Importance
The new DSM-5 “Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders” chapter contains a series of conditions thought to be etiologically related to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). However, the evidence to support this relatedness remains incomplete.
Objective
To estimate the degree to which genetic and environmental risk factors are shared and/or unique to dimensionally scored OCD, body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), hoarding disorder (HD), trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder) (TTM), and excoriation (skin-picking) disorder (SPD).
Design, Setting, and Participants
Multivariate twin modeling methods involving 5409 female members of the TwinsUK adult population-based twin register.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Scores on the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–Revised, the Dysmorphic Concern Questionnaire, the Hoarding Rating Scale, the Massachusetts General Hospital Hairpulling Scale, and the Skin Picking Scale.
Results
A 2–latent factor common pathway model fitted the data best; the first latent factor loaded on all 5 phenotypes, particularly on OCD, BDD, and HD. A second factor loaded exclusively on TTM and SPD. Disorder-specific genetic (for OCD, BDD, and HD only) and particularly nonshared environmental risk factors were also evident. Shared environmental influences were negligible.
Conclusions and Relevance
Obsessive-compulsive and related disorders may be influenced by 2 distinct liability factors rather than a single liability factor. One of these factors was common to all disorders, and another was exclusive to TTM and SPD. Disorder-specific genetic factors unique to OCD, BDD, and HD were also apparent, whereas TTM and SPD were largely influenced by the same latent genetic factor. Environmental influences were largely disorder specific. The results help explain the apparent similarities as well as some important differences between the disorders included in the new Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders chapter.
Quiz Ref IDThe DSM-5 includes a new “Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders” (OCRDs) chapter, comprising a series of mental disorders thought to be phenomenologically and etiologically related to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).1 The disorders classified alongside OCD are body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), hoarding disorder (HD), trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder) (TTM), and excoriation (skin-picking) disorder (SPD). Body dysmorphic disorder and TTM were previously classified in other sections of the DSM-IV, whereas HD and SPD are new to the nomenclature.2-4 There are similarities as well as differences across the OCRDs on a number of diagnostic validators, including symptomatology, patterns of comorbidity, familiality, brain circuitry, and treatment response.2 However, the removal of OCD from the anxiety disorders grouping and the inclusion of TTM and SPD in the new OCRDs chapter are somewhat controversial.5-7 Fundamentally, whether these conditions are etiologically related still remains to be conclusively demonstrated, as is the precise nature of such relatedness.
Perhaps the strongest evidence to support the relatedness of the OCRDs originates from controlled family studies, which have consistently reported higher rates of OCRDs in relatives of OCD probands compared with relatives of matched controls.8 However, family studies cannot address whether OCRDs run in families due to shared genetic risk factors, shared environmental risk factors, or both. Multivariate twin studies are better suited to examine the structure of genetic and environmental risk factors associated with the co-occurrence of various mental disorders. Such studies have, for instance, helped clarify the etiologic structure of personality disorders and other common psychiatric disorders.9,10 Using self-report questionnaires, one previous twin study suggested that the association between OCD and BDD symptoms is likely to be explained by genetic influences largely common to both disorders.11 However, no twin study has included all OCRDs in the same analysis; therefore, the extent to which OCRDs share genetic and environmental risk factors remains unknown.
The present twin study aims to clarify the structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for dimensional representations of DSM-5 OCRDs in a large community sample of adult female twins. On the basis of previous family and twin studies, we predicted that the liability to individual OCRDs would be largely influenced by genetic and nonshared environmental factors, while shared environmental effects would be negligible.12-19 Furthermore, consistent with the new DSM-5 classification, we expected the covariance between the OCRDs to be largely explained by a single latent phenotypic construct conferring risk to all disorders, as well as disorder-specific genetic and environmental influences.
Participants were monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins from the TwinsUK adult twin register (www.twinsuk.ac.uk). The register is a volunteer, general population sample of approximately 10 000 white adult twins aged 16 to 90 years.20 The sample is comparable to age-matched population singletons in terms of disease-related and lifestyle characteristics.21 Zygosity was determined using the Peas in a Pod Questionnaire22 and further established via DNA confirmation methods in uncertain cases. The study was approved by the St Thomas’ Hospital Research Ethics Committee, and all participants provided written informed consent.
Five dimensional self-report measures of OCRDs were sent to all active twins in the register in different waves of data collection. Response rates ranged between 49.1% (BDD) and 63.3% (TTM and SPD). The sample consisted of 6310 individual twins who completed at least one of the questionnaires (2752 complete and 806 incomplete pairs). Male twins (n = 666), DZ opposite-sex twins (n = 122), and twins for whom co-twin sex (n = 90) or zygosity (n = 23) was unknown were excluded since the small number of cases in these categories did not allow sufficient power to investigate sex differences. Twin analyses were carried out on a final sample of female twins (N = 5409), including 3042 MZ and 2367 DZ female twins.
The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–Revised, a widely used, 18-item measure of distress associated with various OCD symptoms,23 has strong reliability, validity, and clinical utility; correlates strongly with other measures of OCD; and discriminates well between OCD and other anxiety disorders.23,24 A cutoff score of 21 discriminates patients with OCD from nonpatients, with a sensitivity of 78.2% and a specificity of 65.0%.24 In the current study, the hoarding subscale was excluded from the total score, and Cronbach α was 0.87.
The Dysmorphic Concern Questionnaire is a 7-item self-report questionnaire assessing the extent of concern with one’s own physical appearance and body malfunction (eg, body odor). Its items measure a broad dimensional construct closely related, but not limited to, BDD. The scale has strong psychometric properties; correlates well with other measures of BDD symptoms, including the BDD version of the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; and discriminates patients from controls.25-28 An empirically derived cutoff score of 11 is indicative of a likely BDD diagnosis, with a sensitivity of 89.1% and a specificity of 94.7%.25-28 Cronbach α was 0.85.
The Hoarding Rating Scale29 is a 5-item self-report questionnaire measuring clutter, difficulty discarding, excessive acquisition, distress, and impairment. The scale is psychometrically sound, with much empirical support for its reliability, validity, and clinical utility.29-31 Scores above 17 discriminate between hoarding and nonhoarding cases, with a sensitivity and a specificity of 95%.29 Cronbach α was 0.86.
The Massachusetts General Hospital Hairpulling Scale,32-34 a 7-item self-report instrument assessing the severity of hair pulling, measures the frequency, intensity, and control over urges to pull hair, as well as the frequency, distress, resistance, and control over hair-pulling behaviors. To our knowledge, no studies have identified cutoff scores, but the scale has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and strong convergent and divergent validity.32-34 Cronbach α was 0.93.
The Skin Picking Scale,35 a 6-item self-report instrument, assesses the extent and severity of skin-picking behavior, including the frequency and intensity of urges, frequency of skin-picking behavior, and the associated interference, distress, and avoidance. The scale has excellent psychometric properties, supporting its reliability and validity.35 A cutoff value of 7 differentiates between self-injurious and non–self-injurious skin pickers, with a sensitivity of 96.4% and a specificity of 92.2%.35 In the current study, Cronbach α was 0.78.
Univariate twin analyses seek to decompose phenotypic variance into 3 factors: A (additive genetic; ie, the proportion of phenotypic variation attributed to genetic factors), C (common or shared environment; ie, environmental effects shared by twins), and E (unique environment; ie, environmental effects unique to each twin, plus measurement error). In multivariate twin analyses, the covariance between multiple phenotypes is partitioned in much the same way.
Three models are commonly tested in multivariate twin analyses. The Cholesky decomposition estimates as many A, C, and E factors as phenotypes under study, making no assumptions regarding the genetic and environmental structure underlying their covariance. The independent pathway (IP) model estimates a set of common genetic and environmental factors hypothesized to directly influence all phenotypes. Finally, the common pathway (CP) model, nested within the IP model, assumes that genetic and environmental factors act via 1 or more latent factors to influence all phenotypes. Both the IP and CP models allow specific genetic and environmental influences to account for the remaining disorder-specific variance. Both models can be extended to have multiple genetic and environmental factors, albeit with additional constraints to identify the model. The Akaike information criterion value provides an indication of the goodness of fit of these models. To explain the pattern of covariance using as few parameters as possible, researchers commonly test reduced submodels against the best-fitting full model.36
Since the data were substantially skewed, we used liability-threshold modeling.37 This approach is well suited to the study of continuously distributed traits. Consistent with previous studies by our group,11,15,16,18,19 scores on each scale were categorized using 3 thresholds, ranging from 0 (ie, no symptoms) to 3 (ie, clinically significant symptoms, with scores above empirically derived cutoffs for each measure) (eTable 1 in the Supplement).
Twin analyses were carried out using Mx (http://www.vcu.edu/mx/). Data were first fitted to a saturated Cholesky ACE model. Varimax- and oblimin-rotated principal component analyses of the total scores on the self-report measures of OCRDs were carried out in SPSS (version 20; SPSS, Inc) to examine their phenotypic structure. On the basis of these results, we tested an IP model containing the resulting genetic, shared environmental, and unique environmental factors common to all disorders, as well as disorder-specific factors. This model was then compared with a CP model, in which the common influences acted via latent phenotypic factors. Goodness of fit was established by likelihood ratio and the Akaike information criterion. Reduced submodels were then tested; the difference in the χ2 value relative to the change in degrees of freedom provided an indication of the goodness of fit of the simplified models.36
Polychoric Correlations and Principal Component Analyses
Phenotypic correlations (with 95% CIs) are presented in Table 1. As expected, we found moderate phenotypic associations across all variables; the stronger associations were between OCD, BDD, and HD on one hand and TTM and SPD on the other. Cross-twin within-trait and cross-twin cross-trait correlations for MZ and DZ twins are also depicted in Table 1. The pattern of correlations (ie, strongest in MZ vs DZ pairs) suggests a meaningful genetic contribution to the liability of each trait and to their co-occurrence. The moderate MZ correlations are indicative of unique environmental influences.
The principal component analyses retained 2 phenotypic factors with eigenvalues greater than 1, explaining 54.7% of the total variance (eTable 2 in the Supplement). Factor 1 included OCD, BDD, and HD, while factor 2 included TTM and SPD.
For model-fitting results, see Table 2. The 2-factor CP model (model 3), with a first latent factor loading on all variables and a second one constrained to load on TTM and SPD only, provided a better fit to the data than the 2-factor IP (model 2), as indicated by a more negative Akaike information criterion value. To explain the data using as few parameters as possible, we fitted nested submodels 4 through 6. A CP model with only 1 common latent factor (model 4) produced a significantly worse fit, indicating the significant contribution of the second phenotypic latent factor in explaining common variance. Dropping disorder-specific genetic factors (model 6) also resulted in a poorer fit, providing evidence of their contribution in the liability to OCRD traits. A better fit was obtained by dropping all shared environmental parameters (model 5). Finally, we reduced model 5 by setting the contributions of specific genetic influences for TTM and SPD to 0, without loss of fit.
According to the best-fitting model (model 7), the covariance between the 5 OCD-related phenotypes was best explained by a 2–latent factor structure. The first factor loaded highly on all variables, particularly on OCD (0.76), HD (0.63), and BDD (0.47), while a second latent factor loaded on TTM and SPD. Both latent factors were strongly influenced by genetic factors (0.63-0.74). Disorder-specific genetic factors moderately contributed to the risk for OCD, BDD, and HD symptoms but not for TTM and SPD, suggesting the genetic influences on these 2 phenotypes are mostly shared. Unique environmental risk factors (including measurement error) appeared to be mostly disorder specific, with estimates ranging between 0.31 (OCD) and 0.55 (TTM). Finally, shared environmental factors played a marginal role. Parameter estimates under the best-fitting model (model 7) are shown in Figure 1 and Table 3. Figure 2 depicts the proportion of genetic and unique environmental variance shared among the OCRDs vs disorder specific.
The new DSM-5 OCRDs chapter includes a series of disorders thought to be etiologically related to OCD. However, the evidence to support this relatedness is incomplete and, perhaps unsurprisingly, the field has yet to universally agree with the amalgamation of some of these disorders. We used multivariate twin models to clarify the degree to which genetic and environmental risk factors are shared and/or unique to the 5 OCRDs. We draw the following main conclusions.
OCRDs Are Moderately Heritable
Quiz Ref IDAs predicted, the heritability of all OCRD symptoms was moderate, ranging from 31.6% (TTM) to 51.1% (HD); nonshared environment (plus measurement error) accounted for the remaining variance, while shared environmental factors appeared to be negligible. In the only previous TTM study, which included 34 twin pairs recruited from a patient organization (the Trichotillomania Learning Centre), heritability estimates ranged between 0.76 and 0.78.17 To our knowledge, ours is the first study to estimate the contribution of genetic and environmental risk factors to TTM symptoms in a large community twin sample, resulting in substantially lower heritability estimates (32%). These discrepancies are likely attributable to the substantial methodologic differences between the 2 studies (ie, volunteers from a patient organization vs a population-based twin sample).
Two, Rather Than One, Common Liability Factors
The pattern of co-occurrence between OCRDs was better explained by 2 common liability factors rather than a single liability factor. Both latent factors were substantially heritable, with approximately one-third of the variance attributable to nonshared environment (plus measurement error).
Quiz Ref IDThe first latent factor had substantial loadings on all OCRDs. Largely influenced by genes (63.3%; 95% CI, 59%-66%), this factor could be conceptualized as a nonspecific genetic vulnerability conferring risk to all OCRDs and possibly explaining the phenomenologic similarities, patterns of comorbidity, and familiality described in the OCRD literature.8,38-41 While all 5 OCRDs loaded on this common “OCRD latent factor,” the strongest loadings were for OCD, BDD, and HD, the so-called cognitive OCRDs.2 By contrast, TTM and SPD, sometimes referred to as “body-focused repetitive behavioral disorders”4 because they lack a prominent cognitive component, had substantially lower loadings on this factor.
Disorder-specific genetic influences were also evident for OCD, BDD, and HD. The implication for molecular genetic studies is that, in addition to generalist “pleitropic” genes conferring risk to all OCRDs, it is likely that there will be a set of disorder-specific genes for OCD, BDD, and HD. While the search for OCD-related genes is well under way with the recent publication of the first genome-wide association study,42 gene-searching efforts in the remaining OCRDs lag considerably behind. Recruiting and combining sufficiently large samples of individuals with all OCRDs rather than conducting separate studies for each OCRD may potentially facilitate the search for common and disorder-specific genes for these disorders.43 In this regard, the new OCRDs chapter could potentially stimulate such endeavors.44
The second latent factor, which was also strongly genetically influenced (73.7%; 95% CI, 30%-99%), loaded exclusively on TTM and SPD, suggesting a set of genetic influences exclusive to these 2 disorders. In clinical samples, there is a high degree of symptom overlap and comorbidity among TTM, SPD, and other body-focused repetitive behaviors (eg, nail biting, thumb sucking, and knuckle cracking).4 Some authors have suggested that TTM and SPD are strongly etiologically related.45-47 Our finding that all genetic variance of the second latent factor is shared between TTM and SPD would in fact suggest that these disorders might represent alternative phenotypic expressions of the same condition, with most differences being attributable to environmental risk factors unique to each disorder (see below). The implication for molecular genetic studies is that the same genes will likely confer risk to both TTM and SPD (and possibly other body-focused repetitive behaviors) and that these genes will be distinct from those conferring general risk to OCRDs. Gene-searching efforts in TTM and SPD are still in their infancy, but some promising susceptibility genes have been identified, such as SAPAP3.48-50
Quiz Ref IDThe bifactorial etiologic architecture of OCRDs has not been previously reported, to our knowledge, and may help explain both the apparent similarities and differences between the “cognitive” and the “body-focused repetitive” OCRDs. Indeed, while there are some similarities between these disorders,2,44 there are also important differences in terms of symptomatology (eg, absence of obsessions and gratification or pleasure after the repetitive behavior in some patients with TTM and SPD), sex distribution (TTM and SPD more frequent in females), and treatment response (poor or incomplete response to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and preferential response to habit-reversal therapy in TTM and SPD).2
Future twin studies should examine the overlap between OCRDs and other phenotypes, such as anxiety and mood disorders, since some of the “generalist” genes, conferring risk to all OCRDs, may also be shared with other emotional disorders.43,51 This work may shed further light on the somewhat controversial removal of OCD from the anxiety disorders grouping in the DSM-5.
Disorder-Specific Environmental Factors
Quiz Ref IDMost unique environmental risk factors appear to be disorder specific. The implication for future research is that the identification of environmental risk factors that contribute to developing one particular OCRD may not necessarily confer risk to acquiring another. Research in this area is sparse and methodologically questionable, with some inconsistent evidence for perinatal complications, traumatic life events, and streptococcal infections as potential nonspecific environmental risk factors for various OCRDs.52,53 Disorder-specific environmental risk factors are yet to be identified, but some research suggests that appearance-related teasing may be important in BDD,54,55 while trauma-related loss may be highly relevant in HD.56 Further longitudinal, population-based research is clearly needed to identify specific environmental factors conferring risk to each of the OCRDs. This will be equally if not more important than gene-searching efforts since environmental factors are potentially amenable to prevention and early intervention. Similarly, the identification of gene-environment correlations and gene-×-environment interactions will remain a major goal for the future.
Twin analyses were limited to adult female white twins; future studies should include male participants and extend to younger cohorts. Symptoms of OCRDs were measured using self-report questionnaires primarily validated on DSM-IV diagnostic criteria, although their items map reasonably well onto the new DSM-5 criteria. Of all applied measures, the Dysmorphic Concern Questionnaire shows a less perfect match with DSM-5 BDD criteria; it includes items on bodily function and thus measures a closely related, but somewhat broader, construct. Using self-report questionnaires, we were also unable to determine whether OCRDs were attributable to other mental or medical conditions. Future studies including psychiatric diagnostic interviews for DSM-5 OCRDs are strongly encouraged. Finally, findings need to be interpreted in view of the general limitations of the twin design, particularly the equal environment assumption.57
In conclusion, OCRDs have a complex etiologic architecture consisting of 2 latent liability factors largely under genetic control. One of these factors was common to all disorders, and another was exclusive to TTM and SPD. Disorder-specific genetic factors unique to OCD, BDD, and HD were also apparent, whereas environmental influences were largely disorder specific. The results help explain the apparent similarities as well as some important differences between the disorders included in the new OCRDs chapter. Further research is needed to identify common as well as disorder-specific genes and nonshared environmental risk factors that increase the susceptibility to these disabling conditions, as well as their likely interaction.
Submitted for Publication: April 3, 2013; final revision received June 28, 2013; accepted August 8, 2013.
Corresponding Author: Benedetta Monzani, PhD, Department of Psychosis Studies, King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, PO Box 69, De Crespigny Park Rd, London SE5 8AF, England (benedetta.monzani@kcl.ac.uk).
Published Online: December 25, 2013. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3524.
Author Contributions: Drs Monzani and Mataix-Cols had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analyses.
Study concept and design: Monzani, Harris, Mataix-Cols.
Acquisition of data: Harris.
Analysis and interpretation of data: Monzani, Rijsdijk, Mataix-Cols.
Drafting of the manuscript: All authors.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Monzani, Rijsdijk.
Obtained funding: Mataix-Cols.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Harris.
Study supervision: Rijsdijk, Mataix-Cols.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by The Wellcome Trust and the Department of Health via the National Institute for Health Research Comprehensive Biomedical Research Centre award to Guy’s & St Thomas’ National Health Service Foundation Trust, in partnership with King’s College London.
Role of the Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
2.Phillips
KA, Stein
DJ, Rauch
SL,
et al. Should an obsessive-compulsive spectrum grouping of disorders be included in
DSM-V?
Depress Anxiety. 2010;27(6):528-555.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Mataix-Cols
D, Frost
RO, Pertusa
A,
et al. Hoarding disorder: a new diagnosis for
DSM-V?
Depress Anxiety. 2010;27(6):556-572.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Stein
DJ, Grant
JE, Franklin
ME,
et al. Trichotillomania (hair pulling disorder), skin picking disorder, and stereotypic movement disorder: toward
DSM-V.
Depress Anxiety. 2010;27(6):611-626.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Mataix-Cols
D, Pertusa
A, Leckman
JF. Issues for
DSM-V: how should obsessive-compulsive and related disorders be classified?
Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(9):1313-1314.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Storch
EA, Abramowitz
J, Goodman
WK. Where does obsessive-compulsive disorder belong in
DSM-V?
Depress Anxiety. 2008;25(4):336-347.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Starcevic
V, Janca
A. Obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders: still in search of the concept-affirming boundaries.
Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2011;24(1):55-60.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Bienvenu
OJ, Samuels
JF, Wuyek
LA,
et al. Is obsessive-compulsive disorder an anxiety disorder, and what, if any, are spectrum conditions? a family study perspective.
Psychol Med. 2012;42(1):1-13.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Kendler
KS, Aggen
SH, Czajkowski
N,
et al. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for
DSM-IV personality disorders: a multivariate twin study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(12):1438-1446.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Kendler
KS, Prescott
CA, Myers
J, Neale
MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(9):929-937.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Monzani
B, Rijsdijk
F, Iervolino
AC, Anson
M, Cherkas
L, Mataix-Cols
D. Evidence for a genetic overlap between body dysmorphic concerns and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in an adult female community twin sample.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B(4):376-382.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Mataix-Cols
D, Boman
M, Monzani
B,
et al. Population-based, multigenerational family clustering study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(7):709-717.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.van Grootheest
DS, Cath
DC, Beekman
AT, Boomsma
DI. Twin studies on obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review.
Twin Res Hum Genet. 2005;8(5):450-458.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Van Grootheest
DS, Cath
DC, Beekman
AT, Boomsma
DI. Genetic and environmental influences on obsessive-compulsive symptoms in adults: a population-based twin-family study.
Psychol Med. 2007;37(11):1635-1644.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Iervolino
AC, Perroud
N, Fullana
MA,
et al. Prevalence and heritability of compulsive hoarding: a twin study.
Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(10):1156-1161.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Novak
CE, Keuthen
NJ, Stewart
SE, Pauls
DL. A twin concordance study of trichotillomania.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(7):944-949.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Monzani
B, Rijsdijk
F, Cherkas
L, Harris
J, Keuthen
N, Mataix-Cols
D. Prevalence and heritability of skin picking in an adult community sample: a twin study.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2012;159B(5):605-610.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Iervolino
AC, Rijsdijk
FV, Cherkas
L, Fullana
MA, Mataix-Cols
D. A multivariate twin study of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(6):637-644.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Spector
TD, MacGregor
AJ. The St. Thomas’ UK Adult Twin Registry.
Twin Res. 2002;5(5):440-443.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Andrew
T, Hart
DJ, Snieder
H, de Lange
M, Spector
TD, MacGregor
AJ. Are twins and singletons comparable? a study of disease-related and lifestyle characteristics in adult women.
Twin Res. 2001;4(6):464-477.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 22.Sarna
S, Kaprio
J, Sistonen
P, Koskenvuo
M. Diagnosis of twin zygosity by mailed questionnaire.
Hum Hered. 1978;28(4):241-254.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Foa
EB, Huppert
JD, Leiberg
S,
et al. The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version.
Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485-496.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Abramowitz
JS, Deacon
BJ. Psychometric properties and construct validity of the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–Revised: replication and extension with a clinical sample.
J Anxiety Disord. 2006;20(8):1016-1035.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Jorgensen
L, Castle
D, Roberts
C, Groth-Marnat
G. A clinical validation of the Dysmorphic Concern Questionnaire.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001;35(1):124-128.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Mancuso
SG, Knoesen
NP, Castle
DJ. The Dysmorphic Concern Questionnaire: a screening measure for body dysmorphic disorder.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry.2010;44(6):535-542.
Google Scholar 27.Stangier
U, Janich
C, Adam-Schwebe
S, Berger
P, Wolter
M. Screening for body dysmorphic disorder in dermatological outpatients.
Dermatol Psychosom.2003;4(2):66-71.
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Oosthuizen
P, Lambert
T, Castle
DJ. Dysmorphic concern: prevalence and associations with clinical variables.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1998;32(1):129-132.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Tolin
DF, Frost
RO, Steketee
G. A brief interview for assessing compulsive hoarding: the Hoarding Rating Scale–Interview.
Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(1):147-152.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Tolin
DF, Frost
RO, Steketee
G, Gray
KD, Fitch
KE. The economic and social burden of compulsive hoarding.
Psychiatry Res. 2008;160(2):200-211.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Tolin
DF, Frost
RO, Steketee
G, Fitch
KE. Family burden of compulsive hoarding: results of an Internet survey.
Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(3):334-344.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Keuthen
NJ, O’Sullivan
RL, Ricciardi
JN,
et al. The Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) Hairpulling Scale, 1: development and factor analyses.
Psychother Psychosom. 1995;64(3-4):141-145.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.O’Sullivan
RL, Keuthen
NJ, Hayday
CF,
et al. The Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) Hairpulling Scale, 2: reliability and validity.
Psychother Psychosom. 1995;64(3-4):146-148.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Diefenbach
GJ, Tolin
DF, Crocetto
J, Maltby
N, Hannan
S. Assessment of trichotillomania: a psychometric evaluation of hair-pulling scales.
J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2005;27(3):169-178.
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Keuthen
NJ, Wilhelm
S, Deckersbach
T,
et al. The Skin Picking Scale: scale construction and psychometric analyses.
J Psychosom Res. 2001;50(6):337-341.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Neale
M, Cardon
LR. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic; 1992.
37.Rijsdijk
FV, Sham
PC. Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models.
Brief Bioinform. 2002;3(2):119-133.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Pavan
C, Simonato
P, Marini
M, Mazzoleni
F, Pavan
L, Vindigni
V. Psychopathologic aspects of body dysmorphic disorder: a literature review.
Aesthetic Plast Surg. 2008;32(3):473-484.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Phillips
KA, Menard
W, Fay
C, Weisberg
R. Demographic characteristics, phenomenology, comorbidity, and family history in 200 individuals with body dysmorphic disorder.
Psychosomatics. 2005;46(4):317-325.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Grant
JE, Stein
DJ, Woods
D. Trichotillomania, Skin Picking, and Other Body-Focused Repetitive Behaviors. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2011.
42.Stewart
SE, Yu
D, Scharf
JM,
et al. Genome-wide association study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(7):788-798.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Taylor
S, Jang
KL, Asmundson
GJ. Etiology of obsessions and compulsions: a behavioral-genetic analysis.
J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(4):672-682.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Hollander
E, Kim
S, Braun
A, Simeon
D, Zohar
J. Cross-cutting issues and future directions for the OCD spectrum.
Psychiatry Res. 2009;170(1):3-6.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Lochner
C, Simeon
D, Niehaus
DJ, Stein
DJ. Trichotillomania and skin-picking: a phenomenological comparison.
Depress Anxiety. 2002;15(2):83-86.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Odlaug
BL, Grant
JE. Trichotillomania and pathologic skin picking: clinical comparison with an examination of comorbidity.
Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2008;20(2):57-63.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Snorrason
I, Belleau
EL, Woods
DW. How related are hair pulling disorder (trichotillomania) and skin picking disorder? a review of evidence for comorbidity, similarities and shared etiology.
Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32(7):618-629.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Welch
JM, Lu
J, Rodriguiz
RM,
et al. Cortico-striatal synaptic defects and OCD-like behaviours in SAPAP3 mutant mice.
Nature. 2007;448(7156):894-900.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Züchner
S, Wendland
JR, Ashley-Koch
AE,
et al. Multiple rare
SAPAP3 missense variants in trichotillomania and OCD.
Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14(1):6-9.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Bienvenu
OJ, Wang
Y, Shugart
YY,
et al.
Sapap3 and pathological grooming in humans: results from the OCD Collaborative Genetics Study.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(5):710-720.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Bolhuis
K, McAdams
TA, Monzani
B,
et al. Aetiological overlap between obsessive-compulsive and depressive symptoms: a longitudinal twin study in adolescents and adults [published online August 7, 2013].
Psychol Med. doi:10.1017/S0033291713001591.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 52.Grisham
JR, Fullana
MA, Mataix-Cols
D, Moffitt
TE, Caspi
A, Poulton
R. Risk factors prospectively associated with adult obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Psychol Med. 2011;41(12):2495-2506.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Murphy
DL, Timpano
KR, Wheaton
MG, Greenberg
BD, Miguel
EC. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and its related disorders: a reappraisal of obsessive-compulsive spectrum concepts.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2010;12(2):131-148.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 54.Buhlmann
U, Cook
LM, Fama
JM, Wilhelm
S. Perceived teasing experiences in body dysmorphic disorder.
Body Image. 2007;4(4):381-385.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Phillips
KA. Understanding Body Dysmorphic Disorder. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2009.
56.Landau
D, Iervolino
AC, Pertusa
A, Santo
S, Singh
S, Mataix-Cols
D. Stressful life events and material deprivation in hoarding disorder.
J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25(2):192-202.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref