Context Decreased brain serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) levels are considered to mediate depressive relapse induced by the tryptophan depletion paradigm. However, in patients who recently achieved remission from a major depressive episode with antidepressant treatment, only about half become depressed following tryptophan depletion. We hypothesized that downregulation of brain serotonin2 receptors might be a compensatory mechanism that prevents some patients from becoming depressed with tryptophan depletion.
Objective To assess, with use of positron emission tomography, whether brain serotonin2 receptor downregulation occurs in patients with recently remitted depression who do not have depressive relapse, but not in those who become depressed, following tryptophan depletion.
Design Each patient underwent 2 fluorine 18–labeled– setoperone positron emission tomography scans, one following a tryptophan depletion session and another following a control session. The order of scanning was counterbalanced.
Setting Academic university hospital with imaging facilities.
Participants Seventeen patients in recent remission from a DSM-IV major depressive episode following treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.
Main Outcome Measures Changes in brain serotonin2 receptor binding.
Results Of the 17 patients, 8 (47%) became depressed during the tryptophan depletion session, and none developed depression during the control session. The depletion session was associated with a significant reduction in brain serotonin2 receptor binding compared with the control session for all participants. A subgroup analysis revealed that the reduction in serotonin2 receptor binding was significant only for the nondepressed group.
Conclusion Reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors might be a potential compensatory mechanism to prevent tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse.
The tryptophan depletion paradigm has been used extensively to investigate the neurobiology of depression and other psychiatric disorders.1-5 Preclinical studies5,6 in animals suggest that the tryptophan depletion challenge paradigm reduces plasma and brain free and total tryptophan levels, with a consequent reduction in brain serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) levels. Studies2,7,8 in humans have shown that ingestion of an amino acid mixture containing 15 amino acids without tryptophan induces a marked reduction in plasma total and free tryptophan levels within 5 hours. Furthermore, a positron emission tomography (PET) study9 has shown that tryptophan depletion leads to a reduction in brain serotonin synthesis.
The effects of tryptophan depletion on mood in healthy volunteers have been fairly consistent, with most studies reporting no changes.10,11 In contrast, tryptophan depletion has been reported to induce significant depressive symptoms in patients who recently achieved remission from a major depressive episode following treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs).7,12,13 This finding has been interpreted to suggest that adequate levels of brain serotonin are necessary to maintain the antidepressant effects of SSRIs. If such were to be the case, all patients with recent remission from depression resulting from SSRI treatment should become depressed with tryptophan depletion, as serotonin depletion likely occurs in those who ingest the amino acid mixture. However, studies11 have indicated that only approximately 50% of patients in remission become depressed following tryptophan depletion. This would suggest that those who do not become depressed may mount a neurobiological compensatory mechanism to counter the effects of tryptophan depletion and thus prevent depression. If so, what might that compensatory mechanism be?
Positron emission tomography studies suggest that several effective antidepressant treatments downregulate brain serotonin2 receptors. These include agents with diverse neurobiological mechanisms, such as the norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor desipramine,14 the serotonin reuptake inhibitor paroxetine,15 and the serotonin reuptake inhibitor and serotonin2 antagonist nefazodone,16 as well as somatic intervention electroconvulsive therapy.17 Furthermore, healthy volunteers who did not become depressed following tryptophan depletion have been reported to develop serotonin2 receptor downregulation within 5 hours after ingestion of the tryptophan-deficient amino acid mixture.18 Taken together, these data suggest that downregulation of serotonin2 receptors may be associated with relief from or prevention of depressive symptoms. Hence, serotonin2 receptor downregulation may be a potential compensatory mechanism that might prevent patients with recently remitted depression from becoming depressed following the acute tryptophan depletion paradigm.
In this study, we examined the effects of tryptophan depletion on mood and brain serotonin2 receptors in patients who achieved remission from a major depressive episode with SSRI treatment within the past 3 months. We measured brain serotonin2 receptors using PET with setoperone as a ligand in each participant 5 hours following the ingestion of a tryptophan-deficient amino acid mixture on one day and a balanced amino acid mixture (containing tryptophan) on another day. We hypothesized that brain serotonin2 receptors would be downregulated in patients who did not become depressed during the tryptophan depletion session compared with the control session but not in those who became depressed.
The study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Board of the University of British Columbia. Eligible patients were recently treated for a major depressive episode with an SSRI (fluoxetine, paroxetine, sertraline, citalopram, or escitalopram), currently in remission from depression for a minimum of 1 week but less than 12 weeks, and able to provide informed consent. The diagnosis of major depressive disorder was based on all the clinical information, including a clinical interview and a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM Disorders. Remission was defined as a Hamilton Scale for Depression (HAM-D) score of 12 or less on the 29-item scale19 for at least 1 week. Those taking other psychotropic medications, such as antipsychotics, lithium, valproate, lamotrigine, carbamazepine, or other antidepressants, were excluded. Individuals with substance abuse or other Axis I comorbidity within the past 6 months were also excluded, as were women who were pregnant and those not taking adequate contraceptive precautions. Each participant underwent magnetic resonance imaging to exclude cerebral abnormalities and for coregistration of PET images.
The protocol consisted of PET scanning on 2 days 5 hours after the ingestion of amino acid mixtures. The order of the sessions (control vs tryptophan depletion) was counterbalanced. Both the participants and raters who administered behavioral rating tests were blinded to the condition. The tryptophan depletion session involved ingesting an amino acid mixture consisting of 15 amino acids, without tryptophan. The control session included the same amino acid mixture with the addition of 2.3 g of L-tryptophan. The composition of the amino acid mixture was the same as that used by Delgado et al.13 Chocolate syrup was added to the amino acid mixture drink to make it palatable, but amino acids with an unpleasant taste were administered in a capsule form.
Participants fasted from midnight and arrived at the mood disorders outpatient research program between 7 AM and 8 AM. Clinical assessment and behavioral ratings including a 20-item HAM-D scale (29-item HAM-D scale less 9 items that could not be rated within the same day, eg, sleep, eating, weight, and diurnal variation) were completed to confirm remission of depression. An intravenous cannula was then inserted and a blood sample was drawn to assay for total and free tryptophan. Participants then ingested the amino acid mixture and remained within a room for the next 5 hours reading newspapers or magazines of their choice. Approximately 5 hours later, the patients were escorted to the PET suite and a second blood sample was drawn. Behavioral ratings were completed at that time and PET was started immediately. Relapse of depression was defined as an increase in depression rating scores by 50% or more and a total score of 13 or higher.
Blood samples were immediately processed. Blood was centrifuged for 10 minutes at room temperature at 5000 g. To obtain an ultrafiltrate of plasma, the sample was further centrifuged (2000 g) at room temperature for 30 minutes through a cellulose ultrafiltration membrane system (Amicon Co). The ultrafiltrate samples were frozen at −70°C and were later assayed for free tryptophan levels using high-performance liquid chromatography with fluorometric detection.20 In 2 patients, free tryptophan levels could not be estimated because the ultrafiltrate sample was too small.
The protocol for PET image acquisition was similar to that previously reported.18 Briefly, after a transmission scan to correct PET images for attenuation, each participant was given 148 to 259 MBq of fluorine 18–labeled (18F)–setoperone intravenously. A PET camera system (ECAT 953B/31; CTI/Siemens), which has a field of view of 10.8 cm, was used to measure radioactivity in the brain. The scanning began immediately after injection of 18F-setoperone and lasted for 110 minutes, during which 15 frame-dynamic emission scans were acquired (5 frames, each 2 minutes' duration; then 4 frames, each 5 minutes; then 4 frames, each 10 minutes; and then 2 frames, each 20 minutes). Positron emission tomography was repeated within 7 days so that each patient had 2 scans: one after the control session and the other after the tryptophan depletion session. At the end of each test session, patients were assessed clinically.
Image processing and data analysis
Images from all frames of the first and second scan of each participant were realigned to the image obtained from the last 30 minutes of the data acquired during the first scanning session using the automated image realignment algorithm.21,22 This procedure was done to correct for motion during a scanning sequence and to ensure that the brain structures were in the same position of the image space in the 2 scanning sequences to facilitate voxel-by-voxel comparison. The Logan tissue-input graphical analysis23 was applied to the time activity curve of each voxel separately; the time activity curve of an elliptical region of interest ([ROI] area, 2590 mm2) placed on the cerebellar image was used as an input function. The population-based tissue to plasma efflux constant (k2) for the reference region, required for the Logan analysis, was derived for 18F-setoperone with a 3-tissue compartment model to be k2 = 0.109/min.24 The Logan slope was calculated from data acquired 30 minutes after injection. These data were applied to create a parametric setoperone distribution volume ratio (DVR) image for each scanning session. This resulted in a DVR (DVR = BPND + 1, where BPND indicates nondisplaceable binding potential)25 parametric image for control and depletion sessions for each participant.
Statistical parametric mapping analysis
The Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM5) software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5) was used to coregister each participant's DVR images for control and depletion sessions with that person's magnetic resonance image. Each magnetic resonance image was normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) T1 template in SPM5, and these normalization parameters were applied to DVR images to bring them to the standard MNI template space. The normalized DVR images were smoothed using an isotropic gaussian kernel of 10 mm, full width at half maximum.
The gray matter threshold was set at 1.0 times the mean global cerebral image intensity to exclude most nongray matter voxels in the analysis. A full factorial model with 2 sessions and 2 groups as implemented in SPM5 was used to determine the effect of session, group, and group × session interaction. Post hoc pairwise contrasts included assessing differences in DVRs within responders and nonresponders between the control session and depletion session. Because previous studies18 indicated that any changes were likely to be spatially extensive, the primary analysis used a criterion of cluster significance to provide the greatest statistical power for detecting spatially extensive effects.26 The corrected cluster significance was set at P < .005, and the statistical significance for height threshold for inclusion of contiguous voxels in a cluster was set at P < .005 uncorrected. We also assessed the significance of differences in DVRs in each voxel between the sessions, and a stringent familywise error (FWE) corrected significance was set at P < .05.
Previous studies4,27,28 that examined the effects of tryptophan depletion on cerebral blood or glucose metabolism have consistently shown changes in the medial orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate regions. Hence, these 2 regions were selected for ROI analysis of changes in DVRs of setoperone binding. The Pick_Atlas program (Wake Forest University School of Medicine; http://www.ansir.wfubmc.edu) was used to create masks for anterior cingulate cortex and medial orbitofrontal cortex regions, and these masks were applied to DVR images (tryptophan and depletion scans) to extract DVRs of setoperone binding. We performed repeated-measures analysis of variance to examine the effects of session, group, and group × session interaction. Furthermore, we performed paired t tests to determine whether DVRs were different in depressed or nondepressed groups between the 2 sessions.
Behavioral and plasma measures
A repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance was used to assess the effects of order (ie, those who had the control session first vs those who had the tryptophan depletion session first) as a grouping variable and time (baseline vs postsession) and session (control vs depletion session) as repeated measures. Data are presented as mean (SD) for behavioral measures and mean (SE) for plasma tryptophan levels, and all tests were 2-tailed, with significance set at P < .05.
Of the 20 patients recruited, 3 dropped out after the first PET scan. All 17 patients included in the analysis were in recent remission from a DSM-IV major depressive episode following treatment with SSRIs. The clinical characteristics of the patients are displayed in the Table. The mean age of the 17 (5 men, 12 women) patients who had both control and depletion scans was 41.8 (10.7) years. The patients had a mean of 3.5 (5.3) previous depressive episodes, and the duration of treatment for the current episode was 16.8 (8.6) weeks. The mean duration of remission from the most recent depressive episode was 5.4 (3.6) weeks.
Plasma tryptophan levels and behavioral data
The mean (SE) plasma tryptophan concentrations, both total and free, during the tryptophan and depletion sessions are displayed in Figure 1. There were no significant differences in baseline total (t16 = 1.5; P = .16) and free (t14 = 1.0; P = .34) tryptophan levels between the control and depletion sessions. As expected, there was no main effect for order (F1,15 = 0.9; P = .35) or time (F1,15 = 3.4; P = .08), but there was a significant session × time interaction effect for plasma total tryptophan levels (F1,15 = 33.4; P < .001). Similarly, there was no order (F1,13 = 0.03 ;P = .85) or time (F1,13 = 2.4; P = .14) effect, but a significant session × time interaction (F1,13 = 23.6; P < .001) was observed for free tryptophan levels. Plasma total and free tryptophan levels were significantly lower 5 hours after ingestion of amino acids in the depletion session (total tryptophan reduced by 75%; free tryptophan reduced by 66%), and the levels were significantly higher in the control session (total tryptophan increased by 46%; free tryptophan increased by 99%) (Figure 1).
There were no significant differences in 29-item HAM-D scores at baseline between control (5.5 [4.1]) and depletion (4.2 [3.1]) (t16 = 1.1; P = .30) sessions. The changes in HAM-D scores for participants during each session are depicted in Figure 2. There was no main effect for order (F1,15 = 0.1; P = .91) or session (F1,15 = 2.2; P = .15), but there was a significant effect for time (F1,15 = 7.5; P = .01) and for session × time interaction (F1,15 = 14.0; P = .002). Paired t tests showed that the HAM-D scores were significantly higher in the depletion session relative to baseline (10.8 [7.8] vs 4.24 [3.1]; t16 = 3.9; P = .001) but not in the control session (5.9 [4.9] vs 5.5 [4.1]; t16 = 0.4; P = .73). Eight patients met HAM-D criteria for relapse during the depletion session (the depressed group), and 9 patients did not experience relapse (the nondepressed group). All participants who experienced relapse were women (8 of 12); none of the 5 men relapsed, and this difference was significant (P = .03, Fisher exact test). However, none of the women experienced relapse during the control session. There were no significant differences in any clinical characteristics (Table) or total or free plasma tryptophan levels between those who experienced relapse and those who did not.
Statistical Parametric Mapping analysis of DVRs of setoperone binding revealed a significant session effect but no group effect or group × session interaction. The DVRs were significantly lower in the depletion session compared with the control session as revealed by an extensive cluster of voxels embracing right frontal, left medial frontal, right temporal, parietal, and occipital regions, as well as left medial parietal cortical regions (Figure 3). The cluster included 13 571 voxels, and the reduction in DVRs in this cluster was highly significant, even after correction for multiple comparisons (P < .001). The mean reduction in DVR for the cluster was 13%. There were 2 voxels in the cluster that survived the FWE correction, and these were located in the right superior temporal gyrus and right uncus. There were no significant differences in DVRs between the first and second scans, indicating that scanning order had no systematic effect on DVRs.
Post hoc contrasts assessed whether DVRs were different in the depletion session compared with the control session in the nondepressed and depressed groups. The DVRs were significantly lower in the depletion session in the nondepressed group but not in the depressed group. The SPM analysis revealed a cluster of 14 097 voxels in which DVRs were significantly lower (corrected P < .001) in the nondepressed group, and this cluster embraced right frontal, left medial frontal, right temporal and parietal, and right and left occipital regions (Figures 4, 5, and 6). The mean reduction in DVRs in the cluster was 13%.
There were also several independent voxels that met criteria for an FWE-corrected significance, and these were located in the right superior and inferior temporal gyri, right medial frontal gyrus, right middle occipital gyrus, and left middle frontal gyrus. The reduction in binding in these voxels ranged from 8.4% to 13.2%. In the group who became depressed, decrease in DVR was observed in a cluster of only 4 voxels (uncorrected P = .09). A cluster of at least this size would be very likely to occur under the null hypothesis. Furthermore, the peak z value within this cluster was only 2.62 (P = .56; FWE).
Consistent with the SPM analysis, the ROI analysis revealed a significant effect for session for both anterior cingulate (F1,15 = 14.2; P = .002) and medial orbitofrontal cortex (F1,15 = 8.8; P = .01) but no effect for group (anterior cingulate, F1,15 = 3.3; P = .09; medial orbitofrontal cortex, F1,15 = 3.5; P = .08) or group × session interaction (anterior cingulate, F1,15 = 0.01; P = .99; medial orbitofrontal cortex, F1,15 = 0.1; P = .75) . The percentage change in setoperone binding in the anterior cingulate region for the depressed and nondepressed groups is displayed in Figure 7. Post hoc paired t tests showed that setoperone binding was significantly decreased in the depletion session compared with the control session in the nondepressed group (anterior cingulate, t8 = 3.8; P = .005; medial orbitofrontal cortex, t8 = 3.1; P = .02) but not in the depressed group (anterior cingulate, t7, = 2.1; P = .08; medial orbitofrontal cortex, t7 = 1.7; P = .13). There were no correlations between changes in depression scores and reduction in setoperone binding in the anterior cingulate (Pearson r = 0.11; P = .64) or medial orbitofrontal cortex (Pearson r = 0.40; P = .10). There was also no correlation between duration of remission and changes in setoperone binding in the anterior cingulate (r = 0.29; P = .91) or medial orbitofrontal cortex (r = 0.02; P = .93).
To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the effects of tryptophan depletion on brain serotonin2 receptors in subjects recently remitted from major depression following treatment with SSRIs. The major findings are (1) 47% of patients experienced relapse of depressive symptoms in the depletion session compared with none in the control session; (2) a significant reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors, as indicated by a reduction in DVRs, was observed in the depletion session compared with the control session for the entire sample, notably by a large cluster of voxels that occupied right frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital, and left medial frontal and parietal cortical regions; and (3) in contrast to patients who became depressed, those who did not become depressed during tryptophan depletion showed a robust reduction in the similar brain regions in serotonin2 binding compared with the control session.
Previous studies3,7,11 of tryptophan depletion in patients with recently remitted depression achieved with SSRI treatment reported that approximately half the patients experienced transient relapse of depressive symptoms. The finding in the present study of 47% of patients experiencing a depressive relapse is thus consistent with findings of previous tryptophan depletion studies. Furthermore, the fact that depressive relapse occurred in 8 of 12 women, but in none of 5 men, is also consistent with previous studies29 in that women are more vulnerable to tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapses than are men. The tryptophan depletion led to reduction in both total and free plasma tryptophan levels in all participants, with no significant differences in those who became depressed compared with those who did not. Hence, changes in plasma tryptophan levels cannot explain why some patients became depressed during tryptophan depletion while others did not.
Reduction in plasma tryptophan levels is expected to reduce brain tryptophan levels, with a consequent reduction in brain serotonin synthesis. The receptor regulation theory predicts an increase in brain postsynaptic serotonin receptors as a consequence of a reduction in brain serotonin levels. Thus, the finding of a reduction in brain serotonin2 receptor binding in this study after tryptophan depletion is at odds with the classic concepts of receptor pharmacologic action. However, a reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors as a consequence of lower brain serotonin levels is consistent with the well-documented paradoxical regulation of brain serotonin2 receptors, since previous studies30 reported that both agonists and some antagonists downregulate these receptors. Furthermore, the finding of a reduction in serotonin2 receptor binding in the present study following tryptophan depletion is also consistent with the results of the 2 previous studies18,31 of tryptophan depletion in healthy volunteers.
The reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors was observed in an extensive cluster of voxels that embraced bilateral frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital regions in healthy volunteers who did not become depressed following tryptophan depletion.18 The reduction in serotonin2 binding during the tryptophan depletion session was also observed for the entire sample in the present study. However, the reduction in binding was less extensive bilaterally and was more prominent in the right cortical regions. The spatial extent of reduction in serotonin2 receptors observed in SPM analysis depends on the threshold used for inclusion of voxels. Therefore, it is not possible to draw any confident conclusion regarding the lesser spatial extent of the reduction in serotonin2 binding observed in patients with remitted depression compared with that observed in healthy individuals, especially in light of the fact that this study did not include a healthy comparison group. Notwithstanding these limitations, the results of this study may suggest that healthy individuals are more efficient at mounting compensatory mechanisms to reduce brain serotonin2 receptors than are patients with recently remitted depression. If such were the case, the less-extensive reduction of brain serotonin2 receptors in the patient group may explain why only about half of them became depressed during tryptophan depletion. Indeed, post hoc contrasts revealed a significant reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors in patients who did not become depressed with tryptophan depletion but not in those who became depressed. Furthermore, the reduction in serotonin2 binding in the nondepressed group was more extensive than the reduction in the right cortical region observed for the entire sample, since it additionally included the left medial frontal, lateral frontal, orbitofrontal, and left temporal regions. These data suggest that the more extensive reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors in both the right and left cortical regions is more effective in preventing tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse.
Alternatively, it is conceivable that the reductions in brain serotonin2 receptors in certain brain regions might be more critical than others for providing a compensatory protection against tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse. If such were the case, what might be those regions? Previous studies of cerebral blood and glucose metabolism have shown that tryptophan depletion is consistently associated with changes in neural activity in brain regions such as the orbitofrontal, lateral frontal, and cingulate cortices. For instance, tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse is associated with reduced neural activity in the orbitofrontal cortex,4,27 ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, pregenual cingulate,27 and ventral anterior cingulate cortical regions.4 However, another study28 found no significant differences in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rates during tryptophan depletion between patients who became depressed and those who did not but instead reported an increase in glucose metabolic rates in comparison with healthy individuals in the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior and posterior cingulate cortical regions, medial thalamus, and ventral striatum. Thus, these data suggest that changes in the orbitofrontal and cingulate regions appear to be consistent correlates of tryptophan depletion. Consistent with this, the ROI analysis performed in the present study indicated significant reductions in serotonin2 receptor binding in the medial orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, suggesting that the changes in serotonin2 receptors in these regions might be more critical for prevention of depression.
The findings of this study are limited first by the fact that we did not find a significant effect for group and session interaction. This may be because all participants mounted a compensatory reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors in response to tryptophan depletion, but in some the extent of reduction or magnitude of reduction in critical areas was not sufficient to prevent transient relapse of depressive symptoms. This is supported by the observation that the extent of reduction in binding was greater in the nondepressed group compared with the reduction observed for the entire sample. Furthermore, the reduction in binding in potential critical areas, such as the anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal regions, was slightly more than 6% in the nondepressed group and was less than 3% for the depressed group. The sample size of subgroups was small and hence may not have adequate statistical power to detect significant differences between the 2 groups. Second, because all participants who had depressive relapse in the present study were women and the magnitude of reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors was smaller in this group, one could argue that this may be related to a sex effect in that women are less efficient at mounting a compensatory response. Given that the study included only 4 women who did not have a depressive relapse, this sample size does not permit a meaningful analysis of sex effect. However, a widespread and robust reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors occurred in a PET study of healthy volunteers,18 and all participants in that study were women, which makes the sex effect an unlikely explanation for the findings of the present study. Third, we did not use arterial sampling for input function to estimate DVR for setoperone binding. Instead, we used the cerebellum as a reference region, and there is a suggestion that nonspecific binding of setoperone is slightly different between the cerebellum and cortex. However, a previous study32 showed that setoperone binding potential estimated using the cerebellum as a reference region has a high correlation with the estimate using arterial input function, thus validating this method. Furthermore, several studies15,33,34 used the cerebellum as a reference region to estimate setoperone binding potential. Fourth, the binding of some PET ligands to receptors is affected by changes in the levels of endogenous neurotransmitter.35,36 Therefore, one could argue that changes in brain serotonin levels during the tryptophan depletion session might have accounted for the reduction in serotonin2 binding observed in the present study. However, such is not likely because tryptophan depletion is expected to reduce brain serotonin levels, which would leave more serotonin2 receptors available for setoperone binding. In such circumstances, one would expect to see an increase rather than a reduction in setoperone binding observed in the participants in this study. Finally, we cannot ascertain whether the reduction in setoperone binding observed was due to changes in affinity or receptor internalization or density of receptors because the methods used in this study cannot provide an independent determination of these measures. However, previous studies37,38 of the effects of pharmacologic treatments have shown that the changes in binding are due to receptor density and not affinity; thus, it is likely that the reduction in setoperone binding observed in the present study is the result of either downregulation or internalization of brain serotonin2 receptors.
Notwithstanding the limitations, the findings of the present study suggest that reduction in brain serotonin2 receptors may be the compensatory mechanism that prevents tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse. In patients with recently remitted depression treated with SSRIs, reduction of brain serotonin levels is likely to lead to relapse of depression, particularly in women, unless they are able to mount a compensatory mechanism to sufficiently reduce brain serotonin2 receptors. The findings of this study have important clinical implications because they raise the possibility that SSRIs given in conjunction with agents that block serotonin2 receptors (eg, atypical antipsychotics) may be more effective in sustaining remission in depressed patients. If confirmed in further studies, the findings of this study also have important implications for understanding of the neurobiology factors of depression and its treatment.
Correspondence: Lakshmi N. Yatham, MBBS, FRCPC, Mood Disorders Program, Department of Psychiatry, Detwiller Pavilion, Room 2C7, The University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 2A1, Canada (yatham@exchange.ubc.ca).
Submitted for Publication: May 19, 2011; final revision received October 26, 2011; accepted October 27, 2011.
Author Contributions: Dr Sossi had full access to all the data and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Financial Disclosure: Dr Yatham is a member of the speaker/advisory boards for, or has received research grants from, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research, Pfizer, Servier, and the Stanley Foundation. Dr Lam is on the speaker/advisory boards for, or has received research grants from, Aquaceutica, AstraZeneca, Biovail Pharmaceuticals, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Institute of Mental Health/Coast Capital Savings, Janssen, Litebook Company, Ltd, Lundbeck, Lundbeck Institute, Pfizer, Servier, and St. Jude Medical. Dr Liddle has been a speaker for, received research grants from, and/or served on advisory boards for AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Janssen, Lilly, and Novartis.
Funding/Support: This project was supported by the funding from a National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Independent Investigator Award to Dr Yatham, as well as research grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Canadian Psychiatric Research Foundation.
Additional Contributions: Mauricio Kunz, MD, provided assistance with patient recruitment and biochemical data analysis.
1.Delgado PL, Price LH, Miller HL, Salomon RM, Licinio J, Krystal JH, Heninger GR, Charney DS. Rapid serotonin depletion as a provocative challenge test for patients with major depression: relevance to antidepressant action and the neurobiology of depression.
Psychopharmacol Bull. 1991;27(3):321-3301775606
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.Moreno FA, Parkinson D, Palmer C, Castro WL, Misiaszek J, El Khoury A, Mathé AA, Wright R, Delgado PL. CSF neurochemicals during tryptophan depletion in individuals with remitted depression and healthy controls.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20(1):18-2419896342
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Van der Does AJ. The effects of tryptophan depletion on mood and psychiatric symptoms.
J Affect Disord. 2001;64(2-3):107-11911313078
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Smith KA, Morris JS, Friston KJ, Cowen PJ, Dolan RJ. Brain mechanisms associated with depressive relapse and associated cognitive impairment following acute tryptophan depletion.
Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174:525-52910616631
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Young SN, Ervin FR, Pihl RO, Finn P. Biochemical aspects of tryptophan depletion in primates.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1989;98(4):508-5112505291
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Curzon G. Relationships between plasma, CSF and brain tryptophan.
J Neural Transm Suppl. 1979;(15):81-92290764
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Delgado PL, Miller HL, Salomon RM, Licinio J, Krystal JH, Moreno FA, Heninger GR, Charney DS. Tryptophan-depletion challenge in depressed patients treated with desipramine or fluoxetine: implications for the role of serotonin in the mechanism of antidepressant action.
Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(2):212-22010418696
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Lam RW, Zis AP, Grewal A, Delgado PL, Charney DS, Krystal JH. Effects of rapid tryptophan depletion in patients with seasonal affective disorder in remission after light therapy.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(1):41-448540776
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Nishizawa S, Benkelfat C, Young SN, Leyton M, Mzengeza S, de Montigny C, Blier P, Diksic M. Differences between males and females in rates of serotonin synthesis in human brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(10):5308-53139144233
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Ruhé HG, Mason NS, Schene AH. Mood is indirectly related to serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of monoamine depletion studies.
Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(4):331-35917389902
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Booij L, Van der Does AJ, Riedel WJ. Monoamine depletion in psychiatric and healthy populations: review.
Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8(12):951-97314647394
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Booij L, Van der Does AJ, Haffmans PM, Riedel WJ. Acute tryptophan depletion in depressed patients treated with a selective serotonin–noradrenalin reuptake inhibitor: augmentation of antidepressant response?
J Affect Disord. 2005;86(2-3):305-31115935252
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Delgado PL, Charney DS, Price LH, Aghajanian GK, Landis H, Heninger GR. Serotonin function and the mechanism of antidepressant action: reversal of antidepressant-induced remission by rapid depletion of plasma tryptophan.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47(5):411-4182184795
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Yatham LN, Liddle PF, Dennie J, Shiah IS, Adam MJ, Lane CJ, Lam RW, Ruth TJ. Decrease in brain serotonin 2 receptor binding in patients with major depression following desipramine treatment: a positron emission tomography study with fluorine-18–labeled setoperone.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(8):705-71110435604
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Meyer JH, Kapur S, Eisfeld B, Brown GM, Houle S, DaSilva J, Wilson AA, Rafi-Tari S, Mayberg HS, Kennedy SH. The effect of paroxetine on 5-HT
2A receptors in depression: an [
18F]setoperone PET imaging study.
Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(1):78-8511136637
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Mischoulon D, Dougherty DD, Bottonari KA, Gresham RL, Sonawalla SB, Fischman AJ, Fava M. An open pilot study of nefazodone in depression with anger attacks: relationship between clinical response and receptor binding.
Psychiatry Res. 2002;116(3):151-16112477599
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Yatham LN, Liddle PF, Lam RW, Zis AP, Stoessl AJ, Sossi V, Adam MJ, Ruth TJ. Effect of electroconvulsive therapy on brain 5-HT
2 receptors in major depression.
Br J Psychiatry. 2010;196(6):474-47920513859
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Yatham LN, Liddle PF, Shiah IS, Lam RW, Adam MJ, Zis AP, Ruth TJ. Effects of rapid tryptophan depletion on brain 5-HT
2 receptors: a PET study.
Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178:448-45311331561
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Williams JBW, Link MJ, Rosenthal NE, Terman M. Structured Interview Guide for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, Seasonal Affective Disorders Version (SIGH-SAD). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1988
20.Anderson GM, Young JG, Cohen DJ, Schlicht KR, Patel N. Liquid-chromatographic determination of serotonin and tryptophan in whole blood and plasma.
Clin Chem. 1981;27(5):775-7767226512
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Woods RP, Cherry SR, Mazziotta JC. Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images.
J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1992;16(4):620-6331629424
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, Cherry SR. MRI-PET registration with automated algorithm.
J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1993;17(4):536-5468331222
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Logan J, Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ding YS, Alexoff DL. Distribution volume ratios without blood sampling from graphical analysis of PET data.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1996;16(5):834-8408784228
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Petit-Taboué MC, Landeau B, Barré L, Onfroy MC, Noël MH, Baron JC. Parametric PET imaging of 5HT
2A receptor distribution with
18F-setoperone in the normal human neocortex.
J Nucl Med. 1999;40(1):25-329935052
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Innis RB, Cunningham VJ, Delforge J, Fujita M, Gjedde A, Gunn RN, Holden J, Houle S, Huang SC, Ichise M, Iida H, Ito H, Kimura Y, Koeppe RA, Knudsen GM, Knuuti J, Lammertsma AA, Laruelle M, Logan J, Maguire RP, Mintun MA, Morris ED, Parsey R, Price JC, Slifstein M, Sossi V, Suhara T, Votaw JR, Wong DF, Carson RE. Consensus nomenclature for in vivo imaging of reversibly binding radioligands.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2007;27(9):1533-153917519979
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Friston KJ, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Mazziotta JC, Evans AC. Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent.
Hum Brain Mapp. 1993;1(3):210-220
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Bremner JD, Innis RB, Salomon RM, Staib LH, Ng CK, Miller HL, Bronen RA, Krystal JH, Duncan J, Rich D, Price LH, Malison R, Dey H, Soufer R, Charney DS. Positron emission tomography measurement of cerebral metabolic correlates of tryptophan depletion–induced depressive relapse.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(4):364-3749107153
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Neumeister A, Nugent AC, Waldeck T, Geraci M, Schwarz M, Bonne O, Bain EE, Luckenbaugh DA, Herscovitch P, Charney DS, Drevets WC. Neural and behavioral responses to tryptophan depletion in unmedicated patients with remitted major depressive disorder and controls.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(8):765-77315289275
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Moreno FA, McGahuey CA, Freeman MP, Delgado PL. Sex differences in depressive response during monoamine depletions in remitted depressive subjects.
J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67(10):1618-162317107256
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Roth BL. Irving Page Lecture: 5-HT
2A serotonin receptor biology: interacting proteins, kinases and paradoxical regulation.
Neuropharmacology. 2011;61(3):348-35421288474
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Talbot PS, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Scher E, Abi-Dargham A, Laruelle M. Extended characterisation of the serotonin 2A (5-HT
2A) receptor–selective PET radiotracer
11C-MDL100907 in humans: quantitative analysis, test-retest reproducibility, and vulnerability to endogenous 5-HT tone.
Neuroimage. 2012;59(1):271-28521782029
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Petit-Taboué MC, Landeau B, Osmont A, Tillet I, Barré L, Baron JC. Estimation of neocortical serotonin-2 receptor binding potential by single-dose fluorine-18–setoperone kinetic PET data analysis.
J Nucl Med. 1996;37(1):95-1048544011
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Cho R, Kapur S, Du LS, Hrdina P. Relationship between central and peripheral serotonin 5-HT
2A receptors: a positron emission tomography study in healthy individuals.
Neurosci Lett. 1999;261(3):139-14210081968
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Kapur S, Zipursky R, Remington G, Jones C, McKay G, Houle S. PET evidence that loxapine is an equipotent blocker of 5-HT
2 and D
2 receptors: implications for the therapeutics of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(11):1525-15299356559
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Breier A, Su TP, Saunders R, Carson RE, Kolachana BS, de Bartolomeis A, Weinberger DR, Weisenfeld N, Malhotra AK, Eckelman WC, Pickar D. Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94(6):2569-25749122236
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Laruelle M, D’Souza CD, Baldwin RM, Abi-Dargham A, Kanes SJ, Fingado CL, Seibyl JP, Zoghbi SS, Bowers MB, Jatlow P, Charney DS, Innis RB. Imaging D
2 receptor occupancy by endogenous dopamine in humans.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17(3):162-1749272483
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Kellar KJ, Stockmeier CA. Effects of electroconvulsive shock and serotonin axon lesions on beta-adrenergic and serotonin-2 receptors in rat brain.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;462:76-903010791
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Klimek V, Zak-Knapik J, Mackowiak M. Effects of repeated treatment with fluoxetine and citalopram, 5-HT uptake inhibitors, on 5-HT
1A and 5-HT
2 receptors in the rat brain.
J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1994;19(1):63-678148368
PubMedGoogle Scholar