that were thinner in ADHD probands than in comparison groups. Similar findings were obtained when we contrasted probands with persistent ADHD and the comparison group without ADHD. However, we failed to observe hypothesized group differences in prefrontal regions.1,3
First, we found widespread thinner cortex and decreased GM density in bilateral parietal and precentral regions, overlapping areas of the dorsal attentional network. The bilateral dorsal network, which mediates goal-directed, top-down executive control processes, interacts with a right-sided ventral system (stimulus-driven, bottom-up) during attentional functioning,1,51 particularly in redirecting attention. The core areas constituting the dorsal attentional network include the intraparietal sulcus and the conjunction of the precentral and superior frontal sulcus (frontal eye fields),51 which were particularly affected in the ADHD probands. Strikingly, we also observed significantly thinner cortex in the precuneus and superior parietal lobe, which along with the dorsal network core regions are implicated in top-down processing of shifting of attention.55 These findings are consistent with studies of ADHD that report abnormal patterns of activation in parietal regions48 during working memory,56-58 attentional,59-61 or response inhibition62,63 tasks.
We also found occipital cortical thinning in probands with persistent ADHD vs the non-ADHD comparison group. The occipital cortex has been recently found to interact with the dorsal network in maintaining attention55 and in suppressing responses to irrelevant stimuli.64,65 Individuals with ADHD are easily distracted when required to ignore extraneous signals.66,67 Top-down control deficits when responding to irrelevant stimuli are associated with impaired working memory.68,69 Abnormal activation of the occipital cortex has been found in youth70 and adults71-73 with ADHD during working memory tasks. Similarly, in a meta-analysis48 of functional imaging studies, children and adolescents with ADHD showed activation decreases in the left middle occipital gyrus (BA19) compared with controls. In addition, a recent VBM study in adults with ADHD found significant bilateral reduction of GM volume only in the early visual cortex.74
Our VBM analysis revealed cerebellar, thalamic, and striatal GM deficits in ADHD. Cerebellar involvement in ADHD is well established, with findings in children reported mostly in the vermis1-4,75 and in adults in the hemispheres, as in this sample.56,76,77 Early anatomic studies of ADHD did not specifically examine thalamic nuclei, although thalamic hypoactivation emerged in an unbiased meta-analysis.48 Recently, several studies have identified thalamic abnormalities in children and adolescents49,78 and in adults with ADHD.60,79
Second, our analyses revealed thinner cortex in probands, and particularly those with persistent ADHD, across multiple limbic regions, such as the temporal poles (BA38), insula (BA13), and subgenual ACC (BA25). The insula and ACC play important roles in sensorimotor, emotional, and cognitive function.80,81 Specifically, subgenual ACC is implicated in emotional processing and pain perception.82 In humans, subgenual ACC is functionally connected with multiple limbic regions, including the temporal poles83 and insula.84 In turn, the insula, along with participating in performance of demanding tasks,85 is clearly also related to affective processing.86 Abnormal activations in insula and subgenual ACC were reported in a meta-analysis of ADHD functional imaging.48
Cortical thickness studies in ADHD have downplayed findings in the temporal pole, which have been reported but not discussed.11-13 The temporal pole (BA38) is classified as a paralimbic region, based on its interconnections with the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, and is implicated in social and emotional processes.87 Altered activation in temporal pole is associated with deficits in face recognition88-95 and mentalizing, that is, the theory of mind.96-99 The temporal poles have been proposed as a channel for the integration of emotion and perception, playing an important role in emotional and social functions.87
Our findings are consistent with pathophysiological models of ADHD highlighting not only cognitive executive functions (“cool” processes) but also emotional and motivational deficits (“hot” processes).100 Anatomic spiraling circuits begin with emotional and motivational pathways that influence the cool cognitive processes, which in turn control motor responses.101 We observed thinner cortex in regions subserving emotional regulation (the temporal pole, insula, parahippocampus, and subgenual ACC) and top-down attentional regulation (the dorsal attentional network and medial occipital cortex). Furthermore, our exploratory analyses suggest that thinner cortex and diminished GM in the dorsal attentional network and limbic relay regions is related to the trait of having had ADHD in childhood, regardless of current diagnostic status.
Third, the lack of proband-comparison differences in the prefrontal cortex or the ACC was unexpected.7,8,16-18 To better understand possible differences between persistent and remitting ADHD, we performed uncorrected exploratory analyses. In regions in which we found suggestive differences, we observed remarkable congruence between probands with remitting ADHD and comparison groups in the left superior temporal gyrus, ACC, parahippocampus, and occipital cortical thickness as well as in thalamus and cerebellum GM density. We cannot rule out that probands with remitting ADHD may have differed from those with persistent ADHD in these regions since childhood, but the most parsimonious explanation is offered by the hypothesis that remission entails compensatory processes11,102 underpinned by prefrontal cortical maturation. Although we found supporting evidence of ACC and orbitofrontal involvement in diagnostic remission of ADHD, our data also suggest superior temporal, medial occipital, and thalamocerebellar involvement in remission.
Our findings must be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, despite our prospective longitudinal design, we examined brain imaging data only cross-sectionally in middle adulthood. Nevertheless, this is the largest sample of children with ADHD followed up into adulthood, obviating the unreliability of retrospective recall of childhood symptoms. In addition, we report on the largest sample to date of adults with confirmed childhood ADHD who underwent remission. We were able to analyze imaging data from only 28.5% of the original ADHD proband group and 44.9% of the original comparison participants. However, these probands and comparison participants did not differ from the original sample, and the probands studied did not differ significantly from those excluded on nearly all clinical and demographic variables except for significantly higher rates of substance use disorders at the 18FU in probands who underwent scanning. Nevertheless, we did not observe significant relationships between brain anatomic measures and substance use disorders. Finally, as is generally the case, our probands had significantly lower IQ scores than the comparison participants in childhood or adolescence and in adulthood. The issue of whether to covary for IQ in disorders such as ADHD is not settled.103 As shown in eFigure 1 (see also eTable 7, available at the authors' Web site), our principal findings of persistent differences in brain anatomy survived covarying for IQ even with conservative full-brain correction.
We were surprised by the rate of ADHD-NOS diagnosed in comparison participants, which was comparable to the rate in probands. We speculate that secular changes in the general public's awareness of ADHD may have contributed. Although we cannot rule out instrument-related error (see the supplementary Instrument available on the authors' Web site), the use of similar approaches did not yield high rates of ADHD symptoms in comparison participants in 2 previous blinded assessments.21,23 Nevertheless, analyses excluding ADHD-NOS did not alter results appreciably.
We limited study participants to white boys because the number of originally diagnosed girls with ADHD was too small for meaningful statistical comparisons. Thus, our results may not be generalizable to ADHD in women or to other racial or ethnic groups. However, this constraint avoided potential confounds from possible sex, ethnic, or socioeconomic differences. Exclusion of conduct disorder comorbidity (see the supplementary text, available on the authors' Web site) in childhood also averted confusion as to the origin of the deficits found in cortical thickness or GM density.
We cannot comment on cortical thickness or GM density in ADHD in the absence of medication treatment because all but 4 of the probands who underwent scanning were treated with methylphenidate as children. We also did not detect significant effects of childhood treatment with stimulants or thioridazine in cortical thickness or VBM analyses. Medication treatment has been reported to affect cortical thickness,42 although the durability of such effects is unknown, and treatment had been discontinued for all study participants for several decades.
For logistical reasons, we used 2 scanners. Fortunately, scans were approximately counterbalanced across probands and comparison participants, and there were no significant main effects or interactions related to scanner type. Secondary analyses (eFigure 2) also showed that we obtained comparable results when we examined only the 98 scans obtained on the Allegra scanner. Finally, the analyses presented herein were limited to cortical thickness and VBM; ongoing analyses will examine WM structure using diffusion tensor imaging.
In this first study of childhood ADHD prospectively examined in adulthood, we found thinner overall cortex in probands with childhood ADHD that was even more pronounced in those with persistent ADHD. Beyond this global effect, we also detected significant reductions in cortex thickness in the parietal, temporal, and posterior frontal regions corresponding to the dorsal attentional network and limbic areas. These findings were largely echoed by VBM, which in addition highlighted decreased GM in the caudate. These regions underpin the top-down control of attention and the regulation of emotion and motivation and were comparably diminished in probands with remitting ADHD or persistent ADHD. Thus, these differences seem to primarily reflect the childhood diagnosis of ADHD. By contrast, probands with remitting ADHD tended to differ from those with persistent ADHD in the medial occipital cortex, temporal pole, insula, orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampus, and frontal pole and subcortically in the cerebellum and thalamus. This supports the suggestion that symptom amelioration and diagnostic remission may result in part from compensatory maturation of frontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuits.102,104
Correspondence: F. Xavier Castellanos, MD, Phyllis Green and Randolph Cowen Institute for Pediatric Neuroscience, Child Study Center, New York University Langone School of Medicine, 215 Lexington Ave, 14th Floor, New York, NY 10016 (francisco.castellanos@nyumc.org).
Submitted for Publication: September 13, 2010; final revision received March 3, 2011; accepted April 15, 2011.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Previous Presentations: This study was presented at the 16th Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping; June 9, 2010; Barcelona, Spain; and at the Third International Congress on ADHD; May 29, 2011; Berlin, Germany.
Additional Contributions: We thank the participants and their families who have contributed to this research over the decades. Christine Cox, PhD, provided a careful reading of an earlier version of the manuscript. Pierre Bellec, PhD, Yasser Aleman, PhD, and Joost Janssen, PhD, provided helpful discussions regarding methods.
1.Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE, Seidman LJ. Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(12):1361-136916950217
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, Blumenthal JD, James RS, Ebens CL, Walter JM, Zijdenbos A, Evans AC, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
JAMA. 2002;288(14):1740-174812365958
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Durston S, Hulshoff Pol HE, Schnack HG, Buitelaar JK, Steenhuis MP, Minderaa RB, Kahn RS, van Engeland H. Magnetic resonance imaging of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43(3):332-34015076267
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, Faraone SV, Greenhill LL, Howes MJ, Secnik K, Spencer T, Ustun TB, Walters EE, Zaslavsky AM. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.
Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):716-72316585449
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Hesslinger B, Tebartz van Elst L, Thiel T, Haegele K, Hennig J, Ebert D. Frontoorbital volume reductions in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Neurosci Lett. 2002;328(3):319-32112147334
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N, Monuteaux MC, Boriel DL, Kelkar K, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Bush G, Aleardi M, Faraone SV, Biederman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging.
Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(10):1071-108016876137
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Makris N, Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS Jr, Bush G, Crum K, Brown AB, Faraone SV. Anterior cingulate volumetric alterations in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD: a pilot study.
J Atten Disord. 2010;13(4):407-41320008822
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Monuteaux MC, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Makris N, Spencer T, Valera E, Brown A, Bush G, Doyle AE, Hughes S, Helliesen M, Mick E, Biederman J. A preliminary study of dopamine D4 receptor genotype and structural brain alterations in adults with ADHD.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B(8):1436-144118951431
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Castellanos FX, Proal E. Location, location, and thickness: volumetric neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder comes of age.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(10):979-98120854767
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Giedd J, Rapoport JL. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(49):19649-1965418024590
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Narr KL, Woods RP, Lin J, Kim J, Phillips OR, Del’Homme M, Caplan R, Toga AW, McCracken JT, Levitt JG. Widespread cortical thinning is a robust anatomical marker for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(10):1014-102219730275
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, Giedd J, Castellanos FX, Rapoport J. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):540-54916651511
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Almeida LG, Ricardo-Garcell J, Prado H, Barajas L, Fernández-Bouzas A, Avila D, Martínez RB. Reduced right frontal cortical thickness in children, adolescents and adults with ADHD and its correlation to clinical variables: a cross-sectional study.
J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(16):1214-122320510424
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Faraone SV, Biederman J, Feighner JA, Monuteaux MC. Assessing symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adults: which is more valid?
J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68(5):830-84211068969
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Makris N, Buka SL, Biederman J, Papadimitriou GM, Hodge SM, Valera EM, Brown AB, Bush G, Monuteaux MC, Caviness VS, Kennedy DN, Seidman LJ. Attention and executive systems abnormalities in adults with childhood ADHD: a DT-MRI study of connections.
Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(5):1210-122017906338
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Makris N, Biederman J, Valera EM, Bush G, Kaiser J, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(6):1364-137516920883
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Biederman J, Makris N, Valera EM, Monuteaux MC, Goldstein JM, Buka S, Boriel DL, Bandyopadhyay S, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Bush G, Aleardi M, Hammerness P, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Towards further understanding of the co-morbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and bipolar disorder: a MRI study of brain volumes.
Psychol Med. 2008;38(7):1045-105617935640
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Valera EM, Faraone SV, Biederman J, Poldrack RA, Seidman LJ. Functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(5):439-44715737657
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Klein DF, Bessler A, Shrout P. Accuracy of adult recall of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(11):1882-188812411223
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bonagura N, Malloy P, Giampino TL, Addalli KA. Hyperactive boys almost grown up, V: replication of psychiatric status.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(1):77-831984764
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(5):816-81810784477
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Gittelman R, Mannuzza S, Shenker R, Bonagura N. Hyperactive boys almost grown up, I: psychiatric status.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42(10):937-9474037987
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult outcome of hyperactive boys: educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50(7):565-5768317950
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(4):493-4989545994
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Gittelman-Klein R, Klein DF, Katz S, Saraf K, Pollack E. Comparative effects of methylphenidate and thioridazine in hyperkinetic children, I: clinical results.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976;33(10):1217-1231971031
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1968
28.Shaw P, Gornick M, Lerch J, Addington A, Seal J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Evans A, Giedd JN, Castellanos FX, Rapoport JL. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome, and cortical structure in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(8):921-93117679637
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Gittelman R, Abikoff H, Pollack E, Klein DF, Katz S, Mattes JA. Controlled trial of behavior modification and methylphenidate in hyperactive children. In: Whalen CK, Henker B, eds. Hyperactive Children: The Ecology of Identification and Treatment. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1980:221-243
30.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Truong NL, Moulton JL III, Roizen ER, Howell KH, Castellanos FX. Age of methylphenidate treatment initiation in children with ADHD and later substance abuse: prospective follow-up into adulthood.
Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(5):604-60918381904
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Robbins S, Evans AC, Collins DL, Whitesides S. Tuning and comparing spatial normalization methods.
Med Image Anal. 2004;8(3):311-32315450225
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC. A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 1998;17(1):87-979617910
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Zijdenbos AP, Forghani R, Evans AC. Automatic“pipeline” analysis of 3-D MRI data for clinical trials: application to multiple sclerosis.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2002;21(10):1280-129112585710
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Lyttelton O, Boucher M, Robbins S, Evans A. An unbiased iterative group registration template for cortical surface analysis.
Neuroimage. 2007;34(4):1535-154417188895
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Tohka J, Zijdenbos A, Evans A. Fast and robust parameter estimation for statistical partial volume models in brain MRI.
Neuroimage. 2004;23(1):84-9715325355
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.MacDonald D, Kabani N, Avis D, Evans AC. Automated 3-D extraction of inner and outer surfaces of cerebral cortex from MRI.
Neuroimage. 2000;12(3):340-35610944416
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Lerch JP, Evans AC. Cortical thickness analysis examined through power analysis and a population simulation.
Neuroimage. 2005;24(1):163-17315588607
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.R Development Core Team. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2009
40.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.
J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1995;57(1):289-300
Google Scholar 41.Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate.
Neuroimage. 2002;15(4):870-87811906227
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Shaw P, Sharp WS, Morrison M, Eckstrand K, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Psychostimulant treatment and the developing cortex in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(1):58-6318794206
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Fleiss JL, Cohen J. The Global Assessment Scale: a procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976;33(6):766-771
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Hutton C, Draganski B, Ashburner J, Weiskopf N. A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging.
Neuroimage. 2009;48(2):371-38019559801
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW, Peterson BS. Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Lancet. 2003;362(9397):1699-170714643117
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Carmona S, Proal E, Hoekzema EA, Gispert JD, Picado M, Moreno I, Soliva JC, Bielsa A, Rovira M, Hilferty J, Bulbena A, Casas M, Tobeña A, Vilarroya O. Ventro-striatal reductions underpin symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(10):972-97719576573
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Carmona S, Vilarroya O, Bielsa A, Trèmols V, Soliva JC, Rovira M, Tomàs J, Raheb C, Gispert JD, Batlle S, Bulbena A. Global and regional gray matter reductions in ADHD: a voxel-based morphometric study.
Neurosci Lett. 2005;389(2):88-9316129560
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(10):1051-106217073984
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Ivanov I, Bansal R, Hao X, Zhu H, Kellendonk C, Miller L, Sanchez-Pena J, Miller AM, Chakravarty MM, Klahr K, Durkin K, Greenhill LL, Peterson BS. Morphological abnormalities of the thalamus in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(4):397-40820123910
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201-21511994752
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Shaw P, Lalonde F, Lepage C, Rabin C, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Greenstein D, Evans A, Giedd JN, Rapoport J. Development of cortical asymmetry in typically developing children and its disruption in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):888-89619652128
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Wolosin SM, Richardson ME, Hennessey JG, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Abnormal cerebral cortex structure in children with ADHD.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(1):175-18417985349
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Batty MJ, Liddle EB, Pitiot A, Toro R, Groom MJ, Scerif G, Liotti M, Liddle PF, Paus T, Hollis C. Cortical gray matter in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(3):229-23820410712
PubMedGoogle Scholar 55.Shulman GL, Astafiev SV, Franke D, Pope DL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy MP, Corbetta M. Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks.
J Neurosci. 2009;29(14):4392-440719357267
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Wolf RC, Plichta MM, Sambataro F, Fallgatter AJ, Jacob C, Lesch KP, Herrmann MJ, Schönfeldt-Lecuona C, Connemann BJ, Grön G, Vasic N. Regional brain activation changes and abnormal functional connectivity of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during working memory processing in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(7):2252-226619107748
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Bayerl M, Dielentheis TF, Vucurevic G, Gesierich T, Vogel F, Fehr C, Stoeter P, Huss M, Konrad A. Disturbed brain activation during a working memory task in drug-naive adult patients with ADHD.
Neuroreport. 2010;21(6):442-44620224456
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Burgess GC, Depue BE, Ruzic L, Willcutt EG, Du YP, Banich MT. Attentional control activation relates to working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):632-64020060961
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Banich MT, Burgess GC, Depue BE, Ruzic L, Bidwell LC, Hitt-Laustsen S, Du YP, Willcutt EG. The neural basis of sustained and transient attentional control in young adults with ADHD.
Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(14):3095-310419619566
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Cubillo A, Halari R, Ecker C, Giampietro V, Taylor E, Rubia K. Reduced activation and inter-regional functional connectivity of fronto-striatal networks in adults with childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and persisting symptoms during tasks of motor inhibition and cognitive switching.
J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(10):629-63920060129
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Schneider MF, Krick CM, Retz W, Hengesch G, Retz-Junginger P, Reith W, Rösler M. Impairment of fronto-striatal and parietal cerebral networks correlates with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) psychopathology in adults: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study.
Psychiatry Res. 2010;183(1):75-8420558047
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Karch S, Thalmeier T, Lutz J, Cerovecki A, Opgen-Rhein M, Hock B, Leicht G, Hennig-Fast K, Meindl T, Riedel M, Mulert C, Pogarell O. Neural correlates (ERP/fMRI) of voluntary selection in adult ADHD patients.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260(5):427-44019907927
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Dillo W, Goke A, Prox-Vagedes V, Szycik GR, Roy M, Donnerstag F, Emrich HM, Ohlmeier MD. Neuronal correlates of ADHD in adults with evidence for compensation strategies: a functional MRI study with a Go/No-Go paradigm.
Ger Med Sci. 2010;8:Doc0920421953
PubMedGoogle Scholar 64.Capotosto P, Babiloni C, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Frontoparietal cortex controls spatial attention through modulation of anticipatory alpha rhythms.
J Neurosci. 2009;29(18):5863-587219420253
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Mevorach C, Hodsoll J, Allen H, Shalev L, Humphreys G. Ignoring the elephant in the room: a neural circuit to downregulate salience.
J Neurosci. 2010;30(17):6072-607920427665
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Lijffijt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control?
J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114(2):216-22215869352
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Clark L, Blackwell AD, Aron AR, Turner DC, Dowson J, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Association between response inhibition and working memory in adult ADHD: a link to right frontal cortex pathology?
Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(12):1395-140117046725
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Zanto TP, Rubens MT, Bollinger J, Gazzaley A. Top-down modulation of visual feature processing: the role of the inferior frontal junction.
Neuroimage. 2010;53(2):736-74520600999
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti IM, Yang Y, Ulug AM, Casey BJ. Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD.
Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(10):871-87812742674
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Schweitzer JB, Faber TL, Grafton ST, Tune LE, Hoffman JM, Kilts CD. Alterations in the functional anatomy of working memory in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(2):278-28010671402
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Schweitzer JB, Lee DO, Hanford RB, Tagamets MA, Hoffman JM, Grafton ST, Kilts CD. A positron emission tomography study of methylphenidate in adults with ADHD: alterations in resting blood flow and predicting treatment response.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(5):967-97312700698
PubMedGoogle Scholar 73.Schweitzer JB, Hanford RB, Medoff DR. Working memory deficits in adults with ADHD: is there evidence for subtype differences?
Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:4317173676
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Ahrendts J, Rusch N, Wilke M, Philipsen A, Eickhoff SB, Glauche V, Perlov E, Ebert D, Hennig J, Tebartz van Elst L. Visual cortex abnormalities in adults with ADHD: a structural MRI study [published online September 29, 2010].
World J Biol Psychiatry20879808
PubMedGoogle Scholar 75.Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Berquin PC, Walter JM, Sharp W, Tran T, Vaituzis AC, Blumenthal JD, Nelson J, Bastain TM, Zijdenbos A, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(3):289-29511231836
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Perlov E, Tebarzt van Elst L, Buechert M, Maier S, Matthies S, Ebert D, Hesslinger B, Philipsen AH. H¹-MR-spectroscopy of cerebellum in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44(14):938-94320332052
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Valera EM, Spencer RM, Zeffiro TA, Makris N, Spencer TJ, Faraone SV, Biederman J, Seidman LJ. Neural substrates of impaired sensorimotor timing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(4):359-36720619827
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Qiu MG, Ye Z, Li QY, Liu GJ, Xie B, Wang J. Changes of brain structure and function in ADHD children [published online December 30, 2010].
Brain Topogr21191807
PubMedGoogle Scholar 79.Dibbets P, Evers EA, Hurks PP, Bakker K, Jolles J. Differential brain activation patterns in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) associated with task switching.
Neuropsychology. 2010;24(4):413-42320604616
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Pollatos O, Gramann K, Schandry R. Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28(1):9-1816729289
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Craig AD. How do you feel? interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(8):655-66612154366
PubMedGoogle Scholar 82.Vogt BA, Vogt L, Farber NB, Bush G. Architecture and neurocytology of monkey cingulate gyrus.
J Comp Neurol. 2005;485(3):218-23915791645
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex.
Neuroimage. 2007;37(2):579-58817604651
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 84.Taylor KS, Seminowicz DA, Davis KD. Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(9):2731-274519072897
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 85.Eckert MA, Menon V, Walczak A, Ahlstrom J, Denslow S, Horwitz A, Dubno JR. At the heart of the ventral attention system: the right anterior insula.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(8):2530-254119072895
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 86.Quartz SR. Reason, emotion and decision-making: risk and reward computation with feeling.
Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(5):209-21519362037
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y. The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing.
Brain. 2007;130(pt 7):1718-173117392317
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 88.Mackay CE, Roberts N, Mayes AR, Downes JJ, Foster JK, Mann D. An exploratory study of the relationship between face recognition memory and the volume of medial temporal lobe structures in healthy young males.
Behav Neurol. 1998;11(1):3-2011568398
PubMedGoogle Scholar 89.Nakamura K, Kawashima R, Sato N, Nakamura A, Sugiura M, Kato T, Hatano K, Ito K, Fukuda H, Schormann T, Zilles K. Functional delineation of the human occipito-temporal areas related to face and scene processing: a PET study.
Brain. 2000;123(pt 9):1903-191210960054
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 90.Grabowski TJ, Damasio H, Tranel D, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD, Damasio AR. A role for left temporal pole in the retrieval of words for unique entities.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;13(4):199-21211410949
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 91.Dupont S. Investigating temporal pole function by functional imaging.
Epileptic Disord. 2002;4:(suppl 1)
S17-S2212424086
PubMedGoogle Scholar 92.Nelson EE, McClure EB, Monk CS, Zarahn E, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Ernst M. Developmental differences in neuronal engagement during implicit encoding of emotional faces: an event-related fMRI study.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(7):1015-102414531584
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 93.Tsukiura T, Namiki M, Fujii T, Iijima T. Time-dependent neural activations related to recognition of people's names in emotional and neutral face-name associative learning: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):784-79414568451
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 94.Griffith HR, Richardson E, Pyzalski RW, Bell B, Dow C, Hermann BP, Seidenberg M. Memory for famous faces and the temporal pole: functional imaging findings in temporal lobe epilepsy.
Epilepsy Behav. 2006;9(1):173-18016766230
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 95.Kim JW, Kim JJ, Jeong BS, Ki SW, Im DM, Lee SJ, Lee HS. Neural mechanism for judging the appropriateness of facial affect.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25(3):659-66716226433
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 96.Jimura K, Konishi S, Asari T, Miyashita Y. Temporal pole activity during understanding other persons' mental states correlates with neuroticism trait.
Brain Res. 2010;1328:104-11220226769
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 97.Mier D, Lis S, Neuthe K, Sauer C, Esslinger C, Gallhofer B, Kirsch P. The involvement of emotion recognition in affective theory of mind.
Psychophysiology. 2010;47(6):1028-103920456660
PubMedGoogle Scholar 98.Völlm BA, Taylor AN, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, Deakin JF, Elliott R. Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task.
Neuroimage. 2006;29(1):90-9816122944
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 99.Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Mori T, Matsuda H, Komaki G. Changes of brain activity in the neural substrates for theory of mind during childhood and adolescence.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;61(4):355-36317610659
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 100.Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction.
Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(3):117-12316460990
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 102.Halperin JM, Schulz KP. Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Psychol Bull. 2006;132(4):560-58116822167
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 103.Meehl P. Nuisance variables and the ex post facto design. In: Radner MWS, ed. Analyses of Theories and Methods of Physics and Psychology. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 1970:373-402
104.Fassbender C, Schweitzer JB. Is there evidence for neural compensation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? a review of the functional neuroimaging literature.
Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26(4):445-46516500007
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref