Context Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is an etiologically heterogeneous neurodevelopmental condition with long-term negative outcomes. However, the early developmental course of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms and their association with previous environmental risk factors are still poorly understood
Objectives To describe the developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms and to identify their prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal risk factors.
Design Birth cohort from the general population.
Setting Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development.
Participants The sample consisted of 2057 individuals, followed up from age 5 months to 8 years.
Main Outcome Measures Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal risk factors assessed at age 5 months were considered predictors of group membership in high hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years.
Results The frequency of hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms tended to slightly decrease with age, whereas the frequency of inattention symptoms substantially increased up to age 6 years. However, trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms were significantly associated with each other. Risk factors for high trajectories of both types of symptoms were premature birth (adjusted odds ratio [aOR], 1.93; 95% CI, 1.07-3.50), low birth weight (2.11; 1.12-3.98), prenatal tobacco exposure (1.41; 1.03-1.93), nonintact family (1.85; 1.26-2.70), young maternal age at birth of the target child (1.78; 1.17-2.69), paternal history of antisocial behavior (1.78; 1.28-2.47), and maternal depression (1.35; 1.18-1.54).
Conclusions A large range of early risk factors, including prenatal, perinatal social, and parental psychopathology variables, act independently to heighten the likelihood of having persistently high levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from infancy to middle childhood. Early interventions should be experimented with to provide effective tools for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder prevention.
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a clinically significant condition with early childhood onset and persisting lifelong features. Etiologically, ADHD is believed to be a heterogeneous disorder within a developmental psychopathologic framework.1,2 The large amount of research regarding the putative causes of ADHD clearly implicates genetic and environmental risk factors. Despite the high heritability of ADHD, recent molecular studies3,4 suggest only small effects of multiple candidate genes contributing each to a relatively modest proportion of variance in ADHD expression. Conversely, environmental risk factors could play an influential role in the emergence of ADHD, particularly during the sensitive developmental periods of fetal and early life.1,5 Consequently, a better identification of early environmental risk factors may afford better hindsight for mechanism understanding and potential interventions. This is all the more relevant because, to date, genetic studies have not provided immediate targets for intervention, and available therapeutic strategies have shown limited long-term efficacy.6
The most consistent research findings regarding the role of environmental factors in ADHD suggest prenatal, perinatal, and early postnatal factors. Smoking during pregnancy has been regularly linked to ADHD in offspring.7,8 Other prenatal factors, such as alcohol, illegal drug, and psychotropic maternal consumptions, have yielded more conflicting results.8-10 Perinatal factors, such as low birth weight, preterm birth, and birth complications, have been studied but are difficult to untangle.9,11-13 Studies10,13-16 of postnatal social and relational factors have involved social adversity, hostile parenting, and parental psychopathology, whereas studies9,17 of postnatal physical factors have suggested traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, food additives, and dietary deficiencies as putative causes.
This body of research regarding early environmental risk factors of ADHD has at least 5 important limitations. First, in most studies, the participants were elementary school children. This choice of participants prevented understanding the development of the disorder during the preschool years. Second, measures of risk factors were often retrospective and potentially affected by a memory bias. Third, the outcomes of most studies were the traditional ADHD taxonomic categories assessed at 1 point in time. This approach precluded understanding the dimensional and developmental nature of the disorder.18 It is becoming clearer that hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention may be more accurately conceptualized as 2 phenotypic dimensions varying with age rather than discrete categories with a clear age at onset.1,5,19 The rare studies20,21 of preschool developmental trajectories did not consider inattention. However, earlier developmental trajectories are necessary to assess chronic behavior problems rather than a 1-point diagnostic evaluation of older children. In addition, they have been shown to be associated with full disorder and later negative outcomes.22,23 A fourth limitation is that many investigations relied either on clinical samples of males presenting with ADHD or on cohorts of at-risk individuals (ie, preterm or low-birth-weight newborns). This jeopardized the generalizability of the findings to community and female populations. Fifth, studies rarely simultaneously considered a large range of risk factors, which enhanced the possibility of residual confounding. Overall, despite their potential value, studies regarding the impact of a scope of environmental risk factors on early trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms are scarce in population-based samples.
