[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. Example of a male identity used for creating the 9 morphs in the current experiment. 100% angry, 75%/25% angry/neutral, 50%/50% angry/neutral, 25%/75% angry/neutral, neutral, 25%/75% happy/neutral, 50%/50% happy/neutral, 75%/25% happy/neutral, and 100% happy.

Figure 1. Example of a male identity used for creating the 9 morphs in the current experiment. 100% angry, 75%/25% angry/neutral, 50%/50% angry/neutral, 25%/75% angry/neutral, neutral, 25%/75% happy/neutral, 50%/50% happy/neutral, 75%/25% happy/neutral, and 100% happy.

Figure 2. Examples of the beginning of hostility and nose-width rating blocks. The event-related paradigm consisted of 2 runs of 4 blocks each, 2 hostility and 2 nose-width (47 trials/block). Attention state (nose vs hostility) block order was randomized across participants.

Figure 2. Examples of the beginning of hostility and nose-width rating blocks. The event-related paradigm consisted of 2 runs of 4 blocks each, 2 hostility and 2 nose-width (47 trials/block). Attention state (nose vs hostility) block order was randomized across participants.

Figure 3. Left amygdala region of interest (ROI) demonstrated that healthy volunteers (HVs) had increased activation with increased anger. However, amygdala activity was not modulated by anger in patients with bipolar disorder (BD) or in patients with severe mood dysregulation (SMD). A, Coronal, sagittal, and transverse views in an example of the left (L) amygdala ROI (in red). B, β Weights for each group along each neutral-to-angry morph in the L amygdala. C, Linear trends for each group along the neutral-to-angry morph in the L amygdala. A25 indicates 25%/75% angry/neutral; A50, 50%/50% angry/neutral; A75, 75%/25% angry/neutral; A100, 100% angry; Neut, neutral; and R, right.

Figure 3. Left amygdala region of interest (ROI) demonstrated that healthy volunteers (HVs) had increased activation with increased anger. However, amygdala activity was not modulated by anger in patients with bipolar disorder (BD) or in patients with severe mood dysregulation (SMD). A, Coronal, sagittal, and transverse views in an example of the left (L) amygdala ROI (in red). B, β Weights for each group along each neutral-to-angry morph in the L amygdala. C, Linear trends for each group along the neutral-to-angry morph in the L amygdala. A25 indicates 25%/75% angry/neutral; A50, 50%/50% angry/neutral; A75, 75%/25% angry/neutral; A100, 100% angry; Neut, neutral; and R, right.

Figure 4. In the right inferior parietal lobule (BA40), as in the other whole-brain clusters significant for the neutral-to-happy (N→H) morph, patients with bipolar disorder (BD) showed increased activity. However, patients with severe mood dysregulation (SMD) showed decreased activity, and healthy volunteers (HVs) did not modulate activity when viewing increased happiness in the face. A, Coronal, sagittal, and axial views in an example of the right (R) inferior parietal lobule (BA40) cluster. Color indicates the intensity of the group effect within that cluster. The warmer the color, the higher the significance of the main effect of group in that cluster. B, β Weights for each group along each N→H morph in the R inferior parietal lobule (BA40). C, Linear trends for each group along the neutral-to-happy morph in the R inferior parietal lobule (BA40). H25 indicates 25%/75% happy/neutral; H50, 50%/50% happy/neutral; H75, 75%/25% happy/neutral; H100, 100% happy; and Neut, neutral.

Figure 4. In the right inferior parietal lobule (BA40), as in the other whole-brain clusters significant for the neutral-to-happy (N→H) morph, patients with bipolar disorder (BD) showed increased activity. However, patients with severe mood dysregulation (SMD) showed decreased activity, and healthy volunteers (HVs) did not modulate activity when viewing increased happiness in the face. A, Coronal, sagittal, and axial views in an example of the right (R) inferior parietal lobule (BA40) cluster. Color indicates the intensity of the group effect within that cluster. The warmer the color, the higher the significance of the main effect of group in that cluster. B, β Weights for each group along each N→H morph in the R inferior parietal lobule (BA40). C, Linear trends for each group along the neutral-to-happy morph in the R inferior parietal lobule (BA40). H25 indicates 25%/75% happy/neutral; H50, 50%/50% happy/neutral; H75, 75%/25% happy/neutral; H100, 100% happy; and Neut, neutral.

Table 1. Demographic Data
Table 1. Demographic Data
Table 2. Group Main Effects Identified by Whole-Brain Analysis
Table 2. Group Main Effects Identified by Whole-Brain Analysis
1.
Moreno C, Laje G, Blanco C, Jiang H, Schmidt AB, Olfson M. National trends in the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar disorder in youth.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(9):1032-103917768268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Towbin KE, Bhangoo RK, Pine DS. Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(3):430-43712611821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Brotman MA, Kassem L, Reising MM, Guyer AE, Dickstein DP, Rich BA, Towbin KE, Pine DS, McMahon FJ, Leibenluft E. Parental diagnoses in youth with narrow phenotype bipolar disorder or severe mood dysregulation.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(8):1238-124117671287PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, Costello EJ, Egger HL, Angold A, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(9):991-99717056393PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Stringaris A, Baroni A, Haimm C, Brotman M, Lowe CH, Myers F, Rustgi E, Wheeler W, Kayser R, Towbin K, Leibenluft E. Pediatric bipolar disorder versus severe mood dysregulation: risk for manic episodes on follow-up.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(4):397-40520410732PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler AD, Brotman MA, Rich BA, Kimes AS, Pine DS, Ernst M, Leibenluft E. Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood psychopathology.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(9):863-87117714371PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Rich BA, Grimley ME, Schmajuk M, Blair KS, Blair RJ, Leibenluft E. Face emotion labeling deficits in children with bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation.  Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20(2):529-54618423093PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Schenkel LS, Pavuluri MN, Herbener ES, Harral EM, Sweeney JA. Facial emotion processing in acutely ill and euthymic patients with pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(8):1070-107917667485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
McClure EB, Treland JE, Snow J, Schmajuk M, Dickstein DP, Towbin KE, Charney DS, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(9):1644-165116135623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Brotman MA, Rich BA, Guyer AE, Lunsford JR, Horsey SE, Reising MM, Thomas LA, Fromm SJ, Towbin K, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Amygdala activation during emotion processing of neutral faces in children with severe mood dysregulation versus ADHD or bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(1):61-6919917597PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Rich BA, Schmajuk M, Perez-Edgar KE, Fox NA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Different psychophysiological and behavioral responses elicited by frustration in pediatric bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(2):309-31717267795PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Darwin C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Reprint ed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1965. Original work published in 1872
13.
Kalmar JH, Wang F, Chepenik LG, Womer FY, Jones MM, Pittman B, Shah MP, Martin A, Constable RT, Blumberg HP. Relation between amygdala structure and function in adolescents with bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(6):636-64219454919PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN. Fronto-limbic dysfunction in mania pre-treatment and persistent amygdala over-activity post-treatment in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;216(4):485-49921390505PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Pavuluri MN, O’Connor MM, Harral E, Sweeney JA. Affective neural circuitry during facial emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(2):158-16717097071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Pavuluri MN, Passarotti A. Neural bases of emotional processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8(9):1381-138718759550PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Pavuluri MN, Passarotti AM, Harral EM, Sweeney JA. An fMRI study of the neural correlates of incidental versus directed emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(3):308-31919242292PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Olsavsky AK, Brotman MA, Rutenberg JG, Muhrer EJ, Deveney CM, Fromm SJ, Towbin K, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Amygdala hyperactivation during face emotion processing in bipolar disorder and children at familial risk for bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(3):294-30322365465PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder.  Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(9):829, 833-85718574483PubMedGoogle Scholar
20.
Rich BA, Vinton DT, Roberson-Nay R, Hommer RE, Berghorst LH, McClure EB, Fromm SJ, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Limbic hyperactivation during processing of neutral facial expressions in children with bipolar disorder.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(23):8900-890516735472PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;9(2):93-10510680766PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN. Differential engagement of cognitive and affective neural systems in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(1):106-11719849880PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980-9889204677PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ekman P, Friesen W. Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists; 1976
25.
LaBar KS, Crupain MJ, Voyvodic JT, McCarthy G. Dynamic perception of facial affect and identity in the human brain.  Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(10):1023-103312967919PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brian. New York, NY: Thieme; 1988
27.
Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.  Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29(3):162-1738812068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Adleman NE, Kayser R, Dickstein D, Blair RJ, Pine D, Leibenluft E. Neural correlates of reversal learning in severe mood dysregulation and pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(11):1173-1185Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions.  Nature. 1996;383(6603):812-8158893004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Chen CH, Lennox B, Jacob R, Calder A, Lupson V, Bisbrown-Chippendale R, Suckling J, Bullmore E. Explicit and implicit facial affect recognition in manic and depressed states of bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(1):31-3916112653PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Foland LC, Altshuler LL, Bookheimer SY, Eisenberger N, Townsend J, Thompson PM. Evidence for deficient modulation of amygdala response by prefrontal cortex in bipolar mania.  Psychiatry Res. 2008;162(1):27-3718063349PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Surguladze S, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, Andrew C, Frangou S, Ecker C, Phillips ML. Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(6):578-58715013826PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Surguladze SA, Marshall N, Schulze K, Hall MH, Walshe M, Bramon E, Phillips ML, Murray RM, McDonald C. Exaggerated neural response to emotional faces in patients with bipolar disorder and their first-degree relatives.  Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):58-6420595014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Versace A, Thompson WK, Zhou D, Almeida JR, Hassel S, Klein CR, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Abnormal left and right amygdala-orbitofrontal cortical functional connectivity to emotional faces: state versus trait vulnerability markers of depression in bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):422-43120159144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Strakowski SM, Eliassen JC, Lamy M, Cerullo MA, Allendorfer JB, Madore M, Lee JH, Welge JA, DelBello MP, Fleck DE, Adler CM. Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain activation in bipolar mania: evidence for disruption of the ventrolateral prefrontal-amygdala emotional pathway.  Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(4):381-38821051038PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Almeida JR, Versace A, Hassel S, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Elevated amygdala activity to sad facial expressions: a state marker of bipolar but not unipolar depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):414-42119931855PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18(1):30-4112454910PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Morris R, Paxinos G, Petrides M. Architectonic analysis of the human retrosplenial cortex.  J Comp Neurol. 2000;421(1):14-2810813770PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Mohanty A, Gitelman DR, Small DM, Mesulam MM. The spatial attention network interacts with limbic and monoaminergic systems to modulate motivation-induced attention shifts.  Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2604-261318308706PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, Lupson V, Bullmore ET. Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):795-80215500300PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Phillips ML, Bullmore ET, Howard R, Woodruff PW, Wright IC, Williams SC, Simmons A, Andrew C, Brammer M, David AS. Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study.  Psychiatry Res. 1998;83(3):127-1389849722PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Almeida JR, Versace A, Mechelli A, Hassel S, Quevedo K, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Abnormal amygdala-prefrontal effective connectivity to happy faces differentiates bipolar from major depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):451-45919450794PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Hassel S, Almeida JR, Kerr N, Nau S, Ladouceur CD, Fissell K, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Elevated striatal and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in response to emotional stimuli in euthymic bipolar disorder: no associations with psychotropic medication load.  Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(8):916-92719594507PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Hassel S, Almeida JR, Frank E, Versace A, Nau SA, Klein CR, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Prefrontal cortical and striatal activity to happy and fear faces in bipolar disorder is associated with comorbid substance abuse and eating disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2009;118(1-3):19-2719243839PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Radua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, El-Hage W, McDonald C, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, David AS, Surguladze SA. Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults.  Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):939-94619699306PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przuntek H. Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc Biol Sci. 1998;265(1409):1927-19319821359PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Kanwisher N. Domain specificity in face perception.  Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(8):759-76310903567PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Farrow TF, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JF, Tarrier N, Griffiths PD, Woodruff PW. Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness.  Neuroreport. 2001;12(11):2433-243811496124PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Beer JS, Ochsner KN. Social cognition: a multi level analysis.  Brain Res. 2006;1079(1):98-10516513097PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E. Visual attention: insights from brain imaging.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1(2):91-10011252779PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Maunsell JH. Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention?  Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(6):261-26515165551PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Cerullo MA, Fleck DE, Eliassen JC, Smith MS, DelBello MP, Adler CM, Strakowski SM. A longitudinal functional connectivity analysis of the amygdala in bipolar I disorder across mood states.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(2):175-18422420593PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Leibenluft E. Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(2):129-14221123313PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
Dec 2012

Parametric Modulation of Neural Activity by Emotion in Youth With Bipolar Disorder, Youth With Severe Mood Dysregulation, and Healthy Volunteers

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Emotion and Development Branch (Drs Thomas, Brotman, Deveney, Pine, and Leibenluft; Messrs Muhrer and Bones; and Ms Rosen) and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core (Mr Reynolds), National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Bethesda, Maryland. Dr Thomas is now with the Office of the Director, National Institute on Drug Abuse, Rockville, Maryland.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1257-1266. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.913
Abstract

Context Youth with bipolar disorder (BD) and those with severe, nonepisodic irritability (severe mood dysregulation [SMD]) exhibit amygdala dysfunction during facial emotion processing. However, studies have not compared such patients with each other and with comparison individuals in neural responsiveness to subtle changes in facial emotion; the ability to process such changes is important for social cognition. To evaluate this, we used a novel, parametrically designed faces paradigm.

Objective To compare activation in the amygdala and across the brain in BD patients, SMD patients, and healthy volunteers (HVs).

Design Case-control study.

Setting Government research institute.

Participants Fifty-seven youths (19 BD, 15 SMD, and 23 HVs).

Main Outcome Measure Blood oxygenation level–dependent data. Neutral faces were morphed with angry and happy faces in 25% intervals; static facial stimuli appeared for 3000 milliseconds. Participants performed hostility or nonemotional facial feature (ie, nose width) ratings. The slope of blood oxygenation level–dependent activity was calculated across neutral-to-angry and neutral-to-happy facial stimuli.

Results In HVs, but not BD or SMD participants, there was a positive association between left amygdala activity and anger on the face. In the neutral-to-happy whole-brain analysis, BD and SMD participants modulated parietal, temporal, and medial-frontal areas differently from each other and from that in HVs; with increasing facial happiness, SMD patients demonstrated increased, and BD patients decreased, activity in the parietal, temporal, and frontal regions.

Conclusions Youth with BD or SMD differ from HVs in modulation of amygdala activity in response to small changes in facial anger displays. In contrast, individuals with BD or SMD show distinct perturbations in regions mediating attention and face processing in association with changes in the emotional intensity of facial happiness displays. These findings demonstrate similarities and differences in the neural correlates of facial emotion processing in BD and SMD, suggesting that these distinct clinical presentations may reflect differing dysfunctions along a mood disorders spectrum.

Pediatric bipolar disorder (BD) is diagnosed with increasing frequency,1 perhaps because some suggest that it presents as either severe nonepisodic irritability or episodic mania.2 This suggestion raises questions about clinical and pathophysiologic differences between these 2 presentations. To evaluate this, the phenotype of severe, nonepisodic irritability was operationalized as severe mood dysregulation (SMD).2 Family history3 and longitudinal data4,5 suggest that BD and SMD are dissociable phenotypes, whereas behavioral data find similarities in facial information processing.6-9 Imaging studies can elucidate similarities and differences in neural mediators of such information-processing findings.10,11 In this study, we compared blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) activation patterns during the presentation of parametrically morphed emotional faces in patients with BD or SMD and healthy volunteers (HVs).

Emotional faces are salient nonverbal social cues. Appropriate identification of and responses to such cues are necessary for proper learning and behavior in social situations.12 Children with BD or SMD, but not those with other forms of psychopathologic disorders, show perturbed facial emotion–labeling ability.6-10 On a task in which neutral faces morph into full emotional expressions, both BD and SMD youth, compared with HV youth, require more intense emotion to be displayed on the face before they can identify it.7 In addition, SMD and BD patients perceive neutral faces as more fear producing than do HVs.10

However, although BD and SMD youth exhibit similar behavioral impairments on face-labeling tasks, the only study10 to compare face processing in the 2 phenotypes found that their neural correlates differ. Specifically, Brotman et al10 found that SMD patients, relative to BD patients and HV youth, exhibited amygdala hypoactivation during explicit processing of neutral faces. Most pediatric BD studies13-17 find amygdala hyperactivation during implicit and explicit facial emotion processing, including hostility ratings of neutral faces. Indeed, a recent study18 found that amygdala dysfunction extends to youth at risk for BD, emphasizing the importance of the amygdala as a region of interest in BD. However, although literature on affect dysregulation suggests that the amygdala is important, the amygdala functions in the service of affect regulation and dysregulation as part of a distributed circuit encompassing other regions (including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex).19 Together, these studies suggest that amygdala dysfunction varies as a function of diagnosis. However, insufficient work has compared SMD and BD directly, so the degree to which dysfunction in the amygdala or other regions is shared or specific in these disorders during face processing requires further investigation.

In this study, we built on prior work showing that SMD and BD patients perform more poorly than HVs on facial emotion–processing tasks. Specifically, BD or SMD patients require more intense emotional information than do HVs before they can identify an emotion,7 and BD patients rate neutral faces as being more hostile than do HVs.20 Both SMD and BD patients demonstrate amygdala dysfunction10; in this study we used a parametric design to examine amygdala modulation in the context of an emotion-processing task. We used an implicit and explicit emotion-processing condition, since previous work has demonstrated amygdala dysfunction in BD13-17 and SMD10 during explicit emotion processing, although evidence suggests that implicit emotion processing activates the amygdala more consistently in healthy populations21 and can also distinguish pediatric patients with BD from HVs.17 Therefore, questions remain as to whether group differences in the current study would manifest in both or one attention condition. Specifically, we morphed faces parametrically from neutral to 100% happy or 100% angry in 25% increments and examined the degree to which each group exhibited a linear relationship between facial emotion intensity and behavioral and neural measures. Thus, we examined the “dose-response” curve of neural activity changes with increases in emotion on a face. This approach is sensitive to subtle group differences, avoids problems with rapid amygdala habituation to identical emotional stimuli, and allows us to examine the neural underpinnings to the finding that, compared with HVs, BD and SMD youth require more emotional intensity to label a facial emotion accurately.7

Given our interest in amygdala dysfunction, we conducted a region of interest (ROI) analysis in addition to a whole-brain analysis. On the basis of behavioral findings noted in the previous paragraphs, we hypothesized that both SMD and BD youth would differ from HVs in amygdala responsiveness to subtle changes in emotional expression. However, the directionality of expected differences was unclear, given inconsistencies in prior findings. Finally, given the observed differences in the clinical presentation, longitudinal course, and pathophysiologic characteristics of SMD and BD, we expected diagnosis-specific neural correlates in other brain regions, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which have been shown to be less active in youth with BD compared with HVs in emotional face tasks.14,15,17,22

Methods
Participants

Usable functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired from 57 individuals, including those with BD (n = 19) or SMD (n = 15) and HVs (n = 23) (Table 1); data from 2 HVs, 2 BD patients, and 3 SMD patients were excluded for excessive motion. All participants, aged 8 to 18 years, were enrolled in an institutional review board–approved study at the National Institute of Mental Health. Parents and youths gave written informed consent or assent. Patients and HVs were recruited and assessed using methods described previously.10 Participants were assessed using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age Children–Present and Lifetime Version, including a module to ascertain SMD.23 Interviewers were master's degree and doctoral degree clinicians, with excellent interrater reliability (κ >0.9 for all diagnoses, including differentiating BD from SMD). Diagnoses were based on best-estimate procedures generated in a consensus conference led by a psychiatrist (one of whom was E.L.). Bipolar disorder patients were “narrow phenotype,” with at least 1 full-duration hypomanic or manic episode characterized by abnormally elevated mood and at least 3 DSM -IV B mania symptoms.2 The SMD youth had nonepisodic irritability, overreactivity to negative emotional stimuli at least 3 times per week, and hyperarousal symptoms. Symptoms had to begin before age 12 years, be present for at least 1 year with no symptom-free periods exceeding 2 months, and cause severe impairment in at least 1 setting (ie, home, school, or peers) and mild impairment in another. Euphoric mood or distinct hypomanic or manic episodes lasting more than 1 day were exclusionary.2 The HVs had no lifetime psychiatric diagnoses and, as ascertained by parent interview, no first-degree relatives with a mood disorder.

Exclusion criteria for all participants were Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence IQ lower than 70 and history of head trauma, neurologic disorder, pervasive developmental disorder, unstable medical illness, or substance abuse or dependence. The HVs were receiving no medication; most BD and SMD youths were receiving medication.

Behavioral paradigm

Photographs of faces used for testing were obtained from the Ekman pictures of facial affect24 and were morphed between a 100% angry face and a neutral face and between a 100% happy face and a neutral face (Figure 1). Face morphs were created using methods from LaBar et al25 with commercial software (Stoik MorphMan 2000; Stoik Imaging). Five female faces and 5 male faces were included.

Thee morphs were made at 25% increments, resulting in 9 facial emotions (100% angry [hereinafter called A100], 75%/25% angry/neutral [A75], 50%/50% angry/neutral [A50], 25%/75% angry/neutral [A25], 100% neutral [Neut], 25%/75% happy/neutral [H25], 50%/50% happy/neutral [H50], 75%/25% happy/neutral [H75], and 100% happy [H100]). To have adequate statistical power while keeping the paradigm brief enough to be well tolerated by children with severe psychopathologic disorders, we focused on only 2 emotions: 1 positive (happy) and 1 negative (angry). We chose these emotions because previous neuroimaging work14,15,17 has shown differences in neural activity between youths with BD and HVs while processing them.

There were 2 behavior conditions: nose width and hostility ratings. Ratings were on a 5-point scale, from 1 (least wide/least hostile) to 5 (most wide/most hostile). In each trial, faces appeared for 3000 milliseconds, during which the participants made ratings. The intertrial interval varied from 750 to 1250 milliseconds (average, 1000 milliseconds) (Figure 2).

There were 4 blocks, each including 41 nose-width rating trials, 41 hostility rating trials, and 12 fixation trials to add jitter and provide a baseline. Stimuli and behavior conditions were presented randomly within each block. Since we were especially interested in neural activation while participants viewed the morphs containing ambiguous emotion (ie, A50, A25, Neut, H25, and H50), these had more replicates than emotionally unambiguous faces (ie, A100, A75, H75, and H100). For the heavily weighted morphs, there were 6 trials per morph in each condition of each block (24 total) except that Neut had 5 trials per condition per block (20 total). For the less-weighted morphs, there were 3 trials per morph in each condition of each block (12 total). Each block contained 94 trials at an average of 4000 milliseconds per trial and was approximately 6.3 minutes long, yielding a 25-minute experiment.

Image acquisition

Data were acquired on a scanner (3T; General Electric). Structural images used T1-weighted axial acquisition (3-dimensional spoiled gradient-recall acquisition in the steady state with inversion-recovery preparation pulse; 256 × 192 matrix; one hundred twenty-four 1.2-mm axial sections; 22-cm field of view) to allow normalization to standard space.26 Functional imaging was performed axially using a multisection gradient echo-planar sequence (24-cm field of view, 96 × 96 matrix, 38 contiguous 2.6-mm sections; repetition time, 2300 milliseconds; echo time, 25 milliseconds).

Behavioral data analysis

Ratings and reaction time (RT) were compared in separate repeated-measures 3 (group: SMD, BD, HV) × 2 (behavior: hostility, nose-width ratings) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on linear trend values that describe the data. We divided the analysis into neutral to 100% angry (N→A) and neutral to 100% happy (N→H). Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses were conducted as needed.

Imaging analysis

Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were analyzed with the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages program,27 using preprocessing methods described previously,28 with 6-mm root-mean-square blur. Data were scaled to a percentage of the voxelwise mean, subject to a limit of 200 to avoid truncation artifacts. We censored multiple movement spikes greater than 1.5 mm and excluded participants with more than 5% censored times to repetition. Regressors for each morph in each behavior condition were created by convolving stimulus times with a γ-variate hemodynamic response function. Linear regression modeling was performed per voxel with 18 regressors, 1 for each stimulus condition (9 morphs × 2 behavior states), a third-order polynomial modeling the baseline drift, and 6 motion parameters. Blank fixation trials provided a baseline. Individual β-coefficient maps were warped into standard space with a high-resolution anatomic image that had been manually normalized by identifying the anterior-posterior commissures, midsagittal plane, and outer boundary.

To model neural activity to the faces morphing from N→A and N→H at the individual participant level, 2 trend analyses were performed for the parametrically modeled stimulus classes, 1 each for N→A and N→H. The regressors were analyzed for linear trends between BOLD signal and facial stimulus intensity.

For the amygdala ROI, we extracted average BOLD linear trend magnitudes for N→A and N→H in the right and left amygdala based on the Talairach-Tournoux Daemon.26 For each amygdala ROI, a repeated-measures group (BD, SMD, HV) × behavior (hostility, nose-width) ANOVA was conducted on the linear trend magnitudes. If there was a main effect of group, we used commercial software (PASW 18.0.1; SPSS Inc) to evaluate whether the slope value for each group differed significantly from zero using a within-group, 1-sample t test with a single group against a test value of zero, and we conducted Tukey-corrected between-group post hoc analyses.

At the whole-brain level, we examined linear trends for both N→A and N→H in a group × behavior ANOVA. We used the AlphaSim Monte Carlo simulation in the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages program to calculate the cluster size associated with a mapwise (entire brain mask), whole-brain, false-positive probability of P < .05. This threshold was applied to data initially passing a P < .005 threshold, yielding a smoothness of 8 to 9 mm. With these features, κ ≥168 at a resolution of 2×2×2 emerged as the significance threshold. For clusters meeting this threshold, average signal was extracted and post hoc ANOVAs and Tukey-corrected analyses were performed in commercial software (PASW 18.0.1). Anatomic locations were labeled using the Talairach-Tournoux Daemon.

Exploratory post hoc ANOVAs were conducted to examine the impact of potentially confounding variables. For mood state, analyses were conducted in euthymic patients only and in HVs. To test the effect of comorbid anxiety disorders, ANOVAs were used to compare nonanxious SMD patients, nonanxious BD patients, and HVs. For medication, we conducted an ANOVA on BD and SMD participants separately with medication status (with vs without) as the between-group variable. We additionally covaried medication status in the clusters where BD and SMD differed and explored the potential impact of age, sex, and pubertal status (Tanner score), as well as Child Depression Rating Scale scores (eText).

Results
Demographics

The BD, SMD, and HV participants did not differ significantly in age, sex, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence IQ, or Children's Global Assessment Scale score. Participants with BD had higher Child Depression Rating Scale scores than those with SMD (F1,31 = 5.86, P = .02) (Table 1).

Behavior
Neutral→Angry

For the linear trends of angry morph ratings, there was no group × behavior interaction or group main effect. There was a main effect of behavior, with hostile ratings showing a steeper positive slope than nose-width ratings (F1,54 = 117.68, P < .001). This indicates that participants used a broader range of the 1 to 5 scale for hostility ratings than for nose-width ratings.

Linear trend analyses on RT values showed no group × behavior interactions for angry morphs. To assess absolute RT differences between groups, we also conducted a group × behavior × morph ANOVA, and no significant group differences emerged.

Neutral→Happy

For the linear trends of happy ratings, the group × behavior interaction was significant (F2,54 = 4. 36, P = .02). Follow-up ANOVAs demonstrated a group difference for hostile, but not nose-width, ratings of happy morphs (F2,54 = 7.76, P < .001). Two Tukey honestly significant difference–corrected post hoc analyses of the hostile ratings showed that the linear trend for BD patients differed from that of both SMD patients (P = .04) and HVs (P < .001), with BD patients modulating their ratings less than the SMD and HV youth.

Linear trend analyses on RT values showed no group × behavior interactions for happy morphs. To assess absolute RT differences between groups we also conducted a group × behavior × morph ANOVA. No significant group differences emerged.

Imaging
ROI Analysis
Neutral→Angry

Separate group × behavior ANOVAs were conducted for right and left amygdala on the linear trend magnitudes for N→A. In the left amygdala for N→A, there was no group × behavior interaction and no main effect of behavior. However, there was a main effect of group (F2,54 = 3.69, P = .03). Collapsed across the 2 task conditions, mean (SD) linear trend values for the groups were BD, 0.003 (0.021); SMD, 0.001 (0.023); and HV, 0.069 (0.019), indicating stronger modulation by facial emotion features in HVs than in either patient group. Between-group Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses revealed a trend, with HVs differing from BD (P = .06) and SMD (P = .07) patients. In addition, within-group contrasts were conducted with 1-sample t tests. Only HVs had a significant slope across N→A (t22 = 3.3, P = .003) (Figure 3), with an increase in amygdala BOLD activity with increasing anger in the face. For BD and SMD participants, the slope did not differ from zero. In the right amygdala, there was no group × behavior interaction, and there were no main effects.

Neutral→Happy

Separate group × behavior ANOVAs were conducted for right and left amygdala on the linear trend magnitudes for N→H. There was no group × behavior interaction, and there were no main effects in either amygdala.

Whole-Brain Analysis
Neutral→Angry

At the whole-brain level, no clusters showed a group × behavior interaction for N→A. Left posterior cingulate showed a main effect of group (F2,54 = 11.50, P < .001) (Table 2). Three Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses showed that HVs had a more positive slope than BD (P < .001) and SMD (P = .02) participants.

Neutral → Happy

At the whole-brain level, no clusters showed a group × behavior interaction for N→H. However, 4 clusters showed a main effect of group (Table 2): right inferior parietal lobule (Brodmann area [BA]40/7), left middle occipital gyrus and fusiform gyrus (BA37), right middle occipital gyrus and cuneus (BA18/19), and left middle/superior frontal gyrus (BA6/8) (overall F>9.3, P < .001 for all comparisons). In each region, Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses showed that the between-group difference was driven by BD having a more negative slope than SMD (P < .001 for all comparisons) and HV (P < .05 for all comparisons except for left middle occipital gyrus, P = .07) and by SMD having a more positive slope than HV (P < .05 for all comparisons) (Figure 4).

Effect of Potential Confounding Variables
Effects of Mood State

A post hoc analysis in the left amygdala was conducted including only euthymic BD (n = 16) and SMD (n = 15) participants. There remained a group effect (F2,51 = 3.29, P = .03), with Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses showing HVs differing from BD (trend, P = .06) and SMD (P = .03) patients for N→A. A similar analysis in the whole-brain clusters found significant between-group differences. Thus, these analyses revealed no effect of current mood state on our findings.

Effects of Comorbid/Co-occurring Illness

An ANOVA comparing nonanxious SMD (n = 8), nonanxious BD (n = 11), and HV participants on the left amygdala N→A showed a group effect (F2,39 = 3.46, P = .04). Tukey-corrected post hoc analyses showed a significant difference between nonanxious BD patients and HVs (P = .04); nonanxious SMD patients did not differ significantly from either group.

In an ANOVA comparing nonanxious SMD, nonanxious BD, and HV participants for N→A in the posterior cingulate and in the clusters identified in the whole-brain N→H, most of the group differences remained (see eText). Thus, excluding BD and SMD participants with comorbid anxiety disorder lessened some of the group differences, but this effect may be the result of loss of statistical power.

Effects of Medication

The ANOVAs assessed medication effects on left amygdala N→A linear trends. For SMD, an ANOVA comparing unmedicated SMD (n = 6) with medicated SMD patients (n = 8) found no significant difference (P = .47). Only 2 BD participants were not receiving medication. However, correlations between the number of medications and activation in all BD patients on the amygdala and each whole-brain cluster were not significant. We also found no significant differences between SMD patients with vs without medication in any of the whole-brain clusters (P > .16 for all comparisons).

Comment

Research shows that both SMD and BD youth have facial emotion–processing deficits6-9 and amygdala dysfunction on face-viewing tasks.10 However, studies have not explored amygdala modulation in response to varying facial emotion intensity.13-17 Moreover, because only 1 prior brain-imaging study compared these 2 patient groups directly,10 questions remain regarding similarities and differences of face-elicited amygdala dysfunction in BD vs SMD patients. In the present study, we used a novel parametric design to compare BOLD activity in BD, SMD, and HV participants while they made implicit or explicit ratings of emotional faces. We examined modulation of activity with changes in the degree of emotion across faces whose expressions ranged from neutral to 100% angry (N→A) or 100% happy (N→H), in 25% increments.

During implicit and explicit ratings of the N→A morphs, only HVs showed increasing amygdala activity with increasing anger intensity, as has been found using fear faces in healthy adults.29 The fact that neither BD nor SMD youth exhibited this relationship suggests similar amygdala dysfunction in these groups. Of note, amygdala dysfunction was present during hostility and nose-width ratings combined, consistent with studies in BD demonstrating abnormalities during both implicit and explicit facial emotion–processing conditions.13,15,17,20,22 These data suggest that, in youth with SMD or BD vs HVs, there is diminished amygdala responsiveness to changes in facial emotion across several attentional conditions. This decreased amygdala responsiveness is consistent with behavioral data demonstrating that, compared with HVs, SMD and BD patients perform poorly on facial emotion–labeling tasks,6 requiring more intense emotional information before identifying facial emotions.11

The BD and SMD patients differed from each other, and from HVs, in behavioral and neural responses to neutral faces as they morphed toward happy. The HV youth, but not those with BD or SMD, exhibited increasing amygdala activation in response to increasing facial emotion intensity. Also, compared with HVs and SMD patients, BD patients perceived these faces to have a narrower range of hostility. A failure to interpret facial stimuli as having a broad range of hostility may have resulted in the lack of relationship between amygdala response and facial emotion intensity. Alternatively, amygdala and perceived hostility ratings may be driven by an unknown third variable. Although youths with SMD also exhibited a lack of amygdala modulation, they did not display a restricted range of hostility ratings. This may indicate different pathophysiologic mechanisms between the 2 patient groups, warranting additional study.

In this parametric task, we varied the level of emotion depicted in facial stimuli and examined between-group differences in the linear slope describing activation in response to each of the face morphs; this differs from more typical analyses that compare mean activation between groups. Because parametric designs model linear changes along a continuum, they may be more sensitive to subtle between-group differences in activation patterns. Our design, which includes different gradations of emotional expressions, may be particularly useful in research on amygdala function, since the amygdala habituates rapidly to repeated presentations of identical stimuli. However, our results cannot be compared directly with those from studies that report amygdala hyperactivation15,17 or hypoactivation10 in response to faces displaying 100% emotion or in response to a limited number of morphs. Nonetheless, our results are, broadly speaking, consistent with prior studies in youth and adults with BD that find amygdala dysfunction during facial emotion processing,10,14-17,22,30-36 as well as with a prior study in SMD youth.10 Additionally, our data need to be interpreted in light of the tasks used—ratings of hostility and nose width—not with emotion labeling.

At the whole-brain level for N→A, only 1 cluster differentiated groups, that is, in the left posterior cingulate (BA29/30). The positive slope in HVs indicates increased posterior cingulate activation in association with increased anger on the face, perhaps suggesting more effortful processing. In contrast, the negative slopes in BD and SMD suggest that increasing anger in a face is associated with relative disengagement. The posterior cingulate is activated by emotional stimuli37; given its connections with amygdala, insula, orbitofrontal cortex, and parahippocampal regions,38 it is well placed to modulate motivation and spatial attention.39 Data suggest posterior cingulate dysfunction in adult BD while viewing sad40 and happy faces.41 To our knowledge, this is the first study in pediatric BD to report dysfunction in posterior cingulate activity while viewing emotional faces. As in adult BD, such dysfunction in youth could be related to the emotional and attention dysregulation that is central to both BD and SMD.

For N→A morphs, BD and SMD patients demonstrated similar dysfunction relative to HVs in the amygdala ROI and whole-brain analyses. In contrast, for N→H across both implicit and explicit conditions, there were activation differences between BD and SMD in 4 regions important for attention and visual processing: right inferior parietal lobule, left middle occipital/fusiform gyrus, right middle occipital gyrus, and left middle/superior frontal gyrus. These findings are interesting given research demonstrating BD neural dysfunction elicited by happy facial stimuli.33,34,42-44 As neutral faces morphed toward happy, each group showed a characteristic and unique pattern of activity across regions; specifically, BD decreased in activity (negative slope), SMD increased in activity (positive slope), and HV did not modulate activity. Mood states fluctuate in patients with BD, and SMD is characterized by a persistent negative mood state; therefore, we speculate that the processing of positive stimuli may be particularly demanding in patients with chronic, severe irritability,2 that is, those with SMD.

As with the posterior cingulate findings, these results should be viewed with both caution and interest. Caution is warranted given the lack of consistent prior face-viewing imaging work implicating these regions in SMD or BD. Interest is also warranted given evidence implicating these structures in processes relevant to SMD and BD.10 Specifically, in healthy individuals, both inferior parietal lobule and middle occipital/fusiform gyrus are key in face processing.45-47 Additionally, the superior frontal gyrus is active during social emotion processing,48,49 and the inferior parietal lobule mediates attention.50,51

Depression in adult BD has been associated with dysfunction in anatomic and functional connectivity between the amygdala and frontal areas52 and between frontal areas.26 In the present study, depression scores were higher in BD vs SMD youth, and BD and SMD patients showed opposite patterns of frontal activity on N→H with increasing amounts of happiness. Although the between-group differences remained in post hoc analyses covarying Child Depression Rating Scale scores (see eText), the effect of mood state on these findings merits further research.

Mood state, comorbidity, and medication complicate research on BD and SMD. Most BD patients (84%) were euthymic, and results were unchanged when the analysis included only euthymic patients. When only nonanxious BD (n = 11) and SMD (n = 8) patients were included, the differences between BD and HV participants remained but, perhaps because of type II error, SMD patients did not differ from the other 2 groups. This demonstrates the need for a larger sample of nonanxious SMD patients. Because most patients were receiving medication, we cannot rule out the possibility that medication influenced the blunted amygdala response in N→A seen for the BD and SMD patients. The 6 unmedicated vs 8 medicated SMD patients did not differ significantly in amygdala or whole-brain cluster slope values, although this could be the result of insufficient power. When medication status was covaried from the 4 N→H clusters where BD and SMD differed, the groups remained significantly distinct from each other. Given the post hoc nature of this analysis and the small sample sizes, replication in a larger sample is needed. Although it would be ideal to study patients not receiving medication, it is neither ethical nor feasible to test all patients in the medication-free state. Because our paradigm included only angry and happy morphs, we cannot generalize beyond these 2 emotions, eg, to fear or sadness. In addition, prior behavioral studies6-9 demonstrated facial emotion–labeling deficits in BD and SMD; however, facial emotion labeling was not assessed directly here. An additional limitation is that we did not systematically assess handedness.

In this novel parametric face paradigm with 2 attention conditions, we found that BD and SMD have similarities (amygdala) and differences (parietal and medial frontal areas) in BOLD activity dysfunction vs HVs. These data suggest that amygdala activity in both groups does not change in response to increasing amounts of anger. In contrast, in response to increasing amounts of happiness, BD and SMD patients have differing directions of dysfunction in frontal and parietal regions. The current work adds to data demonstrating that BD and SMD have clinical, neuropsychological, and pathophysiologic similarities as well as differences,10,28 suggesting that SMD and BD may be manifestations of differing abnormalities along a mood disorders spectrum. Further work is needed to examine the role of these neural correlates as they relate to differences in clinical outcomes.

The data established in this study cannot be used for diagnostic purposes, but they support the growing need to compare pediatric BD with SMD. Both are severe mood disorders, but SMD patients differ from those with BD in that only BD patients experience manic episodes (presenting instead with nonepisodic irritability) and, longitudinally, SMD patients develop depressive and anxiety disorders.4,5,44,53 Further work can aid in distinguishing BD from SMD pathophysiologically, which has important diagnostic and treatment implications for children receiving care in community clinics for severe irritability and emotional dysregulation.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Laura A. Thomas, PhD, Office of the Director, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 6001 Executive Blvd, Room 5252, Mailstop 9581, Rockville, MD 20852 (tlaura@mail.nih.gov).

Submitted for Publication: October 6, 2011; final revision received June 1, 2012; accepted June 11, 2012.

Published Online: October 1, 2012. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.913

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health.

References
1.
Moreno C, Laje G, Blanco C, Jiang H, Schmidt AB, Olfson M. National trends in the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar disorder in youth.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(9):1032-103917768268PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Leibenluft E, Charney DS, Towbin KE, Bhangoo RK, Pine DS. Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(3):430-43712611821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Brotman MA, Kassem L, Reising MM, Guyer AE, Dickstein DP, Rich BA, Towbin KE, Pine DS, McMahon FJ, Leibenluft E. Parental diagnoses in youth with narrow phenotype bipolar disorder or severe mood dysregulation.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(8):1238-124117671287PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, Costello EJ, Egger HL, Angold A, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(9):991-99717056393PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Stringaris A, Baroni A, Haimm C, Brotman M, Lowe CH, Myers F, Rustgi E, Wheeler W, Kayser R, Towbin K, Leibenluft E. Pediatric bipolar disorder versus severe mood dysregulation: risk for manic episodes on follow-up.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(4):397-40520410732PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler AD, Brotman MA, Rich BA, Kimes AS, Pine DS, Ernst M, Leibenluft E. Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood psychopathology.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(9):863-87117714371PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Rich BA, Grimley ME, Schmajuk M, Blair KS, Blair RJ, Leibenluft E. Face emotion labeling deficits in children with bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation.  Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20(2):529-54618423093PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Schenkel LS, Pavuluri MN, Herbener ES, Harral EM, Sweeney JA. Facial emotion processing in acutely ill and euthymic patients with pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(8):1070-107917667485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
McClure EB, Treland JE, Snow J, Schmajuk M, Dickstein DP, Towbin KE, Charney DS, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(9):1644-165116135623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Brotman MA, Rich BA, Guyer AE, Lunsford JR, Horsey SE, Reising MM, Thomas LA, Fromm SJ, Towbin K, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Amygdala activation during emotion processing of neutral faces in children with severe mood dysregulation versus ADHD or bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(1):61-6919917597PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Rich BA, Schmajuk M, Perez-Edgar KE, Fox NA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Different psychophysiological and behavioral responses elicited by frustration in pediatric bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(2):309-31717267795PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Darwin C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Reprint ed. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1965. Original work published in 1872
13.
Kalmar JH, Wang F, Chepenik LG, Womer FY, Jones MM, Pittman B, Shah MP, Martin A, Constable RT, Blumberg HP. Relation between amygdala structure and function in adolescents with bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(6):636-64219454919PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN. Fronto-limbic dysfunction in mania pre-treatment and persistent amygdala over-activity post-treatment in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;216(4):485-49921390505PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Pavuluri MN, O’Connor MM, Harral E, Sweeney JA. Affective neural circuitry during facial emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(2):158-16717097071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Pavuluri MN, Passarotti A. Neural bases of emotional processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Expert Rev Neurother. 2008;8(9):1381-138718759550PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Pavuluri MN, Passarotti AM, Harral EM, Sweeney JA. An fMRI study of the neural correlates of incidental versus directed emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(3):308-31919242292PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Olsavsky AK, Brotman MA, Rutenberg JG, Muhrer EJ, Deveney CM, Fromm SJ, Towbin K, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Amygdala hyperactivation during face emotion processing in bipolar disorder and children at familial risk for bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(3):294-30322365465PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder.  Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(9):829, 833-85718574483PubMedGoogle Scholar
20.
Rich BA, Vinton DT, Roberson-Nay R, Hommer RE, Berghorst LH, McClure EB, Fromm SJ, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Limbic hyperactivation during processing of neutral facial expressions in children with bipolar disorder.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(23):8900-890516735472PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;9(2):93-10510680766PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN. Differential engagement of cognitive and affective neural systems in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(1):106-11719849880PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980-9889204677PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ekman P, Friesen W. Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists; 1976
25.
LaBar KS, Crupain MJ, Voyvodic JT, McCarthy G. Dynamic perception of facial affect and identity in the human brain.  Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(10):1023-103312967919PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brian. New York, NY: Thieme; 1988
27.
Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.  Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29(3):162-1738812068PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Adleman NE, Kayser R, Dickstein D, Blair RJ, Pine D, Leibenluft E. Neural correlates of reversal learning in severe mood dysregulation and pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(11):1173-1185Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AW, Calder AJ, Dolan RJ. A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions.  Nature. 1996;383(6603):812-8158893004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Chen CH, Lennox B, Jacob R, Calder A, Lupson V, Bisbrown-Chippendale R, Suckling J, Bullmore E. Explicit and implicit facial affect recognition in manic and depressed states of bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(1):31-3916112653PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Foland LC, Altshuler LL, Bookheimer SY, Eisenberger N, Townsend J, Thompson PM. Evidence for deficient modulation of amygdala response by prefrontal cortex in bipolar mania.  Psychiatry Res. 2008;162(1):27-3718063349PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Surguladze S, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, Andrew C, Frangou S, Ecker C, Phillips ML. Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(6):578-58715013826PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Surguladze SA, Marshall N, Schulze K, Hall MH, Walshe M, Bramon E, Phillips ML, Murray RM, McDonald C. Exaggerated neural response to emotional faces in patients with bipolar disorder and their first-degree relatives.  Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):58-6420595014PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Versace A, Thompson WK, Zhou D, Almeida JR, Hassel S, Klein CR, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Abnormal left and right amygdala-orbitofrontal cortical functional connectivity to emotional faces: state versus trait vulnerability markers of depression in bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):422-43120159144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Strakowski SM, Eliassen JC, Lamy M, Cerullo MA, Allendorfer JB, Madore M, Lee JH, Welge JA, DelBello MP, Fleck DE, Adler CM. Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain activation in bipolar mania: evidence for disruption of the ventrolateral prefrontal-amygdala emotional pathway.  Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(4):381-38821051038PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Almeida JR, Versace A, Hassel S, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Elevated amygdala activity to sad facial expressions: a state marker of bipolar but not unipolar depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(5):414-42119931855PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18(1):30-4112454910PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Morris R, Paxinos G, Petrides M. Architectonic analysis of the human retrosplenial cortex.  J Comp Neurol. 2000;421(1):14-2810813770PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Mohanty A, Gitelman DR, Small DM, Mesulam MM. The spatial attention network interacts with limbic and monoaminergic systems to modulate motivation-induced attention shifts.  Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2604-261318308706PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, Lupson V, Bullmore ET. Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):795-80215500300PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Phillips ML, Bullmore ET, Howard R, Woodruff PW, Wright IC, Williams SC, Simmons A, Andrew C, Brammer M, David AS. Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study.  Psychiatry Res. 1998;83(3):127-1389849722PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Almeida JR, Versace A, Mechelli A, Hassel S, Quevedo K, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Abnormal amygdala-prefrontal effective connectivity to happy faces differentiates bipolar from major depression.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):451-45919450794PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Hassel S, Almeida JR, Kerr N, Nau S, Ladouceur CD, Fissell K, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Elevated striatal and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in response to emotional stimuli in euthymic bipolar disorder: no associations with psychotropic medication load.  Bipolar Disord. 2008;10(8):916-92719594507PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Hassel S, Almeida JR, Frank E, Versace A, Nau SA, Klein CR, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Prefrontal cortical and striatal activity to happy and fear faces in bipolar disorder is associated with comorbid substance abuse and eating disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2009;118(1-3):19-2719243839PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Radua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, El-Hage W, McDonald C, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, David AS, Surguladze SA. Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults.  Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):939-94619699306PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Przuntek H. Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc Biol Sci. 1998;265(1409):1927-19319821359PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Kanwisher N. Domain specificity in face perception.  Nat Neurosci. 2000;3(8):759-76310903567PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Farrow TF, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JF, Tarrier N, Griffiths PD, Woodruff PW. Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness.  Neuroreport. 2001;12(11):2433-243811496124PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Beer JS, Ochsner KN. Social cognition: a multi level analysis.  Brain Res. 2006;1079(1):98-10516513097PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E. Visual attention: insights from brain imaging.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2000;1(2):91-10011252779PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Maunsell JH. Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention?  Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(6):261-26515165551PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Cerullo MA, Fleck DE, Eliassen JC, Smith MS, DelBello MP, Adler CM, Strakowski SM. A longitudinal functional connectivity analysis of the amygdala in bipolar I disorder across mood states.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(2):175-18422420593PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Leibenluft E. Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(2):129-14221123313PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×