Importance
Psychosis-risk studies have examined help-seeking adolescents and young adults. Population-based studies evaluating psychotic symptoms and neurocognitive performance across childhood are needed for “growth charting” cognitive development. We hypothesized that psychosis spectrum youths have delayed neurocognitive age relative to chronological age. We expected larger lags with increased symptom severity and in late adolescence and early adulthood.
Objectives
To examine neurocognitive age and compare typically developing participants with psychosis spectrum participants.
Design, Setting, and Participants
The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort is a genotyped sample, with electronic medical records, enrolled in the study of brain behavior. In an academic and children’s hospital health care network, a structured psychiatric evaluation was performed and a computerized neurocognitive battery administered to evaluate performance in several domains. From 18 344 youths in the recruitment pool who were aged 8 to 21 years, physically and cognitively capable of participating, and proficient in English, participants were randomly selected with stratification for age, sex, and ethnicity. A total of 9138 participants were enrolled in the study between November 1, 2009, and November 30, 2011, and 2321 endorsed psychotic symptoms: 1423 significant (psychosis spectrum) and 898 limited (psychosis limited). They had no comorbid medical conditions. They were compared with 981 participants endorsing significant other psychiatric symptoms and with 1963 typically developing children with no psychiatric or medical disorders.
Main Outcomes and Measures
The computerized neurocognitive battery provides accuracy and speed measures on 12 tests and speed measures alone on 2, yielding 26 measures used in a regression analysis to predict chronological age. Prediction was performed on the entire set and separately for each domain (executive, episodic memory, complex cognition, social cognition, and sensorimotor speed).
Results
Throughout childhood and adolescence, the psychosis spectrum group had lower predicted age compared with the typically developing group and the group with other psychiatric symptoms. The psychosis spectrum group had a greater developmental lag than the psychosis limited group. The lags were most pronounced for complex cognition and social cognition and were smallest for sensorimotor speed.
Conclusions and Relevance
Individuals who endorse psychotic symptoms are neurocognitively delayed across the age range; this delay relates to symptom severity and is not prominent in other psychiatric disorders. Combined clinical and neurocognitive assessment can facilitate early detection and targeted intervention to delay or ameliorate disease progression.
Across medicine, early identification of disorders is increasingly recognized as critical for optimizing therapeutic interventions. The schizophrenia prodrome is a prolonged phase of subthreshold symptoms and functional decline preceding diagnostic conversion.1,2 A growing literature has examined the clinical course of help-seeking individuals with prodromal presentation and gauged rates of conversion to schizophrenia.3,4 Because impaired cognition is a core feature of schizophrenia associated with disrupted functioning,5,6 prodromal investigations have incorporated neurocognitive tests sensitive to deficits in schizophrenia.7,8 Meta-analyses conclude that at-risk individuals show a similar deficit profile, albeit with less impairment than evident in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia.9,10 While longitudinal studies are limited, individuals who transition to psychosis manifest greater neurocognitive deficits at intake.9,10
Epidemiologic studies report psychotic symptoms during childhood,11-13 with prevalence of 17% in children (aged 9-12 years) and 7.5% in adolescents (aged 13-18 years),14 but the link to cognition is unknown.1 One study examined psychosis and cognition15 (212 children, aged 11-13 years), reporting 8% prevalence for the risk syndrome associated with processing speed and working memory deficits. Notably, the age range is lower than late adolescence and early adulthood examined in prodromal clinics.
Population-based studies combining clinical and neurocognitive assessment can help transcend symptom-based classifications and incorporate phenotypic biomarkers for integration with genomics. Because schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental disorder, concomitant evaluation of psychosis and cognition is required for creating “growth charts” of cognitive development in psychosis spectrum (PS) individuals relative to typically developing (TD) individuals. Such charts can help integrate brain-behavior phenotypes in a developmental context and detect course characteristics related to psychosis vulnerability, providing tools for staging and intervention.
The developmental course of cognitive performance has been studied with diverse tests.16-18 We have implemented a computerized neurocognitive battery (CNB)19 in large-scale studies,20-23 measuring performance in executive, episodic memory, complex cognition, social cognition, and sensorimotor speed domains. Deficits prominent in schizophrenia are heritable and are milder in unaffected family members.20,21,23-25 The sensitivity and validity of the CNB was established in a developmental cohort of 3500 Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort participants in whom we documented age and sex effects on performance.26
To generate neurocognitive age growth charts, indices are calculated reflecting predicted age based on performance. When neurocognitive age lags behind chronological age, there is a delay or below age-expected performance; conversely, when neurocognitive age surpasses chronological age, there is accelerated or above-expectation performance. Herein, we examine neurocognitive age in the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort sample and compare TD participants with those who endorsed psychotic symptoms. We hypothesized that youths with psychotic features have lower neurocognitive age relative to chronological age. We expected a larger gap with increased symptom severity and in late adolescence and early adulthood, a dynamic maturational period associated with clinical presentation of schizophrenia. We preliminarily evaluated the specificity of the findings by comparing PS individuals with and without other psychiatric morbidity and by examining a group endorsing other psychiatric symptoms.
The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort included children (aged 8-21 years) recruited through a National Institute of Mental Health Grand Opportunity study characterizing clinical and neurobehavioral phenotypes in a genotyped, prospectively accrued community cohort. Participants initially provided written consent for genomic studies when they presented for pediatric services to the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia health care network. They provided blood samples for genetic studies, authorized access to electronic medical records, and provided written informed consent or assent to be recontacted for future studies. Of the 50 540 genotyped subjects, 18 344 met the following entrance criteria and were randomly selected, stratified for age, sex, and ethnicity: (1) ambulatory in stable health; (2) proficient in English; (3) physically and cognitively capable of participating in an interview and performing the CNB; and (4) absence of a disorder that markedly impairs motility or cognition (eg, paresis or palsy, intellectual disability). Notably, participants were not recruited from psychiatric clinics and the sample is not enriched for individuals who seek psychiatric help. Eligible participants were contacted by letter, which was followed by a telephone call that provided an opportunity to ask questions and schedule the study. A total of 9138 participants enrolled between November 1, 2009, and November 30, 2011, and were available for this analysis. Medical comorbidity was established using interview and electronic medical records. Severity of medical conditions was quantified as the following: 1 indicates none, no ongoing conditions requiring sustained intervention or interference with functioning; 2, mild, conditions requiring pediatric visits and sometimes medications but mild in severity; 3, moderate, conditions requiring standing medications and monitoring; and 4, severe, conditions requiring procedures and monitoring and that can be life threatening. Participants provided written informed consent or assent after a description of the study. Institutional review boards at the University of Pennsylvania and the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia approved the protocol.
Participants with medical ratings lower than 3 who endorsed psychotic symptoms were compared with TD youths without psychiatric symptoms. Following established criteria, we defined a PS group and a psychosis limited (PL) group. To address specificity, we included all participants with medical ratings lower than 3 meeting criteria for other psychiatric disorders (OP): mood, anxiety, attention-deficit, disruptive behavior, and eating disorders. Groups differed in race composition, parental education, and Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) scores (Table 1).
The program GOASSESS computerizes the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL).27 Participants aged 11 to 21 years were interviewed individually; collateral information was obtained independently from a caregiver for children aged 8 to 17 years. GOASSESS records demographic characteristics, life events, school performance, updated medical history, screener for psychopathology, Family Interview for Genetic Studies,28 Global Assessment of Functioning,29 and assessor’s observations. GOASSESS probes major psychiatric disorders defined in DSM-IV-TR.29 We evaluated the presence, duration, distress, and impairment associated with symptoms of anxiety, mood, attention-deficit, disruptive behavior, and eating disorders. Psychosis was assessed with added emphasis on subthreshold symptoms. For children older than 11 years, a substance use scale was self-administered.30 The clinical assessment took about 2 hours. Assessors were trained extensively (by M.E.C.), observed, certified, and monitored.
GOASSESS included 3 measures of psychotic symptoms: (1) subthreshold positive symptoms in the past year were assessed with the 12-item assessor-administered PRIME Screen–Revised (PS-R)31 (items were read out loud and self-rated on a 7-point scale [0 indicates definitely disagree; 6, definitely agree], and endorsement is followed by indicating onset of each endorsed symptom rated ≥4); (2) lifetime threshold hallucinations and delusions were evaluated with the K-SADS-PL psychosis questions and structured follow-up probes (a positive psychosis screen required either proband or collateral endorsing hallucinations or delusions lasting ≥1 day outside the context of substance use, illness, or medication use and with significant impairment or distress [rating ≥5 on an 11-point scale ranging from 0-10]); and (3) negative and disorganized symptoms were evaluated using 6 assessor-rated items from the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS)32 (item N2, avolition; N3, expression of emotion; N4, experience of emotions and self; N6, occupational functioning; D3, trouble with focus and attention; and P5, disorganized communication). The SIPS focus and attention scale was rated based on responses to the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder items; expression of emotion was based on interviewer observations throughout the assessment; and the remaining items were based on specific open-ended questions from the SIPS. Participants were included as PS if they met at least 1 of the following criteria: (1) were rated at severity 2 SDs or greater above their age-matched peers on the PS-R or SIPS; (2) had at least 1 PS-R item rated 6 or at least 3 items rated 5 (somewhat agree)31; or (3) endorsed definite or possible hallucinations or delusions on the K-SADS-PL psychosis screen. The PL group included individuals who did not meet criteria for PS but endorsed more subpsychotic (PS-R) or subthreshold negative or disorganized (SIPS) symptoms than their age-matched peers (≥1 SD of the mean). Note that the PL group is smaller than the PS group because it is based only on the age-normed PS-R and Scale of Prodromal Symptoms scores.
Neurocognitive Assessment
The 1-hour CNB includes 14 tests assessing 5 neurobehavioral domains: executive (abstraction and mental flexibility, attention, working memory), episodic memory (words, faces, shapes), complex cognition (verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, spatial processing), social cognition (emotion identification, emotion intensity differentiation, age differentiation), and sensorimotor speed (motor, sensorimotor).19,26 Except for the tests designed exclusively for measuring speed, each test provides measures of both accuracy and speed. The reading subtest of the WRAT33 was administered first to determine participants’ ability to complete the battery and to provide an estimate of IQ.
Raw CNB scores were standardized (z transformed) as previously detailed.26 For consistency of interpretation, higher z scores always reflect better performance; z scores where higher numbers reflected poorer performance (ie, response time) were multiplied by −1.
To develop the neurocognitive growth chart, we performed a regression analysis with 10-fold cross-validation (SAS PROC GLMSELECT), entering age in years, with (date of evaluation − date of birth)/365.25 as a dependent measure to be predicted from the 26 performance measures (12 tests of accuracy and speed, 2 tests of only speed). The regression procedure adds variables to the model until additional variables do not contribute significantly to the predicted variance (R2) in age. Variables selected by the linear model were submitted to further examination of nonlinear components using a general additive model (SAS PROC GAM). Variables with significant nonlinear trends were entered into the linear model to evaluate whether their squared values added to the ability to predict age. These procedures were applied for the entire set of scores and then separately for each domain (executive, episodic memory, complex cognition, social cognition, and sensorimotor speed), entering all the scores from that domain. Regressions were run separately for males and females, and weights were based on the TD sample. As an outcome of these procedures, we calculated the predicted neurocognitive age across domains and for each domain separately. As the regression line was not as steep as the identity line, the predicted age was adjusted to that of the average for the TD group to facilitate interpretability.
The sample size afforded grouping by age in years (eg, 8 years, 9 years, 10 years) except for the group of 21-year-olds, which had fewer than 100 TD participants. We therefore combined the group of 21-year-olds with the 20-year-olds to form a group older than 20 years. Age group, sex, and diagnosis (TD, PS, PL, OP) effects were evaluated as between-group factors in a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) (SAS PROC GLM), with domain as a repeated-measures factor and race (white, other), parental education, and WRAT standard scores as covariates. To examine severity effects, PS and PL were contrasted in a MANCOVA. To address specificity, we used MANCOVAs to compare PS participants with other psychiatric symptoms vs those without and to compare the OP participants with TD participants and with the PS groups.
Prediction of Chronological Age From Performance Across Neurocognitive Domains
The GLMSELECT obtained a model that predicted about 63% of the variance in age. Adjusted R2 was 0.635 using 16 of the 26 scores in males and 0.629 using 13 scores in females (Table 2). Follow-up general additive model procedures did identify several measures with significant quadratic components, but when entered into the model, they did not improve the R2 appreciably. For simplicity in clinical contexts, we retained the initial model (without squared variables) as it was more easily interpretable for diagnosis and intervention. The correlations between chronological and predicted cognitive age were 0.798, 0.780, 0.705, and 0.669 for the TD, OP, PL, and PS groups, respectively.
The age group × sex × diagnosis (TD, OP, PS, PL) MANCOVA on predicted age showed the expected main effects for age group (F12,5193 = 685.22; P < .001), with higher predicted age associated with higher chronological age. The main effect of diagnosis was significant (F3,5193 = 11.37; P < .001), indicating that the groups differed on predicted age. The age group × diagnosis interaction was also significant (F36,5193 = 1.87; P = .001), indicating different age-related effects among groups. Covariate effects were also significant (F1,5193 = 20.08, 639.74, and 8.70 for parental education, WRAT scores, and race, respectively; P < .001). The contrast of PS vs TD showed a main effect of diagnosis (F1,3312 = 30.86; P < .001) and age group (F12,3312 = 433.99; P < .001) as well as a diagnosis × age group interaction (F12,3312 = 2.27; P = .007). Parental education and WRAT scores were again significant covariates (F1,3312 = 12.99, P < .001; and F1,3312 = 390.32, P < .001, respectively), while race was marginal (F1,3312 = 3.68, P = .06). Contrasting the OP group with the TD group showed no effect of diagnosis on predicted age (F1,3335 = 0.19; P = .66). Contrasting PS with OP showed a main effect of diagnosis (F1,2792 = 23.61; P < .001) and age group (F12,2792 = 368.76; P < .001) as well as a diagnosis × age group interaction (F12,2792 = 1.94; P = .03). The means for males and females with PS relative to TD are shown in Figure 1A, and effect sizes for contrasting PS with TD at each age group are summarized in Table 3.
The MANCOVAs contrasting PS with TD on predicted age for the 5 specific domains yielded significant interactions of domain. These included (significance by the Hotelling-Lawley trace) domain × diagnosis × sex (F4,4177 = 2.43; P = .046), indicating that the decrement in predicted age for PS differed by sex. A 3-way interaction of domain × sex × age group (F48,12 272 = 1.71; P = .002) indicated that the sex difference in domain-predicted age was age group related, and a 4-way interaction of domain × sex × age group × diagnosis (F48,12 272 = 1.57; P = .007) indicated that these effects differed between the TD and PS groups. As can be seen in Figure 1B-F, PS males showed an early decrement in memory, complex cognition, and social cognition, which remained stable across the age cohorts with some amelioration for complex cognition. The PS females showed minimal lag in memory across all age groups, while for complex cognition the lag appeared at a later age group but was more persistent.
Contrasting PS and PL Groups
The MANCOVA contrasting PS with PL groups on predicted age showed main effects for severity group (F1,2261 = 15.07; P < .001), with higher predicted age for the PL group compared with the PS group. The means are shown in Figure 2A. The MANCOVA on the domain-related age predictions additionally showed a severity group × domain interaction (F4,2258 = 2.46; P = .04), with lower predicted age for the PS group more pronounced for complex cognition and social cognition domains (Figure 2B-F).
Contrasting PS With and Without Other Psychiatric Comorbidity
The MANCOVA contrasting within the PS cohort those with other psychiatric comorbidities (n = 855) vs those without (n = 568) showed no main effect of group on predicted age (F1,1397 = 1.29; P = .26). It also did not show a group × age group interaction (F2,1397 = 0.57; P = .87).
Prodromal studies in help-seeking youths do not provide data on cognition earlier in development. Cohort studies of adults with schizophrenia document cognitive deficits on testing during school age but limited contemporaneous data on psychotic features. In this largest population-based study to date evaluating psychotic symptoms and neurocognition, 15.5% of the sample endorses significant psychotic symptoms and 9.8% have milder symptoms, comparable to estimates from a meta-analysis in youths.11 The growth charts show that individuals who report psychotic symptoms are neurocognitively behind chronological age compared with TD youths. Furthermore, those with more significant symptoms show greater developmental lag than those with milder symptoms not meeting prodromal criteria. The growth charts suggest that the delay associated with endorsement of psychotic symptoms is across domains, ranges between 6 and 18 months, and is present already at age 8 years. After age 16 years, the departure from the normal growth curve becomes wider. These findings augment the literature documenting cognitive deficits in school-aged children eventually diagnosed as having psychosis.11 Our study demonstrates that both cognitive delay and psychotic symptoms co-occur and are detectable at an early age.
The developmental delay is more pronounced in some cognitive domains than others, with effect sizes ranging from small to medium. The New Zealand longitudinal cohort suggests that children who later develop schizophrenia already have deficits in verbal reasoning in childhood and deficits later evolve in working memory, attention, and processing speed.34 Our cross-sectional data are consistent as complex cognition, including verbal reasoning, already lags in the psychosis groups at age 8 years and remains pronounced. We also documented a lag in social cognition already evident at that age, not evaluated in previous studies.
The lag is not constant across the age span, narrowing at age 12 years and again at age 16 years. In a cross-sectional study, these could reflect cohort effects (eg, placement of severe cases in special programs). However, the effects are consistent for males and females and across severity groups. They could reflect resilience or other factors. Only longitudinal studies can resolve such issues, and the age bands 11 to 13 years and 15 to 17 years seem especially important for longitudinal focus.35,36
Notably, working memory and face memory do not contribute to predicting chronological age and show less age-related improvement than other domains,26 having a protracted developmental course. Sex differences in neurocognitive gap were domain specific, with PS females showing earlier and greater lag in complex and social cognition relative to their TD counterparts. Females with schizophrenia have a later onset and less severe symptoms, reflecting less vulnerability; perhaps greater neurocognitive dysfunction is needed for psychosis to occur.37-40
While our ascertainment procedures differ from current clinical high-risk studies that recruit help seekers, we nonetheless capture an at-risk sample. The WRAT scores and parental education are lower in the psychosis groups, consistent with clinical high-risk samples and population-based studies of psychosis in adults.41,42 The current ascertainment strategy captures a broader continuum of psychotic experiences related to neurocognition in youths who are not help seekers. Importantly, a major feature of psychosis is limited insight and refrain from help seeking.43,44
While the focus of this article is on psychosis, the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort data set enabled examination of psychiatric comorbidities45 and specificity. We found that participants with other psychiatric disorders did not show a neurocognitive delay. Within PS, those who had other psychiatric disorders did not differ from those without such comorbidity. Furthermore, medical comorbidity could be excluded through the interview and electronic medical records. Thus, the neurocognitive delay seems firmly related to the presence and severity of psychotic symptoms.
An obvious limitation of this study is its cross-sectional nature, which precludes separating progression from cohort effects. However, participants have consented to be recontacted and we are following up with comprehensive assessment and neuroimaging in 300 PS and 200 TD individuals. Because it is likely that not all PS will develop into debilitating or distressing psychosis,12 follow-up will provide the opportunity to examine risk and protective factors. Another limitation is the lack of validation of the GOASSESS instrument. We have taken care to ensure reliable administration by highly trained and monitored assessors, and the majority of GOASSESS was derived from the K-SADS-PL27 using similar probes and item wording. GOASSESS DSM-IV-TR disorder indices and National Comorbidity Survey–Adolescent Supplement DSM-IV-TR diagnoses exhibited very good concordance in the National Comorbidity Survey–Adolescent Supplement sample,46 providing support for their validity. However, the National Comorbidity Survey–Adolescent Supplement sample did not assess psychosis. The ratings of psychotic symptoms included in GOASSESS incorporate established instruments, which have shown predictive validity of clinical high-risk status.31,32 Initial evaluation of our follow-up data, which included blinded consensus review, indicates acceptable sensitivity and specificity.47 The ascertainment through pediatric clinics may affect generalizability, and we have taken steps to address this potential limitation. Electronic medical records were first screened for significant disorders that may affect performance. Thus, we did not include children with physical or mental conditions that compromise performance. Indeed, all participants completed the CNB and were fully engaged. The WRAT scores also attest to the average cognitive abilities of the normative sample. We set a relatively low bar for inclusion to capture a diverse sample and further limited the analysis to children who do not have significant medical conditions. Another limitation is reliance on a 1-hour battery to assess neurocognitive performance. Although this duration is vastly shorter than that required by traditional batteries, it is still too long for routine screening. We are applying psychometric methods including item response theory to improve efficiency of testing and facilitate alternative methods of administration.
This study illustrates the utility of neurocognitive age in efforts to understand the developmental unfolding of psychosis. We demonstrated that individuals who endorse psychotic symptoms have a neurocognitive lag already at age 8 years, when they are almost a year behind. While this gap widens and narrows during the developmental span, its existence concomitant with psychotic symptoms at an early age should facilitate early detection and provide information necessary for designing intervention strategies. Although as a group PS individuals show greater lag in complex cognition than other domains, including executive functions, the lag pattern may differ for individuals and can form the basis for designing tailored intervention approaches. Furthermore, neurocognitive age provides a metric that can be used to examine other indices of maturation such as “brain age.” Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies are applying the brain age concept to provide an estimate of neurobiological parameters in relation to chronological age during both childhood and senescence.48,49 Integration of brain measures with neurocognitive parameters provides phenotypes for genomics to achieve early and individualized prognostic profiles informing intervention.
Corresponding Author: Ruben C. Gur, PhD, Brain Behavior Laboratory, 10 Gates, 3400 Spruce, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104 (gur@upenn.edu).
Submitted for Publication: May 21, 2013; final revision received October 8, 2013; accepted October 16, 2013.
Published Online: February 5, 2014. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4190.
Author Contributions: Dr R. C. Gur had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Satterthwaite, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Acquisition of data: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Ruparel, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Analysis and interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Satterthwaite, Ruparel, R. E. Gur.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Satterthwaite, Bilker, Moore, Savitt, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Statistical analysis: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Ruparel, Bilker, Moore, Savitt.
Obtained funding: R. C. Gur, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Administrative, technical, and material support: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Ruparel, Savitt, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Study supervision: R. C. Gur, Calkins, Satterthwaite, Hakonarson, R. E. Gur.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by RC2 grants MH089983 and MH089924 from the National Institutes of Health. Dr Calkins was supported by grant K08MH79364 from the National Institutes of Health.
Role of the Sponsor: The funding organization had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: Kathleen Merikangas, PhD, and Marcy Burstein, PhD, Genetic Epidemiology Research Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health, assisted in development of GOASSESS; they received no compensation from the funding sponsor. We thank participants and families; staff of the Brain Behavior Laboratory, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, for their contribution to data generation; study assessors and recruiters for their invaluable contributions to data collection; and staff from the Center of Applied Genomics, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, for the genomics interphase with brain behavior measures.
1.Fusar-Poli
P, Borgwardt
S, Bechdolf
A,
et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review.
JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(1):107-120.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.McGlashan
T, Walsh
B, Woods
S. The Psychosis-Risk Syndrome: Handbook for Diagnosis and Follow-up. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010.
3.Fusar-Poli
P, Bonoldi
I, Yung
AR,
et al. Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(3):220-229.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Ruhrmann
S, Schultze-Lutter
F, Salokangas
RK,
et al. Prediction of psychosis in adolescents and young adults at high risk: results from the prospective European Prediction of Psychosis Study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):241-251.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Mohamed
S, Rosenheck
R, Swartz
M, Stroup
S, Lieberman
JA, Keefe
RS. Relationship of cognition and psychopathology to functional impairment in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(8):978-987.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Nuechterlein
KH, Subotnik
KL, Ventura
J, Green
MF, Gretchen-Doorly
D, Asarnow
RF. The puzzle of schizophrenia: tracking the core role of cognitive deficits.
Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24(2):529-536.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Seidman
LJ, Giuliano
AJ, Meyer
EC,
et al; North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) Group. Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):578-588.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Dickson
H, Laurens
KR, Cullen
AE, Hodgins
S. Meta-analyses of cognitive and motor function in youth aged 16 years and younger who subsequently develop schizophrenia.
Psychol Med. 2012;42(4):743-755.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Fusar-Poli
P, Deste
G, Smieskova
R,
et al. Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(6):562-571.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Giuliano
AJ, Li
H, Mesholam-Gately
RI, Sorenson
SM, Woodberry
KA, Seidman
LJ. Neurocognition in the psychosis risk syndrome: a quantitative and qualitative review.
Curr Pharm Des. 2012;18(4):399-415.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Kelleher
I, Connor
D, Clarke
MC, Devlin
N, Harley
M, Cannon
M. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies.
Psychol Med. 2012;42(9):1857-1863.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Schimmelmann
BG, Walger
P, Schultze-Lutter
F. The significance of at-risk symptoms for psychosis in children and adolescents.
Can J Psychiatry. 2013;58(1):32-40.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Laurens
KR, Hobbs
MJ, Sunderland
M, Green
MJ, Mould
GL. Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of 8000 children aged 9 to 11 years: an item response theory analysis.
Psychol Med. 2012;42(7):1495-1506.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Kelleher
I, Murtagh
A, Molloy
C,
et al. Identification and characterization of prodromal risk syndromes in young adolescents in the community: a population-based clinical interview study.
Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(2):239-246.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Kelleher
I, Murtagh
A, Clarke
MC, Murphy
J, Rawdon
C, Cannon
M. Neurocognitive performance of a community-based sample of young people at putative ultra high risk for psychosis: support for the processing speed hypothesis.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013;18(1-2):9-25.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Conklin
HM, Luciana
M, Hooper
CJ, Yarger
RS. Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development.
Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31(1):103-128.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Astle
DE, Scerif
G. Using developmental cognitive neuroscience to study behavioral and attentional control.
Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(2):107-118.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Butterworth
B, Kovas
Y. Understanding neurocognitive developmental disorders can improve education for all.
Science. 2013;340(6130):300-305.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Gur
RC, Richard
J, Hughett
P,
et al. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct validation.
J Neurosci Methods. 2010;187(2):254-262.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Calkins
ME, Ray
A, Gur
RC,
et al. Sex differences in familiality effects on neurocognitive performance in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(10):976-984.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Greenwood
TA, Swerdlow
NR, Gur
RE,
et al. Genome-wide linkage analyses of 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(5):521-532.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Roalf
DR, Gur
RC, Almasy
L,
et al. Neurocognitive performance stability in a multiplex multigenerational study of schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(5):1008-1017.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Calkins
ME, Tepper
P, Gur
RC,
et al. Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS): evidence for impairment and heritability of neurocognitive functioning in families of schizophrenia patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(4):459-472.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Greenwood
TA, Lazzeroni
LC, Murray
SS,
et al. Analysis of 94 candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(9):930-946.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Gur
RE, Nimgaonkar
VL, Almasy
L,
et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(5):813-819.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Gur
RC, Richard
J, Calkins
ME,
et al. Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21.
Neuropsychology. 2012;26(2):251-265.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Kaufman
J, Birmaher
B, Brent
DA,
et al. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(7):980-988.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Maxwell
ME. Manual for the FIGS. Bethesda, MD: Clinical Neurogenetics Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health; 1996.
29.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
30.Han
C, McGue
MK, Iacono
WG. Lifetime tobacco, alcohol and other substance use in adolescent Minnesota twins: univariate and multivariate behavioral genetic analyses.
Addiction. 1999;94(7):981-993.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Kobayashi
H, Nemoto
T, Koshikawa
H,
et al. A self-reported instrument for prodromal symptoms of psychosis: testing the clinical validity of the PRIME Screen–Revised (PS-R) in a Japanese population.
Schizophr Res. 2008;106(2-3):356-362.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Miller
TJ, Cicchetti
D, Markovich
PJ,
et al. Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes, Version 4.0. New Haven, CT: PRIME Psychosis Prodrome Research Clinic, Yale School of Medicine; 2003.
33.Wilkinson
GS, Robertson
GJ. Wide Range Achievement Test, Fourth Edition, Introductory Kit. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2006.
34.Reichenberg
A, Caspi
A, Harrington
H,
et al. Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study.
Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(2):160-169.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Linscott
RJ, van Os
J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders.
Psychol Med. 2013;43(6):1133-1149.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Cannon
M, Caspi
A, Moffitt
TE,
et al. Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(5):449-456.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Viveros
MP, Mendrek
A, Paus
T,
et al. A comparative, developmental, and clinical perspective of neurobehavioral sexual dimorphisms.
Front Neurosci. 2012;6:84.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Häfner
H, an der Heiden
W, Behrens
S,
et al. Causes and consequences of the gender difference in age at onset of schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1998;24(1):99-113.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Goldstein
JM, Seidman
LJ, O’Brien
LM,
et al. Impact of normal sexual dimorphisms on sex differences in structural brain abnormalities in schizophrenia assessed by magnetic resonance imaging.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(2):154-164.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Gur
RE, Kohler
C, Turetsky
BI,
et al. A sexually dimorphic ratio of orbitofrontal to amygdala volume is altered in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(5):512-517.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Krabbendam
L, van Os
J. Schizophrenia and urbanicity: a major environmental influence—conditional on genetic risk.
Schizophr Bull. 2005;31(4):795-799.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Vassos
E, Pedersen
CB, Murray
RM, Collier
DA, Lewis
CM. Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(6):1118-1123.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Amador
XF, Flaum
M, Andreasen
NC,
et al. Awareness of illness in schizophrenia and schizoaffective and mood disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(10):826-836.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Keshavan
MS, Rabinowitz
J, DeSmedt
G, Harvey
PD, Schooler
N. Correlates of insight in first episode psychosis.
Schizophr Res. 2004;70(2-3):187-194.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.van Os
J, Linscott
RJ, Myin-Germeys
I, Delespaul
P, Krabbendam
L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder.
Psychol Med. 2009;39(2):179-195.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Merikangas
KR, He
JP, Burstein
M,
et al. Service utilization for lifetime mental disorders in US adolescents: results of the National Comorbidity Survey–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(1):32-45.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Calkins ME, Wolf D, Satterthwaite T, et al. Early identification of children and adolescents at-risk for psychosis in the community: findings from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Paper presented at: 14th International Congress on Schizophrenia Research; April 24, 2013; Orlando, FL.
49.Dosenbach
NU, Nardos
B, Cohen
AL,
et al. Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI.
Science. 2010;329(5997):1358-1361.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref