[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table.  Kinds of Harms Identified in the Belmont Report
Kinds of Harms Identified in the Belmont Report
1.
Kessler  RC, Chiu  WT, Demler  O, Merikangas  KR, Walters  EE.  Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication [published correction appears in Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(7):709].  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617-627.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Whittington  CJ, Kendall  T, Fonagy  P, Cottrell  D, Cotgrove  A, Boddington  E.  Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in childhood depression: systematic review of published vs unpublished data.  Lancet. 2004;363(9418):1341-1345.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Jureidini  JN, Doecke  CJ, Mansfield  PR, Haby  MM, Menkes  DB, Tonkin  AL.  Efficacy and safety of antidepressants for children and adolescents.  BMJ. 2004;328(7444):879-883.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Appelbaum  PS.  Drug-free research in schizophrenia: an overview of the controversy.  IRB. 1996;18(1):1-5.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Mello  MM, Studdert  DM, Brennan  TA.  The rise of litigation in human subjects research.  Ann Intern Med. 2003;139(1):40-45.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Stanley B; National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Interventions for suicidal patients with co-occurring alcohol use disorder. 2007. http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/about-niaaa/our-work/advisory-council/agenda-minutes/advisory-council-meeting-minutes/national-15. Accessed October 11, 2012.
7.
Dunn  LB, Misra  S.  Research ethics issues in geriatric psychiatry.  Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2009;32(2):395-411.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Knesper  DJ; American Association of Suicidology and Suicide Prevention Resource Center.  Continuity of Care for Suicide Prevention and Research: Suicide Attempts and Suicide Deaths Subsequent to Discharge From the Emergency Department or Psychiatry Inpatient Unit. Newton, MA: Education Development Center; 2010.
9.
National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. The Belmont Report: ethical principles and guidelines for the protection of human subjects of research. 1979. http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/humansubjects/guidance/belmont.html. Accessed September 18, 2013.
10.
US Department of Health and Human Services. Title 45: Public welfare. Part 46: Protection of human subjects. http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/humansubjects/guidance/45cfr46.html. Accessed September 18, 2013.
11.
Shah  S, Whittle  A, Wilfond  B, Gensler  G, Wendler  D.  How do institutional review boards apply the federal risk and benefit standards for pediatric research?  JAMA. 2004;291(4):476-482.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Mansbach  J, Acholonu  U, Clark  S, Camargo  CA  Jr.  Variation in institutional review board responses to a standard, observational, pediatric research protocol.  Acad Emerg Med. 2007;14(4):377-380.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Gong  MN, Winkel  G, Rhodes  R, Richardson  LD, Silverstein  JH.  Surrogate consent for research involving adults with impaired decision making: survey of institutional review board practices.  Crit Care Med. 2010;38(11):2146-2154.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McWilliams  R, Hoover-Fong  J, Hamosh  A, Beck  S, Beaty  T, Cutting  G.  Problematic variation in local institutional review of a multicenter genetic epidemiology study.  JAMA. 2003;290(3):360-366.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Abbott  L, Grady  C.  A systematic review of the empirical literature evaluating IRBs: what we know and what we still need to learn.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2011;6(1):3-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Roberts  LW, Warner  TD, Brody  JL, Roberts  B, Lauriello  J, Lyketsos  C.  Patient and psychiatrist ratings of hypothetical schizophrenia research protocols: assessment of harm potential and factors influencing participation decisions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(4):573-584.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Candilis  PJ, Geppert  CM, Fletcher  KE, Lidz  CW, Appelbaum  PS.  Willingness of subjects with thought disorder to participate in research.  Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):159-165.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dunn  LB, Kim  DS, Fellows  IE, Palmer  BW.  Worth the risk? relationship of incentives to risk and benefit perceptions and willingness to participate in schizophrenia research.  Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(4):730-737.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Roberts  LW, Hammond  KG, Hoop  J.  An inverse relationship between perceived harm and participation willingness in schizophrenia research protocols.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(11):2002-2004.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hummer  M, Holzmeister  R, Kemmler  G,  et al.  Attitudes of patients with schizophrenia toward placebo-controlled clinical trials.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;64(3):277-281.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Roberts  LW, Warner  TD, Nguyen  KP, Geppert  CM, Rogers  MK, Roberts  BB.  Schizophrenia patients’ and psychiatrists’ perspectives on ethical aspects of symptom re-emergence during psychopharmacological research participation.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003;171(1):58-67.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Luebbert  R, Tait  RC, Chibnall  JT, Deshields  TL.  IRB member judgments of decisional capacity, coercion, and risk in medical and psychiatric studies.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2008;3(1):15-24.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tait  RC.  Vulnerability in clinical research with patients in pain: a risk analysis.  J Law Med Ethics. 2009;37(1):59-72.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Roberts  LW, Roberts  B.  Psychiatric research ethics: an overview of evolving guidelines and current ethical dilemmas in the study of mental illness.  Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(8):1025-1038.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Berg  J, Appelbaum  P, Lidz  C, Parker  L.  Informed Consent: Legal Theory and Clinical Practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2001.
26.
Appelbaum  PS, Grisso  T, Frank  E, O’Donnell  S, Kupfer  DJ.  Competence of depressed patients for consent to research.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1380-1384.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Carpenter  WT  Jr, Gold  JM, Lahti  AC,  et al.  Decisional capacity for informed consent in schizophrenia research.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(6):533-538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Misra  S, Rosenstein  D, Socherman  R, Ganzini  L.  Bipolar mania and capacity to consent: recommendations for investigators and IRBs.  IRB. 2010;32(1):7-15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Moser  DJ, Schultz  SK, Arndt  S,  et al.  Capacity to provide informed consent for participation in schizophrenia and HIV research.  Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1201-1207.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Stiles  PG, Poythress  NG, Hall  A, Falkenbach  D, Williams  R.  Improving understanding of research consent disclosures among persons with mental illness.  Psychiatr Serv. 2001;52(6):780-785.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Bogardus  ST  Jr, Holmboe  E, Jekel  JF.  Perils, pitfalls, and possibilities in talking about medical risk.  JAMA. 1999;281(11):1037-1041.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Dubler  N.  New directions in human subject research: looking beyond the academic medical center: remaining faithful to the promises given: maintaining standards in changing times.  Seton Hall Law Rev. 2002;32(563):567-568.Google Scholar
33.
Volpe  RL.  Patients’ expressed and unexpressed needs for information for informed consent.  J Clin Ethics. 2010;21(1):45-57.PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Konald  C.  Informal conceptions of probability.  Cogn Instr. 1989;6:59-98.Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Yamagishi  K.  When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.24%.  Appl Cogn Psychol. 1997;11:495-506.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Tverskey  A, Kahneman  D.  Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability.  Cogn Psychol. 1973;5:207-232.Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Tversky  A, Kahneman  D.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.  Science. 1981;211(4481):453-458.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Jasper  JD, Goel  R, Einarson  A, Gallo  M, Koren  G.  Effects of framing on teratogenic risk perception in pregnant women.  Lancet. 2001;358(9289):1237-1238.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Fagerlin  A, Zikmund-Fisher  BJ, Ubel  PA.  “If I’m better than average, then I’m ok?”: comparative information influences beliefs about risk and benefits.  Patient Educ Couns. 2007;69(1-3):140-144.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Health Literacy  IOM.  A Prescription to End Confusion. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2004.
41.
Kirsch  I, Jungeblut  A, Jenkins  L, Kolstad  A.  Adult Literacy in America. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, US Dept of Education; 1993.
42.
Hochhauser  M.  Informed consent and patient’s rights documents: a right, a rite, or a rewrite?  Ethics Behav. 1999;9(1):1-20.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Christopher  PP, Foti  ME, Roy-Bujnowski  K, Appelbaum  PS.  Consent form readability and educational levels of potential participants in mental health research.  Psychiatr Serv. 2007;58(2):227-232.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Baker  FM, Johnson  JT, Velli  SA, Wiley  C.  Congruence between education and reading levels of older persons.  Psychiatr Serv. 1996;47(2):194-196.PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Christensen  RC, Grace  GD.  The prevalence of low literacy in an indigent psychiatric population.  Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(2):262-263.PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Currier  GW, Sitzman  R, Trenton  A.  Literacy in the psychiatric emergency service.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189(1):56-58.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Robinson  G, Merav  A.  Informed consent: recall by patients tested postoperatively.  Bull Am Coll Surg. 1977;62(5):7-9, 30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Turner  P, Williams  C.  Informed consent: patients listen and read, but what information do they retain?  N Z Med J. 2002;115(1164):U218.PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Lloyd  A, Hayes  P, Bell  PR, Naylor  AR.  The role of risk and benefit perception in informed consent for surgery.  Med Decis Making. 2001;21(2):141-149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
50.
Meisel  A, Kuczewski  M.  Legal and ethical myths about informed consent.  Arch Intern Med. 1996;156(22):2521-2526.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
King  NM.  Defining and describing benefit appropriately in clinical trials.  J Law Med Ethics. 2000;28(4):332-343.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Dunn  LB, Lindamer  LA, Palmer  BW, Golshan  S, Schneiderman  LJ, Jeste  DV.  Improving understanding of research consent in middle-aged and elderly patients with psychotic disorders.  Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10(2):142-150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Dunn  LB, Jeste  DV.  Enhancing informed consent for research and treatment.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24(6):595-607.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Jeste  DV, Palmer  BW, Golshan  S,  et al.  Multimedia consent for research in people with schizophrenia and normal subjects: a randomized controlled trial.  Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(4):719-729.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Lipkus  IM, Hollands  JG.  The visual communication of risk.  J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr. 1999;25(25):149-163.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Gigerenzer  G, Edwards  A.  Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight.  BMJ. 2003;327(7417):741-744.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Benatar  JR, Mortimer  J, Stretton  M, Stewart  RAH.  A booklet on participants’ rights to improve consent for clinical research: a randomized trial.  PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47023. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047023.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Prentice  KJ, Appelbaum  PS, Conley  RR, Carpenter  WT.  Maintaining informed consent validity during lengthy research protocols.  IRB. 2007;29(6):1-6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Jeste  DV, Palmer  BW, Appelbaum  PS,  et al.  A new brief instrument for assessing decisional capacity for clinical research.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(8):966-974.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Dunn  LB, Nowrangi  MA, Palmer  BW, Jeste  DV, Saks  ER.  Assessing decisional capacity for clinical research or treatment: a review of instruments.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(8):1323-1334.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Anderson  EE, Iltis  AS.  Assessing and improving research participants’ understanding of risk: potential lessons from the literature on physician-patient risk communication.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2008;3(3):27-37.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
DuBois  JM.  Ethics in Mental Health Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007.
63.
Jefford  M, Moore  R.  Improvement of informed consent and the quality of consent documents.  Lancet Oncol. 2008;9(5):485-493.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Iltis  AS.  Lay concepts in informed consent to biomedical research: the capacity to understand and appreciate risk.  Bioethics. 2006;20(4):180-190.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Lidz  CW, Appelbaum  PS, Arnold  R,  et al.  How closely do institutional review boards follow the Common Rule?  Acad Med. 2012;87(7):969-974.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
DuBois  JM, Beskow  L, Campbell  J,  et al.  Restoring balance: a consensus statement on the protection of vulnerable research participants.  Am J Public Health. 2012;102(12):2220-2225.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Drazen  JM, Wood  AJ.  Don’t mess with the DSMB.  N Engl J Med. 2010;363(5):477-478.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Schneider  LS, Ismail  MS, Dagerman  K,  et al.  Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE): Alzheimer’s disease trial.  Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(1):57-72.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Stroup  TS, Appelbaum  PS.  Evaluation of “subject advocate” procedures in the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia study.  Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):147-152.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Pearson  JL, Stanley  B, King  CA, Fisher  CB.  Intervention research with persons at high risk for suicidality: safety and ethical considerations.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 25):17-26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
71.
Oquendo  MA, Stanley  B, Ellis  SP, Mann  JJ.  Protection of human subjects in intervention research for suicidal behavior.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(9):1558-1563.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Bruce  ML, Ten Have  TR, Reynolds  CF  III,  et al.  Reducing suicidal ideation and depressive symptoms in depressed older primary care patients: a randomized controlled trial.  JAMA. 2004;291(9):1081-1091.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Reynolds  CF  III, Degenholtz  H, Parker  LS,  et al.  Treatment as usual (TAU) control practices in the PROSPECT Study: managing the interaction and tension between research design and ethics.  Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16(6):602-608.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Brown  GK, Ten Have  TR, Henriques  GR, Xie  SX, Hollander  JE, Beck  AT.  Cognitive therapy for the prevention of suicide attempts: a randomized controlled trial.  JAMA. 2005;294(5):563-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Linehan  MM, Comtois  KA, Murray  AM,  et al.  Two-year randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behavior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors and borderline personality disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(7):757-766.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Clark  CC.  Group Leadership Skills for Nurses and Health Professionals. New York, NY: Springer; 2009.
77.
Linehan  MM, Comtois  KA, Ward-Ciesielski  EF.  Assessing and managing risk with suicidal individuals.  Cogn Behav Pract. 2012;19:218-232.Google ScholarCrossref
78.
Beskow  LM, Grady  C, Iltis  AS, Sadler  JZ, Wilfond  BS.  Points to consider: the research ethics consultation service and the IRB.  IRB. 2009;31(6):1-9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004659.PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
McCormick  JB, Boyce  AM, Cho  MK.  Biomedical scientists’ perceptions of ethical and social implications: is there a role for research ethics consultation?  PLoS One. 2009;4(3):e4659.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Emanuel  EJ, Wendler  D, Grady  C.  What makes clinical research ethical?  JAMA. 2000;283(20):2701-2711.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Association of American Medical Colleges–Association of American Universities.  Protecting Patients, Preserving Integrity, Advancing Health: Accelerating the Implementation of COI Policies in Human Subjects Research. Washington, DC: Association of American Medical Colleges–Association of American Universities; 2008.
82.
Angell  M.  Industry-sponsored clinical research: a broken system.  JAMA. 2008;300(9):1069-1071.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Whitaker  R.  Mad in America: Bad Science, Bad Medicine, and the Enduring Mistreatment of the Mentally Ill. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Perseus Publishing; 2001.
84.
 Health care reform for Americans with severe mental illnesses: report of the National Advisory Mental Health Council.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(10):1447-1465.PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
 Research guidelines issued for persons with mental disorders and impaired capacity to make decisions.  Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(1):128-129.PubMedGoogle Scholar
86.
Grof  P, Müller-Oerlinghausen  B.  A critical appraisal of lithium’s efficacy and effectiveness: the last 60 years.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(suppl 2):10-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Nivoli  AM, Murru  A, Vieta  E.  Lithium: still a cornerstone in the long-term treatment in bipolar disorder?  Neuropsychobiology. 2010;62(1):27-35.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Saunders  KE, Hawton  K.  The role of psychopharmacology in suicide prevention.  Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2009;18(3):172-178.PubMedGoogle Scholar
89.
Tondo  L, Baldessarini  RJ.  Long-term lithium treatment in the prevention of suicidal behavior in bipolar disorder patients.  Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2009;18(3):179-183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
90.
Heaton  PC, Garlick  CM, Tran  D.  Evaluating drug safety in children and adolescents with bipolar disorder.  Curr Drug Saf. 2006;1(3):307-318.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Aziz  R, Lorberg  B, Tampi  RR.  Treatments for late-life bipolar disorder.  Am J Geriatr Pharmacother. 2006;4(4):347-364.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Albanese  MJ, Pies  R.  The bipolar patient with comorbid substance use disorder: recognition and management.  CNS Drugs. 2004;18(9):585-596.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Conwell  Y, Duberstein  PR, Caine  ED.  Risk factors for suicide in later life.  Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(3):193-204.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Salloum  IM, Thase  ME.  Impact of substance abuse on the course and treatment of bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2000;2(3, pt 2):269-280.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Gelenberg  AJ, Pies  R.  Matching the bipolar patient and the mood stabilizer.  Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2003;15(3-4):203-216.PubMedGoogle Scholar
96.
Goodwin  FK, Goldstein  MA.  Optimizing lithium treatment in bipolar disorder: a review of the literature and clinical recommendations.  J Psychiatr Pract. 2003;9(5):333-343.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Scott  J, Tacchi  MJ.  A pilot study of concordance therapy for individuals with bipolar disorder who are non-adherent with lithium prophylaxis.  Bipolar Disord. 2002;4(6):386-392.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Mitchell  PB, Hadzi-Pavlovic  D.  Lithium treatment for bipolar disorder.  Bull World Health Organ. 2000;78(4):515-517.PubMedGoogle Scholar
99.
Dubois  JM, Bante  H, Hadley  WB.  Ethics in psychiatric research: a review of 25 years of NIH-funded empirical research projects.  AJOB Prim Res. 2011;2(4):5-17.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
100.
DelVecchio  P, Blyler  CR. Identifying critical outcomes and setting priorities for mental health services research. In: Wallcraft  J, Scharnk  B, Amerin  M, eds.  Handbook of Service User Involvement in Mental Health Research. New York, NY: Wiley; 2009:92-211.
Original Investigation
December 2013

Addressing Risks to Advance Mental Health Research

Author Affiliations
  • 1Center for Bioethics, Health, and Society and Department of Philosophy, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, North Carolina
  • 2Mental Health and Clinical Neurosciences Division, Portland VA Medical Center, Portland, Oregon
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland
  • 4Departments of Psychiatry and Psycho-Oncology, UCSF Helen Diller Family Comprehensive Cancer Center, University of California, San Francisco
  • 5Clinical Psychology in Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
  • 6Center for Bioethics and Humanities, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York
  • 7St Louis Empowerment, St Louis, Missouri
  • 8Department of Psychiatry (Child), Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 9Falk College for Sports and Human Dynamics, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York
  • 10Department of Psychiatry, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York
  • 11Department of Medicine, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 12Center for Health Care Ethics, Saint Louis University, St Louis, Missouri
JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(12):1363-1371. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2105
Abstract

Importance  Risk communication and management are essential to the ethical conduct of research, yet addressing risks may be time consuming for investigators and institutional review boards may reject study designs that seem too risky. This can discourage needed research, particularly in higher-risk protocols or those enrolling potentially vulnerable individuals, such as those with some level of suicidality. Improved mechanisms for addressing research risks may facilitate much needed psychiatric research.

Objective  To provide mental health researchers with practical approaches to (1) identify and define various intrinsic research risks, (2) communicate these risks to others (eg, potential participants, regulatory bodies, and society), (3) manage these risks during the course of a study, and (4) justify the risks.

Evidence Review  As part of a National Institute of Mental Health–funded scientific meeting series, a public conference and a closed-session expert panel meeting were held on managing and disclosing risks in mental health clinical trials. The expert panel reviewed the literature with a focus on empirical studies and developed recommendations for best practices and further research on managing and disclosing risks in mental health clinical trials. No institutional review board–review was required because there were no human subjects.

Findings  Challenges, current data, practical strategies, and topics for future research are addressed for each of 4 key areas pertaining to management and disclosure of risks in clinical trials: identifying and defining risks, communicating risks, managing risks during studies, and justifying research risks.

Conclusions and Relevance  Empirical data on risk communication, managing risks, and the benefits of research can support the ethical conduct of mental health research and may help investigators better conceptualize and confront risks and to gain institutional review board–approval.

With 26.2% of US adults afflicted with a psychiatric disorder, improvements in psychiatric treatments are needed.1 Despite advances for psychiatric disorders, there has been increased public scrutiny of how mental health research is conducted and the risks to participants, particularly in the wake of concerns regarding unpublished data on selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and suicide,2,3 studies on schizophrenia in which medication is withheld,4 and increased litigation.5 Researchers must proactively address risks—from minimizing risks inherent to study design (eg, placebo-controlled studies), to mitigating potential participant misunderstanding of the research purpose, and to addressing risks of worsening symptoms during research participation (eg, emergence of suicidal ideation or psychosis). Yet, the means of addressing risks are not always readily apparent, particularly when those risks may be inherent in the overall research endeavor or associated with the illness itself, not the research. Institutional review boards (IRBs) and sponsors may halt or delay studies over concerns about participant risks. Increased investigator burdens because of real and perceived risks may discourage much-needed research.

Quiz Ref IDFor example, suicide is clearly associated with a history of depression and is the most serious potential outcome of depression, but only 10% of clinical trials examining SSRI efficacy between 1984 and 2001 included individuals with some level of suicidality; there are no SSRI efficacy trials in actively suicidal individuals.6 Because the presence of suicidal ideation is a typical exclusion criterion in research, the community is left with “evidence-based” guidance that is generalized to these excluded individuals. This pattern of exclusion has resulted in a dearth of evidence regarding treatment for individuals with varying degrees of suicidality. To facilitate research in this area, suicide should be treated as a negative outcome comparable to negative outcomes of other diseases—an undesirable yet to-be-expected event.7,8 To ignore this is to continue the unwarranted and unjust exclusion of these individuals.

Mental health researchers must strive to design ethical and scientifically sound research that does not ignore populations or kinds of research merely because of the difficulties involved. If mental health researchers continue to allow risks to discourage research, certain groups that could benefit from research will continue to be harmed by being understudied. Groups perceived as “high risk” deserve scientifically rigorous study as well. Researchers must be able to identify and communicate risks and potential benefits of this research to the public and regulatory bodies, demonstrate that they will manage risks effectively, and provide strong ethical justification for such research.

This article provides mental health researchers with practical approaches to (1) identify and define various intrinsic research risks, (2) communicate risks to others (eg, potential participants, regulatory bodies, and society), (3) manage risks during the course of a study, and (4) justify the risks. These recommendations are the result of a systematic literature review, a public conference during which authors participated as speakers or panelists, a closed-door working session that included all of the authors, and subsequent e-mail and telephone conversations to formulate consensus recommendations. All authors participated in the writing and editing process.

Defining and Identifying Risks
Challenges in Defining Risk in Research

The Belmont Report9 provides the ethical framework for US research regulations and identifies 5 primary kinds of harm relevant to research review and oversight. These are summarized in the Table.

In addition to recognizing the kinds of harms that study participation may pose, investigators and regulatory bodies must determine the studies’ overall risk level. Although risk is an inherent aspect of human life and familiar to all, it is difficult to define operationally. Quiz Ref IDMost regulatory and philosophical definitions of risk include 3 primary concepts: harm and its probability and magnitude. However, there is no agreed-on formula for determining risk level. The Common Rule recognizes 2 risk categories for adult research: minimal and greater than minimal. Minimal risk is defined as “the probability and magnitude of harm or discomfort anticipated in the research are not greater in and of themselves than those ordinarily encountered in daily life or during the performance of routine physical or psychological examinations or tests.”10 Whose daily life and which examinations are routine are not specified and instead left to IRB judgment. The Department of Health and Human Services identifies 3 categories of risk for research involving children: minimal risk, greater than minimal risk with prospect of direct benefit, and minor increase over minimal risk with no direct benefit. Studies that do not fit these categories require special federal review.10

To foresee and address research risks, investigators must identify the types, probability, and magnitude of harm that their study poses and describe risk information in protocols and informed consent discussions and forms clearly. Regulatory bodies, such as IRBs, ultimately define the study risks relevant to review and determine whether the “[r]isks to subjects are reasonable in relation to anticipated benefits, if any, to subjects, and the importance of the knowledge that may reasonably be expected to result.”10 However, guidance about defining and evaluating risks and benefits is limited and often focused on determining the extent to which additional protections are needed for studies deemed to be higher risk. Researchers may educate IRB members about study risks, including a comparison of research risks across different types of studies, when they have data that might inform IRB determinations.

Research Summary: State of the Field

There is no consensus on how best to evaluate and rate research risk levels, and IRBs are inconsistent in how they evaluate risk. Shah et al11 found significant variability among IRB chairs’ rating of risk level. Similar studies of IRB practices have demonstrated inconsistencies in rating protocols as eligible for expedited review vs requiring full review, a determination based largely on assessment of risk level since expedited studies must be minimal risk.12-14 This variability results in uneven participant protections and delays in start dates for funded research.14,15 These issues are magnified in large-scale multisite studies, which can provide greater statistical power with more meaningful and generalizable outcomes yet require review by multiple IRBs.14

Quiz Ref IDDespite data indicating that mental health research is as safe as most other medical research and that participants typically possess the capacity to give consent for themselves,16-21 mental health researchers may find that IRBs, which are not required to and may not have members with substantive expertise in mental health research,10 judge their studies as posing higher risks than comparably risky non–mental health studies and believe that potential participants are less likely to be able to understand risks and given informed consent.22,23

Tips for the Savvy Researcher

Strategies for appropriately identifying research risks are summarized in Box 1 and include the following:

Box Section Ref ID
Box 1.

Strategies for the Savvy Researcher

Defining and Identifying Risks
  • Identify possible concerns about risk or decisional capacity that might affect a study before submitting a proposal

  • Use tools, such as RePEAT, to identify risks and benefits systematically

  • Address anticipated concerns about risk and decisional capacity in research proposals

  • Describe potential research benefits and compare them explicitly with the risks

  • Describe for sponsors and institutional review boards the process for communicating and managing risks

Communicating Risks
  • Use plain language

  • Use interactive informed consent processes

  • Use interactive processes to assess decisional capacity

  • Provide risk information in different formats

  • Be as specific as possible and avoid vague risk-benefit language

  • Periodically review relevant risk information with participants

Managing Risks
  • Use research ethics consultation services or other opportunities to discuss research risks with individuals not affiliated with the study but knowledgeable about research ethics

  • Plan for ongoing staff training

  • Ensure availability of senior investigators who may help research staff address emerging issues in a study

  • Devise a risk management plan that includes detailed information on how different anticipated risks will be managed, how safety will be monitored, what will happen when a participant drops out of a study, the roles and responsibilities of different members of the study team with respect to risk management, and how the effectiveness of the risk management plan will be evaluated during a study

Abbreviation: RePEAT, Research Protocol Ethics Assessment Tool.

  1. Risks should be identified and defined early in study development. The Research Protocol Ethics Assessment Tool (RePEAT), developed by Roberts and Roberts,24 is essentially a checklist that covers multiple ethical domains, including scientific merit of the protocol; risks and benefits; the expertise, commitment, and integrity of the research team; informed consent and decisional capacity issues; incentives for participation; and confidentiality. Researchers can apply this tool (or variants of it) to their protocols to ensure that ethically important research elements are proactively addressed.

  2. Study proposals should explicitly identify and address foreseen potential risks, how these risks will be communicated to potential participants, and how emerging risks will be managed, depending on risk level, once the study begins, including, but not limited to, the use of data and safety monitoring boards (DSMBs) or external monitors. Strategies for communicating and managing risks are explored below.

  3. To conduct research with groups that IRBs often exclude owing to their “vulnerability,” mental health investigators may need to work harder than other investigators to document for IRBs evidence of research risks and the abilities of potential participants (who are otherwise likely to be perceived as vulnerable) to understand risk information and provide informed consent. For example, researchers may collaborate with community advisory boards, review the literature, and conduct focus groups to support inclusion.

Further Research Needed to Advance the Field

A better understanding of participant and IRB views and decision making regarding risk would assist researchers in designing studies. Future research should aim to identify and clarify the risks that matter most to potential participants, including understanding their concerns about privacy violations, legal risks, social harms at the individual level, and the burdens of participating in studies. Finally, more research on how IRBs evaluate risk, perceive institutional risk, compare risks and potential benefits, and make decisions could help investigators explain studies better when applying for IRB approval. Such research might also identify areas for IRB education.

For example, one study compared IRB member judgments of the consent capacity of potential participants in oncology, chronic pain, and major depressive disorder research studies. Persons with major depressive disorder were judged as having significantly less consent capacity than those with non–mental health diagnoses even when IRB members were told of the high incidence of depression as a comorbid condition in patients with cancer and chronic pain. The IRB members also judged the psychiatric study as posing greater legal risk to the institution than the other studies.22,23 If these apparently inconsistent analyses of capacity and risk by IRBs are typical, it would be appropriate to develop educational interventions to help IRBs develop new strategies for evaluating studies. Knowing whether there is a significant difference between perceived and actual institutional risk might help facilitate research. Future research needs are summarized in Box 2.

Box Section Ref ID
Box 2.

Research Needsa

Defining and Identifying Risk
  • How do potential participants and families or surrogates, investigators, and IRB members define risk?

  • What risks are most worrisome to potential participants?

  • What benefits matter most to potential participants, surrogates, and society?

  • How do IRBs make decisions regarding risks, and how do they compare risks and benefits?

Communicating Risks
  • What techniques are most effective to help participants understand research risks? For example, what practices

    • help individuals sort and manage significant amounts of information?

    • help individuals understand numbers and probabilities?

    • reduce cognitive biases?

    • improve understanding of health information?

    • facilitate communication of information typically provided in writing?

    • enhance retention and recall of information?

    • promote the exchange of information that matters most to potential participants?

  • What variables affect understanding and appreciation of risk information?

Managing Risks
  • What “tool kits” may investigators use to help develop and implement effective and comprehensive risk management strategies?

  • What are effective ways to train research staff to manage risk, particularly in higher-risk studies?

  • Are there risk management strategies that should be used when clinicians also serve as investigators?

  • How do participants, researchers, and IRBs respond to different risk management plans?

Abbreviation: IRB, institutional review board.

a

Note that in answering research questions about the views and assessments of participants or populations of potential participants, it is important to attend to individual differences.

Communicating Risks With Research Participants
Importance of Communicating Risks

The regulatory10 and ethical9 obligation to obtain informed consent from research participants (or their legally authorized representatives) requires effective risk communication.25 Risk communication is an information-sharing process in which investigators disclose research risks and benefits in language potential participants can understand and elicit their need for additional information and in which participants understand and appreciate the information relevant to their research participation decision.

Research Summary: State of the Field

Numerous factors can impede effective risk communication for research participants. Individuals with certain mental health conditions may face additional barriers, some of which can be overcome with appropriate interventions.26-30 Many barriers identified here are exacerbated by heavy reliance on informed consent forms as the primary means for disclosing risks and by expectations that potential participants will spontaneously ask for further information or clarification. The factors to consider are as follows.

Volume and Quality of Information

Risk information has 5 dimensions: identity, permanence, timing, probability, and value or seriousness.31 Effective risk communication requires disclosure, understanding, and appreciation of all 5 dimensions. Providing too much information at once, however, particularly without categorizing it or distinguishing between less and more significant and probable risks, impedes effective risk communication. Informed consent documents that incorporate required information from different sources can become long and difficult to read.32 Risk communication involves objective, factual, and subjective information whose relevance depends on individual values, goals, and priorities. There may be a tendency to approach this process with a more legalistic or formalistic vision, whereby disclosure is seen as a standardized process without attention to the specific information particular participants might need and value.33

Poor Numeracy

Adults with average cognitive capacities do not always understand and use basic mathematical and statistical concepts well.34,35 Understanding and appreciating risks requires a conceptual understanding of probability and basic mathematical skills. Poor numeracy interferes with this ability. If the person disclosing risk information does not understand these concepts well, disclosure may be inadequate.

Cognitive Biases

Tversky and Kahneman36,37 and others have identified ways in which cognitive biases affect the understanding and interpretation of numerical information. Cognitive biases include framing (information about the chance of a risk materializing and not materializing is understood differently), compression (small risks seem greater than they are and large risks seem smaller), availability (culturally significant or otherwise well-known risks are overestimated), anchoring (risks are estimated relative to a familiar risk), and comparison bias (risk perception changes when given comparative risk information).38,39

Poor Literacy and Poor Health Literacy

Quiz Ref IDPoor health literacy is well documented in the United States40 and can interfere with the ability to understand and appreciate research risk information. Risk information often is communicated in writing. Given the estimated numbers of functionally illiterate (40 million) and marginally literate (50 million) adults in the United States,41 written communication may be unreliable, particularly when written at a high reading level, as often is the case with research consent documents.42,43 Poor literacy may pose special concerns in psychiatric research because adults with certain disorders may read several grades below their educational level.43-46

Poor Retention and Recall of Information and Working Memory Limitations

The decision to participate in a study is a series of decisions to enroll and remain in a study. Individuals must be able to recall information about the study, including risk information, to make these decisions. Individuals often have difficulty retaining and recalling risk information.47-49 Working memory limitations also can impede understanding and appreciation of risk information, which requires processing and manipulating significant amounts of information simultaneously. This poses special concern for potential participants with mental health conditions that adversely affect working memory, such as schizophrenia, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer disease, and other forms of dementia.

The Subjectivity of Risk Information

Risk evaluation is value laden, and different individuals may evaluate the same risks differently. Knowing which risks individuals would want disclosed can improve risk communication. This envisions a more subjective standard of disclosure over a purely formalistic approach (akin to an “information dump”), as discussed in clinical decision making.50

Poor Communication Regarding Research Benefits

Potential participants not only must understand and appreciate research risks but must also evaluate them in light of the potential benefits of participation. Exaggerating risks, using vague information regarding benefits, or allowing potential participants to think that research participation poses greater personal benefit than it does can impede effective risk communication.51

Tips for the Savvy Researcher

Existing research suggests that specific practices can foster effective risk communication. These recommended practices are summarized in Box 1 and include the following:

  1. Use plain language (see www.plainlanguage.gov).

  2. Use interactive informed consent processes that can foster decisional capacity, including understanding and appreciation of risks, among persons with different learning styles and capacities.27,30,50,52-54

  3. Use graphs, charts, or other means of presenting information, particularly numerical information, to promote understanding among persons with different learning styles and capacities.55-57

  4. Assess the capacity for consent, not only to determine whether a person is ready to give valid informed consent but also because specific processes designed to assess capacity involve educational interventions that can foster understanding and appreciation.58-60

  5. Avoid unnecessary vagueness, and provide necessary information regarding research risks and benefits, distinguishing between probable and remote risks or benefits.51,57,61-63

  6. Review research information periodically to address retention and recall.61

Further Research Needed to Advance the Field

There are few studies on risk communication involving actual participants or persons contemplating enrollment in a real study. Studies about participation in hypothetical trials may not be fully generalizable to real-world decision-making contexts.

As summarized in Box 2, for each barrier identified, additional research is needed to better understand the nature of the barrier and identify cost-effective mechanisms that can overcome it. There also remain important normative questions, such as what level of understanding and appreciation among potential participants is necessary for valid informed consent.64

Managing Risks
Definition and Importance of Managing Risks

High-risk studies are essential to improving knowledge of psychiatric disorders and effective treatment. Effective risk management during these studies is critical. However, IRBs may pay little attention to the investigator’s obligation to minimize risks65 and may either disapprove studies perceived as higher risk or approve studies with insufficient risk management plans. This makes it particularly important for mental health researchers to provide evidence-based risk information and propose appropriate risk management plans when seeking IRB approval. Studies may be considered high risk because the intervention is novel, the study population is deemed high risk, the study targets risky behavior (eg, suicidality or psychosis), or the study targets disorders in which risky behaviors are features of the illnesses (eg, depression or bipolar disorder). Automatic exclusion of participants considered high risk or removal of participants exhibiting risky behavior limits generalizability.66 Risk management strategies instead focus on trying to initiate and maintain study participation safely.

Research Summary: State of the Field

Data and safety monitoring boards may monitor studies, although they can be costly and there has been controversy about their independence from study sponsors. A July 2010 New England Journal of Medicine editorial proposed that DSMBs should be chosen and convened “under the aegis of an independent public body, such as… the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health,” and stated that the journal will be examining the independence of DSMBs for future manuscript submissions when appropriate.67(p478)

For longitudinal studies in populations at risk of fluctuating levels of decisional abilities, there is concern about diminishing capacity and the possibility that participants will be unable to protect their interests if new risks arise during a study. Use of third-party participant advocates who ensure that a research participant’s interests remain protected during a long-term study should the participant exhibit diminished capacity may help researchers allow individuals to remain in a study despite fluctuating or diminishing decisional capacity without adding significant costs.

Quiz Ref IDFor example, the large-scale, 18-month-plus, Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia study examining long-term outcomes used such advocates.68 At enrollment, participants designated an advocate. Although advocates participated in the initial informed consent discussions, all participants had the capacity to give consent. Advocates received training to determine whether the risk-benefit ratio acknowledged by the participant at time of enrollment had changed “substantially and adversely,”68 thus no longer reflecting the participant’s expressed interests. If so, participants were withdrawn. However, if the risk-benefit ratio was not substantially altered, then the advocate could permit the participant to continue participation. Stroup and Appelbaum69 surveyed research personnel and a subset of participants and advocates when participants left or completed the study. A majority in all groups favorably viewed the advocate process; 12% of sampled participants believed that the process negatively affected their autonomy.69

Managing risk during a study may also involve altering standard study designs to address safety concerns and accounting for these changes in data analysis.70,71 Consider the National Institute of Mental Health–funded randomized controlled Prevention of Suicide in Primary Care Elderly–Collaborative Trial (PROSPECT).72 This study randomly assigned depression health specialists in primary care clinics (the study intervention) and measured depression and suicidal ideation rates in elderly primary care patients. These outcomes were compared with those in clinics that were assigned to an enhanced treatment-as-usual (TAU) protocol. Individuals with suicidal ideation were not excluded from participation, and rates of suicidal ideation were examined over time. Unlike a true TAU clinical setting, all participants assigned to TAU received enhanced care in the form of increased psychiatric surveillance (screening and assessment). This enhanced TAU design created potential study limitations, but it was necessary for study design purposes (to initially identify cases and evaluate at follow-up study visits) and to meet stringent standards for assuring participants’ safety.73 Similar designs with high-risk participants have been used in other clinical trials74,75 and support the notion that such approaches facilitate important research that otherwise would be constrained.

Training research staff how to assess and manage risks and anxiety-provoking situations (eg, using role play) has been shown to be effective.76,77

Tips for the Savvy Researcher

Investigators may implement a number of strategies to manage research risks. There are 3 principal strategies:

  1. Conduct research ethics consultations, which involve “interaction between researchers and other stakeholders in the research enterprise” and a person (or persons) knowledgeable about the ethical considerations in research, regarding an ethical question related to any aspect of planning, conducting, interpreting, or disseminating results of research related to human health and well-being. The purpose of the interaction is to provide information; identify, analyze, and/or deliberate about ethical issues; and recommend a course of action.”78(p3)McCormick et al79 reported that psychiatric researchers were more likely than other researchers to find such services useful.

  2. Plan for ongoing research staff training on the use of management plans and ways of responding to different situations. The principal investigator or a qualified coinvestigator should be readily available to research staff during the study.

  3. Identify situations that necessitate a written risk management plan. Such plans may include tools such as checklists for easy and systematic implementation of specific plan elements. Studies examining specific risky behavior require explicit procedures that include frequent assessment and crisis intervention when needed. For example, if investigators anticipate psychiatric emergencies, the plan should address how these will be handled, who will be involved, and how and to what extent confidentiality will be protected. Management plans may include a protocol for handling dropouts from the study and for obtaining follow-up information or maintaining an up-to-date list of multiple ways to contact participants. Risk management plans should clearly delineate the roles of research team members and the relationship between researchers and clinicians in managing safety. These plans should be detailed in the protocol, and elements relevant to participant rights or willingness to participate should be disclosed during the informed consent process.

Further Research Needed to Advance the Field

Further research is needed to create evidence-based risk management plans and train investigators on risk management. This is especially important given the need for more research in high-risk areas, such as suicide. Research to develop empirically based guidance about risk management in mental health research would benefit researchers and regulatory bodies alike. This research agenda should include further examination of the specific types of risky situations necessitating intervention and the specific interventions that are effective; the roles and responsibilities of researchers, clinicians, and clinician-researchers with respect to risk management; and the perspectives of participants, families, and regulatory bodies about the use of risk management strategies in research that carries greater than minimal risk.

Justifying Risks

The Belmont Report and current regulations consistently require that risks be evaluated in light of potential benefits to participants and society; research that is scientifically invalid or otherwise lacks value is unethical insofar as it wastes resources and unnecessarily exposes participants to risk.64,80 Protecting the scientific validity of a study requires management of conflicts of interest that may bias the conduct or reporting of a study.81 Trust in psychiatric research has been harmed by reports of investigators who conducted research of questionable validity with inadequately disclosed and unmanaged conflicts of interest.82

One frequently heard criticism of mental health research is that, despite years of investigation, the field has made few significant advances.83 However, at least 2 federally sponsored studies have shown that available treatments for psychiatric disorders are comparable in efficacy to treatments in other medical specialties.84,85 The lack of research on some types of patients may explain the lack of progress in some areas. Moreover, the development of beneficial treatments is often extremely complex, involving false starts, competing interests, and stigma.

These factors are illustrated well in the research history of lithium, which has been proved effective as an acute and maintenance treatment86,87 in bipolar disorder and is approved by the Food and Drug Administration for these indications. Several studies show that lithium significantly reduces rates of suicide,88,89 yet despite 60 years of clinical use, it has not been systematically and thoroughly studied in either pediatric90 or geriatric91 populations. Furthermore, there have been few controlled studies of lithium in “dual-diagnosis” populations (eg, patients with both bipolar disorder and substance abuse or dependence).92 Ironically, suicide rates are highest in elderly populations, with affective illness a potent risk factor,93 suggesting that the potential benefit of research in this population is reasonable relative to the risks. Similarly, dual-diagnosis status (bipolar and substance use disorder) is associated with poor prognosis and treatment resistance.92,94 Thus, 2 populations at significant risk for morbidity and mortality are understudied with respect to lithium treatment.

Reluctance to undertake research in these groups may stem from legitimate concerns regarding lithium’s adverse effects and potential toxicity95; however, other factors may be at work. For example, unlike many anticonvulsants and “atypical” antipsychotics, lithium receives little marketing support from pharmaceutical companies.96 The role of stigma surrounding lithium must also be considered. Scott and Tacchi describe engaging in cognitive therapy with clients with bipolar disorder to address inaccurate perceptions that “if they took lithium prior to important meetings then they would be like a zombie, namely they believed their cognitive functioning would be impaired.”97(p388) Clearly, if IRB members, researchers, or potential study participants buy into the “zombie” myth, research on lithium is likely to be impeded—notwithstanding a World Health Organization study estimating that lithium treatment saved more than $145 billion in hospitalization costs in the United States between 1970 and 1994.98

It is also important to determine which benefits matter most to participants and when and why they matter most. Some data suggest that the interests of researchers and funding agencies do not always align with those of participants, the primary stakeholders in mental health research. For example, mental health consumers tend to prioritize more than researchers regarding alternative treatment modalities (eg, nutritional or self-help modalities), iatrogenic harms, and qualitative research.99,100

Conclusions

The ethical conduct of research requires investigators to identify, communicate, and manage risks effectively. Investigators’ efforts to identify and communicate research risks and potential benefits clearly, demonstrate knowledge of participants’ perceptions of risks and the kinds of potential harms that most concern them, document potential participants’ capacity to give informed consent, and develop plans to monitor risks during the study and identify new risks may help researchers improve their studies and collaborations with IRBs. For individual investigators, providing IRBs with tables that explain risk information and clear management plans may be useful. Continued research is required to advance researchers’ and IRB understanding of how best to identify, define, effectively communicate, and manage risks. Such efforts are essential to facilitating much needed mental health research.

Back to top
Article Information

Submitted for Publication: October 10, 2012; final revision received March 12, 2013; accepted March 13, 2013.

Corresponding Author: James M. DuBois, PhD, DSc, Albert Gnaegi Center for Health Care Ethics, St Louis University, Salus Center, 3545 Lafayette, 5th Floor, St Louis, MO 63104 (jdubois@wustl.edu).

Published Online: October 30, 2013. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2105.

Author Contributions: Drs Iltis and DuBois had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: All authors.

Acquisition of data: Iltis, Misra, Glowinski, DuBois.

Analysis and interpretation of data: Iltis, Misra, Glowinski, DuBois.

Drafting of the manuscript: Iltis, Misra, Dunn, Glowinski, Hadley, DuBois.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Iltis, Misra, Dunn, Brown, Campbell, Earll, Glowinski, Pies, DuBois.

Obtained funding: DuBois.

Administrative, technical, and material support: Iltis, Misra, Brown, Hadley, DuBois.

Study supervision: Iltis, Misra, DuBois.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Dunn reports serving as a consultant in 2012 to Eli Lilly Inc. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (grant 1R13MH079690) and the National Center for Advancing Translational Science (grant UL1 TR000448), National Institutes of Health.

Role of the Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Kessler  RC, Chiu  WT, Demler  O, Merikangas  KR, Walters  EE.  Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication [published correction appears in Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(7):709].  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617-627.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Whittington  CJ, Kendall  T, Fonagy  P, Cottrell  D, Cotgrove  A, Boddington  E.  Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in childhood depression: systematic review of published vs unpublished data.  Lancet. 2004;363(9418):1341-1345.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Jureidini  JN, Doecke  CJ, Mansfield  PR, Haby  MM, Menkes  DB, Tonkin  AL.  Efficacy and safety of antidepressants for children and adolescents.  BMJ. 2004;328(7444):879-883.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Appelbaum  PS.  Drug-free research in schizophrenia: an overview of the controversy.  IRB. 1996;18(1):1-5.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Mello  MM, Studdert  DM, Brennan  TA.  The rise of litigation in human subjects research.  Ann Intern Med. 2003;139(1):40-45.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Stanley B; National Advisory Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Interventions for suicidal patients with co-occurring alcohol use disorder. 2007. http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/about-niaaa/our-work/advisory-council/agenda-minutes/advisory-council-meeting-minutes/national-15. Accessed October 11, 2012.
7.
Dunn  LB, Misra  S.  Research ethics issues in geriatric psychiatry.  Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2009;32(2):395-411.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Knesper  DJ; American Association of Suicidology and Suicide Prevention Resource Center.  Continuity of Care for Suicide Prevention and Research: Suicide Attempts and Suicide Deaths Subsequent to Discharge From the Emergency Department or Psychiatry Inpatient Unit. Newton, MA: Education Development Center; 2010.
9.
National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. The Belmont Report: ethical principles and guidelines for the protection of human subjects of research. 1979. http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/humansubjects/guidance/belmont.html. Accessed September 18, 2013.
10.
US Department of Health and Human Services. Title 45: Public welfare. Part 46: Protection of human subjects. http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/humansubjects/guidance/45cfr46.html. Accessed September 18, 2013.
11.
Shah  S, Whittle  A, Wilfond  B, Gensler  G, Wendler  D.  How do institutional review boards apply the federal risk and benefit standards for pediatric research?  JAMA. 2004;291(4):476-482.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Mansbach  J, Acholonu  U, Clark  S, Camargo  CA  Jr.  Variation in institutional review board responses to a standard, observational, pediatric research protocol.  Acad Emerg Med. 2007;14(4):377-380.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Gong  MN, Winkel  G, Rhodes  R, Richardson  LD, Silverstein  JH.  Surrogate consent for research involving adults with impaired decision making: survey of institutional review board practices.  Crit Care Med. 2010;38(11):2146-2154.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
McWilliams  R, Hoover-Fong  J, Hamosh  A, Beck  S, Beaty  T, Cutting  G.  Problematic variation in local institutional review of a multicenter genetic epidemiology study.  JAMA. 2003;290(3):360-366.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Abbott  L, Grady  C.  A systematic review of the empirical literature evaluating IRBs: what we know and what we still need to learn.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2011;6(1):3-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Roberts  LW, Warner  TD, Brody  JL, Roberts  B, Lauriello  J, Lyketsos  C.  Patient and psychiatrist ratings of hypothetical schizophrenia research protocols: assessment of harm potential and factors influencing participation decisions.  Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(4):573-584.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Candilis  PJ, Geppert  CM, Fletcher  KE, Lidz  CW, Appelbaum  PS.  Willingness of subjects with thought disorder to participate in research.  Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):159-165.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dunn  LB, Kim  DS, Fellows  IE, Palmer  BW.  Worth the risk? relationship of incentives to risk and benefit perceptions and willingness to participate in schizophrenia research.  Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(4):730-737.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Roberts  LW, Hammond  KG, Hoop  J.  An inverse relationship between perceived harm and participation willingness in schizophrenia research protocols.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(11):2002-2004.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hummer  M, Holzmeister  R, Kemmler  G,  et al.  Attitudes of patients with schizophrenia toward placebo-controlled clinical trials.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;64(3):277-281.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Roberts  LW, Warner  TD, Nguyen  KP, Geppert  CM, Rogers  MK, Roberts  BB.  Schizophrenia patients’ and psychiatrists’ perspectives on ethical aspects of symptom re-emergence during psychopharmacological research participation.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003;171(1):58-67.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Luebbert  R, Tait  RC, Chibnall  JT, Deshields  TL.  IRB member judgments of decisional capacity, coercion, and risk in medical and psychiatric studies.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2008;3(1):15-24.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Tait  RC.  Vulnerability in clinical research with patients in pain: a risk analysis.  J Law Med Ethics. 2009;37(1):59-72.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Roberts  LW, Roberts  B.  Psychiatric research ethics: an overview of evolving guidelines and current ethical dilemmas in the study of mental illness.  Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46(8):1025-1038.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Berg  J, Appelbaum  P, Lidz  C, Parker  L.  Informed Consent: Legal Theory and Clinical Practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2001.
26.
Appelbaum  PS, Grisso  T, Frank  E, O’Donnell  S, Kupfer  DJ.  Competence of depressed patients for consent to research.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1380-1384.PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Carpenter  WT  Jr, Gold  JM, Lahti  AC,  et al.  Decisional capacity for informed consent in schizophrenia research.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(6):533-538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Misra  S, Rosenstein  D, Socherman  R, Ganzini  L.  Bipolar mania and capacity to consent: recommendations for investigators and IRBs.  IRB. 2010;32(1):7-15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Moser  DJ, Schultz  SK, Arndt  S,  et al.  Capacity to provide informed consent for participation in schizophrenia and HIV research.  Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1201-1207.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Stiles  PG, Poythress  NG, Hall  A, Falkenbach  D, Williams  R.  Improving understanding of research consent disclosures among persons with mental illness.  Psychiatr Serv. 2001;52(6):780-785.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Bogardus  ST  Jr, Holmboe  E, Jekel  JF.  Perils, pitfalls, and possibilities in talking about medical risk.  JAMA. 1999;281(11):1037-1041.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Dubler  N.  New directions in human subject research: looking beyond the academic medical center: remaining faithful to the promises given: maintaining standards in changing times.  Seton Hall Law Rev. 2002;32(563):567-568.Google Scholar
33.
Volpe  RL.  Patients’ expressed and unexpressed needs for information for informed consent.  J Clin Ethics. 2010;21(1):45-57.PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Konald  C.  Informal conceptions of probability.  Cogn Instr. 1989;6:59-98.Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Yamagishi  K.  When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.24%.  Appl Cogn Psychol. 1997;11:495-506.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Tverskey  A, Kahneman  D.  Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability.  Cogn Psychol. 1973;5:207-232.Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Tversky  A, Kahneman  D.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.  Science. 1981;211(4481):453-458.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Jasper  JD, Goel  R, Einarson  A, Gallo  M, Koren  G.  Effects of framing on teratogenic risk perception in pregnant women.  Lancet. 2001;358(9289):1237-1238.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Fagerlin  A, Zikmund-Fisher  BJ, Ubel  PA.  “If I’m better than average, then I’m ok?”: comparative information influences beliefs about risk and benefits.  Patient Educ Couns. 2007;69(1-3):140-144.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Health Literacy  IOM.  A Prescription to End Confusion. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2004.
41.
Kirsch  I, Jungeblut  A, Jenkins  L, Kolstad  A.  Adult Literacy in America. Washington, DC: Office of Educational Research and Improvement, US Dept of Education; 1993.
42.
Hochhauser  M.  Informed consent and patient’s rights documents: a right, a rite, or a rewrite?  Ethics Behav. 1999;9(1):1-20.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Christopher  PP, Foti  ME, Roy-Bujnowski  K, Appelbaum  PS.  Consent form readability and educational levels of potential participants in mental health research.  Psychiatr Serv. 2007;58(2):227-232.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Baker  FM, Johnson  JT, Velli  SA, Wiley  C.  Congruence between education and reading levels of older persons.  Psychiatr Serv. 1996;47(2):194-196.PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Christensen  RC, Grace  GD.  The prevalence of low literacy in an indigent psychiatric population.  Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(2):262-263.PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Currier  GW, Sitzman  R, Trenton  A.  Literacy in the psychiatric emergency service.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189(1):56-58.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Robinson  G, Merav  A.  Informed consent: recall by patients tested postoperatively.  Bull Am Coll Surg. 1977;62(5):7-9, 30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Turner  P, Williams  C.  Informed consent: patients listen and read, but what information do they retain?  N Z Med J. 2002;115(1164):U218.PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Lloyd  A, Hayes  P, Bell  PR, Naylor  AR.  The role of risk and benefit perception in informed consent for surgery.  Med Decis Making. 2001;21(2):141-149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
50.
Meisel  A, Kuczewski  M.  Legal and ethical myths about informed consent.  Arch Intern Med. 1996;156(22):2521-2526.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
King  NM.  Defining and describing benefit appropriately in clinical trials.  J Law Med Ethics. 2000;28(4):332-343.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Dunn  LB, Lindamer  LA, Palmer  BW, Golshan  S, Schneiderman  LJ, Jeste  DV.  Improving understanding of research consent in middle-aged and elderly patients with psychotic disorders.  Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10(2):142-150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Dunn  LB, Jeste  DV.  Enhancing informed consent for research and treatment.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24(6):595-607.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Jeste  DV, Palmer  BW, Golshan  S,  et al.  Multimedia consent for research in people with schizophrenia and normal subjects: a randomized controlled trial.  Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(4):719-729.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Lipkus  IM, Hollands  JG.  The visual communication of risk.  J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr. 1999;25(25):149-163.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Gigerenzer  G, Edwards  A.  Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight.  BMJ. 2003;327(7417):741-744.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Benatar  JR, Mortimer  J, Stretton  M, Stewart  RAH.  A booklet on participants’ rights to improve consent for clinical research: a randomized trial.  PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47023. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047023.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Prentice  KJ, Appelbaum  PS, Conley  RR, Carpenter  WT.  Maintaining informed consent validity during lengthy research protocols.  IRB. 2007;29(6):1-6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Jeste  DV, Palmer  BW, Appelbaum  PS,  et al.  A new brief instrument for assessing decisional capacity for clinical research.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(8):966-974.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Dunn  LB, Nowrangi  MA, Palmer  BW, Jeste  DV, Saks  ER.  Assessing decisional capacity for clinical research or treatment: a review of instruments.  Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(8):1323-1334.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Anderson  EE, Iltis  AS.  Assessing and improving research participants’ understanding of risk: potential lessons from the literature on physician-patient risk communication.  J Empir Res Hum Res Ethics. 2008;3(3):27-37.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
DuBois  JM.  Ethics in Mental Health Research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007.
63.
Jefford  M, Moore  R.  Improvement of informed consent and the quality of consent documents.  Lancet Oncol. 2008;9(5):485-493.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Iltis  AS.  Lay concepts in informed consent to biomedical research: the capacity to understand and appreciate risk.  Bioethics. 2006;20(4):180-190.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Lidz  CW, Appelbaum  PS, Arnold  R,  et al.  How closely do institutional review boards follow the Common Rule?  Acad Med. 2012;87(7):969-974.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
DuBois  JM, Beskow  L, Campbell  J,  et al.  Restoring balance: a consensus statement on the protection of vulnerable research participants.  Am J Public Health. 2012;102(12):2220-2225.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Drazen  JM, Wood  AJ.  Don’t mess with the DSMB.  N Engl J Med. 2010;363(5):477-478.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Schneider  LS, Ismail  MS, Dagerman  K,  et al.  Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE): Alzheimer’s disease trial.  Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(1):57-72.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Stroup  TS, Appelbaum  PS.  Evaluation of “subject advocate” procedures in the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) schizophrenia study.  Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):147-152.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Pearson  JL, Stanley  B, King  CA, Fisher  CB.  Intervention research with persons at high risk for suicidality: safety and ethical considerations.  J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(suppl 25):17-26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
71.
Oquendo  MA, Stanley  B, Ellis  SP, Mann  JJ.  Protection of human subjects in intervention research for suicidal behavior.  Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(9):1558-1563.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Bruce  ML, Ten Have  TR, Reynolds  CF  III,  et al.  Reducing suicidal ideation and depressive symptoms in depressed older primary care patients: a randomized controlled trial.  JAMA. 2004;291(9):1081-1091.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Reynolds  CF  III, Degenholtz  H, Parker  LS,  et al.  Treatment as usual (TAU) control practices in the PROSPECT Study: managing the interaction and tension between research design and ethics.  Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16(6):602-608.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Brown  GK, Ten Have  TR, Henriques  GR, Xie  SX, Hollander  JE, Beck  AT.  Cognitive therapy for the prevention of suicide attempts: a randomized controlled trial.  JAMA. 2005;294(5):563-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Linehan  MM, Comtois  KA, Murray  AM,  et al.  Two-year randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behavior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors and borderline personality disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(7):757-766.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Clark  CC.  Group Leadership Skills for Nurses and Health Professionals. New York, NY: Springer; 2009.
77.
Linehan  MM, Comtois  KA, Ward-Ciesielski  EF.  Assessing and managing risk with suicidal individuals.  Cogn Behav Pract. 2012;19:218-232.Google ScholarCrossref
78.
Beskow  LM, Grady  C, Iltis  AS, Sadler  JZ, Wilfond  BS.  Points to consider: the research ethics consultation service and the IRB.  IRB. 2009;31(6):1-9. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004659.PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
McCormick  JB, Boyce  AM, Cho  MK.  Biomedical scientists’ perceptions of ethical and social implications: is there a role for research ethics consultation?  PLoS One. 2009;4(3):e4659.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Emanuel  EJ, Wendler  D, Grady  C.  What makes clinical research ethical?  JAMA. 2000;283(20):2701-2711.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Association of American Medical Colleges–Association of American Universities.  Protecting Patients, Preserving Integrity, Advancing Health: Accelerating the Implementation of COI Policies in Human Subjects Research. Washington, DC: Association of American Medical Colleges–Association of American Universities; 2008.
82.
Angell  M.  Industry-sponsored clinical research: a broken system.  JAMA. 2008;300(9):1069-1071.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Whitaker  R.  Mad in America: Bad Science, Bad Medicine, and the Enduring Mistreatment of the Mentally Ill. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Perseus Publishing; 2001.
84.
 Health care reform for Americans with severe mental illnesses: report of the National Advisory Mental Health Council.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(10):1447-1465.PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
 Research guidelines issued for persons with mental disorders and impaired capacity to make decisions.  Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50(1):128-129.PubMedGoogle Scholar
86.
Grof  P, Müller-Oerlinghausen  B.  A critical appraisal of lithium’s efficacy and effectiveness: the last 60 years.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(suppl 2):10-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Nivoli  AM, Murru  A, Vieta  E.  Lithium: still a cornerstone in the long-term treatment in bipolar disorder?  Neuropsychobiology. 2010;62(1):27-35.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Saunders  KE, Hawton  K.  The role of psychopharmacology in suicide prevention.  Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2009;18(3):172-178.PubMedGoogle Scholar
89.
Tondo  L, Baldessarini  RJ.  Long-term lithium treatment in the prevention of suicidal behavior in bipolar disorder patients.  Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2009;18(3):179-183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
90.
Heaton  PC, Garlick  CM, Tran  D.  Evaluating drug safety in children and adolescents with bipolar disorder.  Curr Drug Saf. 2006;1(3):307-318.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Aziz  R, Lorberg  B, Tampi  RR.  Treatments for late-life bipolar disorder.  Am J Geriatr Pharmacother. 2006;4(4):347-364.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Albanese  MJ, Pies  R.  The bipolar patient with comorbid substance use disorder: recognition and management.  CNS Drugs. 2004;18(9):585-596.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Conwell  Y, Duberstein  PR, Caine  ED.  Risk factors for suicide in later life.  Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(3):193-204.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Salloum  IM, Thase  ME.  Impact of substance abuse on the course and treatment of bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2000;2(3, pt 2):269-280.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Gelenberg  AJ, Pies  R.  Matching the bipolar patient and the mood stabilizer.  Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2003;15(3-4):203-216.PubMedGoogle Scholar
96.
Goodwin  FK, Goldstein  MA.  Optimizing lithium treatment in bipolar disorder: a review of the literature and clinical recommendations.  J Psychiatr Pract. 2003;9(5):333-343.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Scott  J, Tacchi  MJ.  A pilot study of concordance therapy for individuals with bipolar disorder who are non-adherent with lithium prophylaxis.  Bipolar Disord. 2002;4(6):386-392.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Mitchell  PB, Hadzi-Pavlovic  D.  Lithium treatment for bipolar disorder.  Bull World Health Organ. 2000;78(4):515-517.PubMedGoogle Scholar
99.
Dubois  JM, Bante  H, Hadley  WB.  Ethics in psychiatric research: a review of 25 years of NIH-funded empirical research projects.  AJOB Prim Res. 2011;2(4):5-17.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
100.
DelVecchio  P, Blyler  CR. Identifying critical outcomes and setting priorities for mental health services research. In: Wallcraft  J, Scharnk  B, Amerin  M, eds.  Handbook of Service User Involvement in Mental Health Research. New York, NY: Wiley; 2009:92-211.
×