The objective of the present study was to fill these gaps by examining the association between early environmental risk factors and early developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms using a birth cohort representative of the general population. We first determined developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from age 17 months to 8 years using group-based trajectory modeling. We then created joint trajectories and assessed the effect of prenatal, perinatal, postnatal, and parental psychopathology risk factors on the joint trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms.
Participants and procedure
Data were drawn from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development, whose protocol was approved by the Quebec Institute of Statistics (Quebec City, Quebec, Canada) and the St-Justine Hospital Research Center (Montreal) ethics committees. Data were collected by trained interviewers through home interviews regularly conducted with the person most knowledgeable about the child (the mother in 98% of cases) to obtain information about child, parent, and family characteristics and behaviors. Written informed consent was obtained from all the participating families at each assessment. Assessments were conducted at the following ages: 5 months and 1½, 2½, 3½, 4½, 5, 6, and 8 years. The Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development sample was drawn from the Quebec Birth Registry using a stratified procedure based on living area and birth rate. Families were included if the pregnancy lasted 24 to 42 weeks and the mother could speak French or English. The initial sample comprised 2120 children evaluated at age 5 months and was representative of children born in the province of Quebec (Canada) in 1997 and 1998. The average response rate during the 8 years of data collection was 87.0% (range, 68%-100%). Table 1 describes the sociodemographic characteristics of the sample used to build hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories (N = 2057). No significant difference was noted between the 2 samples.
Outcome Variable: Hyperactivity-Impulsivity and Inattention Symptoms
Children's hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms were reported through the Interviewer Computerized Questionnaire when the children were 1½, 2½, 3½, 4½, 5, 6, and 8 years of age. Ratings relied on the early childhood behavior scale from the Canadian National Longitudinal Study of Children and Youth.24 This tool incorporates items from the Child Behavior Checklist,25 the Ontario Child Health Study Scales,26 and the Preschool Behavior Questionnaire.27 This instrument has been shown to have good validity in early childhood and in predicting ADHD.21,23 Five items were used to assess hyperactivity-impulsivity:“can't sit still, is restless or hyperactive,”“fidgets,”“is impulsive, acts without thinking,”“has difficulty waiting for his or her turn in games,” and“cannot settle down to do anything for more than a few moments.” Three items were used to assess inattention:“cannot concentrate, cannot pay attention for long,”“is easily distracted, has trouble sticking to any activity,” and“is inattentive.” All items referred to the past 3 months and were coded on a frequency scale (never or not true = 0, sometimes or somewhat true = 1, and often or very true = 2), which made it possible to build quantitative scores and then develop hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories. Regarding the analysis of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories, 94.8% of the sample had at least 4 data points available.
The analyses were performed in 3 steps. First, individual developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms between 17 and 96 months of age were empirically identified. This identification was conducted through group-based trajectory modeling using semiparametric mixture models with censored-normal distributions.33 Using the proc traj procedure of SAS,34 we established the best models in terms of number of groups and polynomial order of the trajectories based on the Bayesian information criterion. To account for missing data and provide better estimates, subjects were included when at least 1 data point value was available. Second, a joint model of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories was estimated to allow examination of the 2 developmental patterns simultaneously. It provided joint probabilities (estimated percentages of children belonging simultaneously to hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories) and 2 conditional probabilities (probability of a hyperactivity-impulsivity trajectory conditional on a given inattention trajectory and vice versa). Third, multivariate analyses (implemented in a logistic regression framework) were conducted to determine associations between risk factors and joint trajectory groups (high trajectory of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or high trajectory of inattention vs others). To select the predictors included in the multivariate models, we estimated bivariate associations between risk factors and the outcome (bivariate logistic regression). Variables with P < .25 were subsequently entered into the initial multivariate models. Backward selection (variables deleted when P ≥ .05) with control for confounding factors was then conducted. Statistical significance for all analyses was set at P < .05. To test the robustness of the findings, sensitivity analysis was performed using a multiple imputation model (number of imputations = 5) under the missing at random nonresponse mechanism.35 Fourth, interactions between sex and independent variables kept in the final model were tested.
Joint developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention
Modeling the data using a censored-normal distribution, we identified a 3-group model as the best fit for individual developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and a 3-group model as the best fit for individual developmental trajectories of inattention. All polynomial terms were quadratics. The Figure shows the joint trajectory model. The 3 hyperactivity-impulsivity trajectories were as follows: high (16.1%), moderate (52.7%), and low (31.2%). The 3 inattention trajectories were as follows: high (13.0%), moderate (58.2%), and low (28.8%). Hyperactivity-impulsivity symptom scores tended to slightly decrease with age, whereas inattention symptom scores substantially increased up to age 6 years and then declined. The first part of Table 2 displays the probability of joint hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention. In total, 12.3% of participants belonged to both the high hyperactivity-impulsivity group and the high inattention group; 3.7% had a high level of hyperactivity-impulsivity without high inattention; only 0.7% belonged to the high inattention group without being high for hyperactivity-impulsivity; and 83.3% did not belong to any high-level group. Almost no participants had high levels of symptoms of one type and low levels of another. The second part of Table 2 depicts the probabilities of inattention conditional on hyperactivity-impulsivity. Participants in the low hyperactivity-impulsivity group had a high probability (P = .848) of belonging to the low inattention group. Conversely, participants in the high hyperactivity-impulsivity group had a high probability (P = .768) of belonging to the high inattention group. The third part of Table 2 shows the probability of hyperactivity-impulsivity conditional on inattention. Participants in the low inattention group had a high probability (P = .920) of belonging to the low hyperactivity-impulsivity group. Conversely, participants in the high inattention group had a high probability (P = .949) of belonging to the high hyperactivity-impulsivity group.
Early risk factors for high hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention trajectories
Owing to the large overlap between hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention, we decided to consider together high levels on any dimension as the outcome measure. Table 3 provides the results of multiple logistic regression models predicting trajectories of high levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention. Model 1 (n = 1665) shows the results of multivariate models including all risk factors associated with the outcome with a P < .25. This model was significant (Waldχ2 = 151.19, P < .0001), and the fit was good (P = .51). Model 2 (n = 1721) displays results of the multivariate model with backward selection and step-by-step confounding control. This model was significant (Waldχ2 = 154.52, P < .0001), and the fit was good (P = .20). Insufficient income, low maternal education, maternal history of antisocial behaviors, paternal depression, family dysfunction, coercive parenting, and overprotection were significantly related to high hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention in bivariate analyses but not in multivariate models. In the final model, premature birth, low birth weight, prenatal tobacco exposure, nonintact family, maternal age at birth younger than 21 years, paternal childhood/adolescent antisocial behaviors, maternal depression at child age 5 months, sex, methylphenidate lifetime exposure, and difficult temperament were significantly related to trajectories of high levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention. No significant statistical interaction was noted between the sex of the child and other significant risk factors. All the final predictive models were without multicollinearity (all condition index numbers<20). The sample with complete data used for multivariate analyses was largely similar to the initial sample regarding sociodemographic characteristics. However, there was a tendency for less advantaged families, nonintact families, and immigrant mothers (P < .0001 for all) to have missing values. The parameter estimates did not change with sensitivity analysis under the missing at random assumption. In addition, restricting the analyses of the hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention trajectories by using individuals with at least 4 data points led to the same results. Further analyses to predict a high hyperactivity-impulsivity trajectory by adjusting for inattention provided similar results, and analyses predicting a high inattention trajectory by adjusting for hyperactivity-impulsivity led to comparable findings.
To our knowledge, this investigation is the first to focus on the development of joint trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms during the first 8 years of life. Compared with discrete category typing at 1 point in time, developmental trajectory analysis has the advantages of accounting for symptom intensity variations over age and of identifying clusters of individuals following similar typical and atypical courses of development. In addition, this study documented the contribution of a wide range of early risk factors. Results showed that a host of prenatal and early postnatal variables were independently associated with high levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms from infancy to the early school years. These findings rely on a population and sex-balanced longitudinal survey.
Regarding developmental trajectories, we found that 16.1% of children followed a high-declining hyperactivity-impulsivity trajectory, a decrease expected with age. Regarding inattention, 13.0% of children followed a high-ascending trajectory. To our knowledge, no study has reported on early inattention trajectories. This ascending shape of the inattention trajectory may reflect either a specific developmental pattern or a measurement issue, attention being more easily detected with age. Joint analysis indicated that most children concurrently displayed both high levels of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention. This finding is of particular relevance because it sheds new light on the debate concerning changes to the DSM-5, that is, whether to keep the existing structure or replace it by considering a unique combined ADHD type (http://www.dsm5.org). The present data suggest that it is difficult to distinguish parent-reported hyperactivity-impulsivity from inattention up to age 8 years. However, the present study does not rule out the existence of an ADHD inattentive subtype. It is possible that the construct of early developmental trajectories taps into the ADHD combined subtype and that the ADHD inattentive subtype emerges later in life.
Regarding the prenatal and perinatal risk factors, we found an independent contribution of premature birth and low birth weight to the high hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention trajectories. Previous literature linking attention problems and preterm and/or low birth weight arose from heterogeneous studies frequently using small and highly selected samples.36,37 It has been shown that preterm and low-birth-weight newborns disproportionately have a reduction in gray matter and white matter injuries, anomalies associated with attention problems, executive dysfunction, and cognitive impairment.38,39 Multiple biological and psychosocial factors could explain the relationship.40 First, pregnancy and delivery complications may alter the brain and generate observed or more subtle cerebral damages. Second, immaturity, through biological programming or increased vulnerability to neuronal cell death, may produce neurologic sequelae and disruption in cortical development and brain connectivity.38,41 Third, early life adversities associated with intensive care (ie, sensory stress, sleep deprivation, repetitive pain, and disturbance in parent-child interaction) may contribute to similar effects on the developing brain.42 Fourth, prematurity is associated with more negative parent-child and family interactions at 4 months of age.43 However, these last 2 variables do not explain the association in the present study. It has been suggested that being small for gestational age rather than prematurity or low birth weight may account for the risk of ADHD,44 a hypothesis not tested in the present study.
The predictive association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD is consistent with previous research.7,8 Some biological, nonheritable pathways could mediate the effect of prenatal tobacco exposure on later ADHD. Nicotine and other tobacco products, through damage and functional disturbance of the placenta, could generate chronic fetal hypoxia, resulting in observed intrauterine growth retardation and low birth weight.45,46 Tobacco could also affect brain development in a more direct way, through deleterious effects on neurotransmission, neuronal differentiation, and migration.47 Alternatively, this association may reflect a gene-environment correlation. A few genetically informed studies support the view that a common inherited genetic predisposition could link maternal smoking with ADHD in offspring.48,49 Because results are contradictory,50 more research is required to test this hypothesis more fully. Finally, a possible interaction between prenatal tobacco use and polymorphisms of DAT1 and DRD4 genotypes in increasing the risk of ADHD has been suggested.51
Prenatal alcohol and illegal drug exposure was not significantly linked to hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention trajectories. This could be due to a real absence of association, particularly for alcohol, which has been inconsistently related to later ADHD, or to a lack of power, especially for illegal drug exposure.
Perinatal social variables also accounted for the risk of following the high hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention trajectories. However, whereas young maternal age at birth of the target child and being from a nonintact family remained predictors in multivariate models, this was not the case for insufficient family income and low maternal education. Previous studies found connections between low socioeconomic status, low maternal education levels, and ADHD.13,52 However, those studies did not consider a wide range of confounders. A possible explanation for the present results is that insufficient family income and having a mother without a high school diploma are more distal variables and may, thus, have been accounted for by more proximal risk factors in the causal chain, such as young maternal age and membership in a nonintact family. These latter factors could reflect parental conflict, parenting difficulties, violence exposure, neglect, or maltreatment. However, in the present study, the available family and parenting variables at child age 5 months were not conclusive in that regard in multivariate models.
Parental psychopathology increased the liability of following the high hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention trajectories. Maternal depression and paternal history of antisocial behaviors were significant predictors and accounted for the prediction of paternal depression at child age 5 months and childhood/adolescent antisocial behaviors in mothers. These results are relevant because there is little research examining the heterotypic continuity between maternal postnatal depression, antisocial paternal behavior, and ADHD symptoms in offspring.21,53 One possible interpretation is that this pattern of associations merely reflects the fact that parental mental health affected the ratings due to a shared source of variance with child behavior trajectories (ie, a measurement issue). An alternative interpretation is that there is a real association between parental psychopathology and ADHD symptoms. If so, the mechanisms underlying this intergenerational transmission could be complex and may involve a heritable common genetic liability and environmental processes through postnatal relational variables. Parenting at child age 5 months (family dysfunction, mother-child interaction, coercive parenting, and overprotection) was not significant in the adjusted models. This absence of association may be due to the interplay of other adjustment variables or may be related to weak measures of parenting variables in this study. Indeed, chronic or later parenting disturbances may have shown a greater contribution to ADHD trajectories. In addition, unmeasured factors, such as parental ADHD, prenatal and postnatal anxiety, and depression could act as confounders.15
Finally, difficult temperament and being a boy were significantly associated with high trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and/or inattention. Temperament could be implicated in the development of ADHD.54 The link could be mediated through extreme approach tendencies or low effortful control and through some negative parenting resulting from ADHD symptoms.20,21 As for the sex of the child, it is well documented that boys are more susceptible to neurodevelopmental disorders and other disruptive behaviors (among which is ADHD) from early childhood.55
Limitations should be considered when interpreting the present results. First, full-blown categorical ADHD was not assessed because we did not consider the impairment related to developmental trajectories. Clinical impairment will be more reliably assessed at a later age. Second, we relied on parental reports for assessing hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms. Parental ratings remain the most accurate measure to investigate early trajectories. Teacher assessments of these individuals during the elementary school years will provide an opportunity to compare early parent assessments with teacher assessments during the school years. Third, because most risk factors were self-reported, mothers may have underreported their psychopathology and substance use during pregnancy, possibly leading to underestimation of these effects. Fourth, inherited genetic and epigenetic factors were not considered in the present study. This precluded examination of genetic× environment interactions and correlations. This is a point for future research because environmental effects may exert the strongest influence over individuals with a particular genetic vulnerability. Despite their small effect sizes, environmental risk factors may still be of major importance in vulnerable subgroups as genetic and environmental factors likely act together (additively and multiplicatively) to generate a neurobiological risk. This could be especially true during key stages of development, when fetal and social programming can produce long-term changes in gene expression and neurobiological pathways. Further investigations at a molecular level are needed to disentangle the epigenetic effects of prenatal and postnatal factors (silencing/activating genes of susceptibility or with protective effects).1,5,56,57 Fifth, other potential confounders and effect modifiers, such as toxemia, antepartum hemorrhage, maternal hypothyroidism, antenatal stress, maternal depression during pregnancy, parental postnatal anxiety and ADHD status, prenatal and postnatal physical risk factors, breastfeeding, and medications other than methylphenidate (eg, other stimulants, nonstimulants, antihistaminics, antiepileptics, and antipsychotics), were not measured in this study. Sixth, the focus on very early risk factors limited the understanding of developmental pathways after 5 months of age. Nevertheless, later risk and protective factors (eg, other child internalizing and externalizing problems, family and relational difficulties, hostile-reactive parenting, traumas, and education) are likely to affect the developmental trajectories of ADHD symptoms and may represent potential targets to early interventions.
On the whole, these results support the hypothesis of the multifactorial etiology and heterogeneity of environmental causal processes implicated in ADHD. They extend this finding to a wide set of very early and independent risk factors in the prediction of early trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention symptoms. This is especially significant for the understanding of underlying mechanisms and for public health issues. Indeed, developmental trajectories connected to recognizable risk factors, some of which are modifiable, can be identified early on. Early development is a particular period of vulnerability but also plasticity, when influences on programming processes and on the developing brain are subject to modification.1,5,56,57 This dynamic view suggests implementing alternative and complementary preventive tools in the management of ADHD. In addition to the promotion of adequate maternal habits during pregnancy, risk factors (including prenatal, perinatal, perinatal social, and parental psychopathology variables and difficult temperament of the child) could be evaluated early on to help in identifying high-risk children and families who may receive support. However, due to the weak predictive power of early risk indicators in multifactorial diseases and the ethical questions raised by primary intervention, some caution must be taken. Besides, the early risk factors identified are prone to multifinality. Indeed, they have been associated with other deleterious outcomes, including depression, anxiety, and disruptive behaviors.58-60 Hence, experiments on early prevention would be welcome to determine whether and which among universal, selective, and indicated prevention programs would have the greatest effect on the early developmental trajectories of hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention.
Correspondence: Cédric Galéra, MD, PhD, Charles Perrens Hospital, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Bordeaux Segalen, INSERM U897, CHS Charles-Perrens, 121 rue de la Béchade, Bordeaux, 33076 France (cedric.galera@u-bordeaux2.fr).
Submitted for Publication: April 22, 2011; final revision received June 25, 2011; accepted July 7, 2011.
Author Contributions: Dr Galéra had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Financial Disclosure: Dr Bouvard received financial support for the organization of scientific meetings and was also the principle investigator in clinical trials for Shire and Lilly.
Funding/Support: This study was funded by Quebec's Department of Health and Social Services, the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, the Quebec Fund for Research on Society and Culture, the Quebec Fund for Research on Nature and Technology, the Health Research Fund of Quebec, Quebec's Ministry of Research, Science, and Technology, Human Resources Development Canada, Health Canada, the Lucie and André Chagnon Foundation, the National Science Foundation, the University of Montreal, Laval University, and McGill University.
Additional Contributions: We thank the children and families whose ongoing participation made this study possible. We also acknowledge the considerable contribution of the staff and the coordinators of the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development at the Quebec Institute of Statistics, and the tireless work of all the interviewers who assessed the mothers and children during this study. We thank the staff of the Research Unit on Children's Psychosocial Maladjustment for the help in data access, analyses, and management.
Additional Information: Dr Tremblay is the founding scientific director of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Children.
1.Sonuga-Barke EJ, Halperin JM. Developmental phenotypes and causal pathways in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: potential targets for early intervention?
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(4):368-38920015192
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Coghill D, Banaschewski T. The genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Expert Rev Neurother. 2009;9(10):1547-156519831843
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Faraone SV, Mick E. Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2010;33(1):159-18020159345
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Sonuga-Barke EJ. Editorial:‘It's the environment stupid!’ on epigenetics, programming and plasticity in child mental health.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(2):113-11520059628
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, Epstein JN, Hoza B, Hechtman L, Abikoff HB, Elliott GR, Greenhill LL, Newcorn JH, Wells KC, Wigal T, Gibbons RD, Hur K, Houck PR.MTA Cooperative Group. The MTA at 8 years: prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(5):484-50019318991
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Cornelius MD, Day NL. Developmental consequences of prenatal tobacco exposure.
Curr Opin Neurol. 2009;22(2):121-12519532034
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Linnet KM, Dalsgaard S, Obel C, Wisborg K, Henriksen TB, Rodriguez A, Kotimaa A, Moilanen I, Thomsen PH, Olsen J, Jarvelin MR. Maternal lifestyle factors in pregnancy risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated behaviors: review of the current evidence.
Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1028-104012777257
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Millichap JG. Etiologic classification of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Pediatrics. 2008;121(2):e358-e36518245408
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Rodriguez A, Olsen J, Kotimaa AJ, Kaakinen M, Moilanen I, Henriksen TB, Linnet KM, Miettunen J, Obel C, Taanila A, Ebeling H, Järvelin MR. Is prenatal alcohol exposure related to inattention and hyperactivity symptoms in children? disentangling the effects of social adversity.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(9):1073-108319298478
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis.
JAMA. 2002;288(6):728-73712169077
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Milberger S, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Guite J, Tsuang MT. Pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: issues of gene-environment interaction.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(1):65-758988797
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.St Sauver JL, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Jacobsen SJ. Early life risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a population-based cohort study.
Mayo Clin Proc. 2004;79(9):1124-113115357033
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Deault LC. A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2010;41(2):168-19219768532
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.O’Connor TG, Heron J, Golding J, Beveridge M, Glover V. Maternal antenatal anxiety and children's behavioural/emotional problems at 4 years: report from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.
Br J Psychiatry. 2002;180:502-50812042228
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Rodriguez A, Bohlin G. Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children?
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(3):246-25415755301
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Auinger P, Hornung R, Epstein JN, Braun J, Kahn RS. Association of tobacco and lead exposures with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Pediatrics. 2009;124(6):e1054-e106319933729
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Snowling M. Editorial: multiple perspectives on ADHD: implications for future research.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(9):1039-104119709089
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Naugle RI. The latent structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample.
Neuropsychology. 2007;21(1):45-6417201529
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Huijbregts SC, Séguin JR, Zoccolillo M, Boivin M, Tremblay RE. Associations of maternal prenatal smoking with early childhood physical aggression, hyperactivity-impulsivity, and their co-occurrence.
J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35(2):203-21517294130
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Romano E, Tremblay RE, Farhat A, Côté S. Development and prediction of hyperactive symptoms from 2 to 7 years in a population-based sample.
Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):2101-211016740853
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Campbell SB, Shaw DS, Gilliom M. Early externalizing behavior problems: toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment.
Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12(3):467-48811014748
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Séguin JR, Nagin D, Assaad JM, Tremblay RE. Cognitive-neuropsychological function in chronic physical aggression and hyperactivity.
J Abnorm Psychol. 2004;113(4):603-61315535792
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Statistics Canada. Overview of Survey Instruments for 1994-1995 Data Collection, Cycle 1. Ottawa, ON: Statistics Canada; 1995
25.Achenbach TM. Child Behavior Checklist. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1991
26.Boyle MH, Offord DR, Racine Y, Sanford M, Szatmari P, Fleming JE. Evaluation of the original Ontario Child Health Study scales.
Can J Psychiatry. 1993;38(6):397-4058402433
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Tremblay RE, Desmarais-Gervais L, Gagnon C, Charlebois P. The Preschool Behavior Questionnaire: stability of its factor structure between cultures, sexes, ages and socioeconomic classes.
Int J Behav Dev. 1987;10(4):467-484
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Bradley RH, Caldwell BM. The relation of infants' home environments to achievement test performance in first grade: a follow-up study.
Child Dev. 1984;55(3):803-8096734319
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Boivin M, Pérusse D, Dionne G, Saysset V, Zoccolillo M, Tarabulsy GM, Tremblay N, Tremblay RE. The genetic-environmental etiology of parents' perceptions and self-assessed behaviours toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(6):612-63015877767
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Zoccolillo M. Parents' health and social adjustment, part II: social adjustment. In: Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD 1998-2002). Vol 1. No. 9. Québec, Canada: Institut de la Statistique du Québec; 2000
32.Radloff LS. The CESD-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population.
Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385-401
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Nagin D. Group-Based Modeling of Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2005
35.Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York, NY: John Wiley& Sons; 1987
36.Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children.
Pediatrics. 2009;124(2):717-72819651588
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.van Baar AL, Vermaas J, Knots E, de Kleine MJ, Soons P. Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks' gestational age.
Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):251-25719564307
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Tolsa CB, Zimine S, Warfield SK, Freschi M, Sancho Rossignol A, Lazeyras F, Hanquinet S, Pfizenmaier M, Huppi PS. Early alteration of structural and functional brain development in premature infants born with intrauterine growth restriction.
Pediatr Res. 2004;56(1):132-13815128927
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Goldenberg RL, Culhane JF, Iams JD, Romero R. Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth.
Lancet. 2008;371(9606):75-8418177778
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Inder TE, Warfield SK, Wang H, Hüppi PS, Volpe JJ. Abnormal cerebral structure is present at term in premature infants.
Pediatrics. 2005;115(2):286-29415687434
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Perlman JM. Neurobehavioral deficits in premature graduates of intensive care—potential medical and neonatal environmental risk factors.
Pediatrics. 2001;108(6):1339-134811731657
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Feldman R. Maternal versus child risk and the development of parent-child and family relationships in five high-risk populations.
Dev Psychopathol. 2007;19(2):293-31217459172
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Heinonen K, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK, Andersson S, Kajantie E, Eriksson JG, Wolke D, Lano A. Behavioural symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preterm and term children born small and appropriate for gestational age: a longitudinal study.
BMC Pediatr. 2010;10:9121159164
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Herrmann M, King K, Weitzman M. Prenatal tobacco smoke and postnatal secondhand smoke exposure and child neurodevelopment.
Curr Opin Pediatr. 2008;20(2):184-19018332716
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Jauniaux E, Burton GJ. Morphological and biological effects of maternal exposure to tobacco smoke on the feto-placental unit.
Early Hum Dev. 2007;83(11):699-70617900829
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Dwyer JB, McQuown SC, Leslie FM. The dynamic effects of nicotine on the developing brain.
Pharmacol Ther. 2009;122(2):125-13919268688
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Knopik VS. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and child outcomes: real or spurious effect?
Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34(1):1-3619142764
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Thapar A, Rice F, Hay D, Boivin J, Langley K, van den Bree M, Rutter M, Harold G. Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from a novel design.
Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):722-72719596120
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Obel C, Linnet KM, Henriksen TB, Rodriguez A, Järvelin MR, Kotimaa A, Moilanen I, Ebeling H, Bilenberg N, Taanila A, Ye G, Olsen J. Smoking during pregnancy and hyperactivity-inattention in the offspring—comparing results from three Nordic cohorts.
Int J Epidemiol. 2009;38(3):698-70518250076
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Neuman RJ, Lobos E, Reich W, Henderson CA, Sun LW, Todd RD. Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype.
Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(12):1320-132817157268
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Peterson BS, Pine DS, Cohen P, Brook JS. Prospective, longitudinal study of tic, obsessive-compulsive, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders in an epidemiological sample.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(6):685-69511392347
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Bornovalova MA, Hicks BM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Familial transmission and heritability of childhood disruptive disorders.
Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(9):1066-107420634367
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Bishop D, Rutter M. Neurodevelopmental disorders: conceptual issues. In: Rutter M, Bishop D, Pine D, Scott S, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Thapar A, eds. Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5th ed. Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishing; 2008:32-41
56.Bale TL, Baram TZ, Brown AS, Goldstein JM, Insel TR, McCarthy MM, Nemeroff CB, Reyes TM, Simerly RB, Susser ES, Nestler EJ. Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(4):314-31920674602
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Mill J, Petronis A. Pre- and peri-natal environmental risks for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the potential role of epigenetic processes in mediating susceptibility.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(10):1020-103018492038
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Côté SM, Boivin M, Liu X, Nagin DS, Zoccolillo M, Tremblay RE. Depression and anxiety symptoms: onset, developmental course and risk factors during early childhood.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(10):1201-120819519755
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Petitclerc A, Boivin M, Dionne G, Zoccolillo M, Tremblay RE. Disregard for rules: the early development and predictors of a specific dimension of disruptive behavior disorders.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(12):1477-148419686334
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Tremblay RE. Developmental origins of disruptive behaviour problems: the‘original sin’ hypothesis, epigenetics and their consequences for prevention.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(4):341-36720146751
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref