Importance
Genetic factors contribute to risk for bipolar disorder (BP), but its pathogenesis remains poorly understood. A focus on measuring multisystem quantitative traits that may be components of BP psychopathology may enable genetic dissection of this complex disorder, and investigation of extended pedigrees from genetically isolated populations may facilitate the detection of specific genetic variants that affect BP as well as its component phenotypes.
Objective
To identify quantitative neurocognitive, temperament-related, and neuroanatomical phenotypes that appear heritable and associated with severe BP (bipolar I disorder [BP-I]) and therefore suitable for genetic linkage and association studies aimed at identifying variants contributing to BP-I risk.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Multigenerational pedigree study in 2 closely related, genetically isolated populations: the Central Valley of Costa Rica and Antioquia, Colombia. A total of 738 individuals, all from Central Valley of Costa Rica and Antioquia pedigrees, participated; among them, 181 have BP-I.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Familial aggregation (heritability) and association with BP-I of 169 quantitative neurocognitive, temperament, magnetic resonance imaging, and diffusion tensor imaging phenotypes.
Results
Of 169 phenotypes investigated, 119 (70%) were significantly heritable and 51 (30%) were associated with BP-I. About one-quarter of the phenotypes, including measures from each phenotype domain, were both heritable and associated with BP-I. Neuroimaging phenotypes, particularly cortical thickness in prefrontal and temporal regions and volume of the corpus callosum, represented the most promising candidate traits for genetic mapping related to BP based on strong heritability and association with disease. Analyses of phenotypic and genetic covariation identified substantial correlations among the traits, at least some of which share a common underlying genetic architecture.
Conclusions and Relevance
To our knowledge, this is the most extensive investigation of BP-relevant component phenotypes to date. Our results identify brain and behavioral quantitative traits that appear to be genetically influenced and show a pattern of BP-I association within families that is consistent with expectations from case-control studies. Together, these phenotypes provide a basis for identifying loci contributing to BP-I risk and for genetic dissection of the disorder.
Bipolar disorder (BP) encompasses a broad range of phenotypic features. However, most research into its etiology has focused on the overall syndrome1-6 rather than on its components. Although genome-wide association studies have identified the first replicated loci contributing to BP susceptibility,3-6 the small relative risk attributed to these loci may reflect the complex genetic nature of the disorder. This possibility motivates efforts to identify heritable BP-associated quantitative traits for which the genetic basis is simpler and for which higher-impact variants may be detected.7-12
We describe our investigation, in 26 pedigrees selected for multiple cases of severe BP (bipolar I disorder [BP-I]), of quantitative traits hypothesized to represent components of the biology underlying BP. Previous studies of these measures demonstrated association with BP, deficits in euthymic individuals with BP, and values in family members without BP that are intermediate between those of their relatives with BP and control participants. These phenotypes assay temperament,13-15 perceptual creativity,16-18 neurocognitive function,19-21 and neuroanatomy (via structural magnetic resonance imaging [MRI] and diffusion tensor imaging [DTI]).22-24 We also measured sleep, activity, and circadian rhythms, analyses of which are ongoing and will be reported separately.
Previously described pedigrees, including many of those evaluated here,25-28 show BP segregation patterns suggesting the transmission of high-impact risk alleles. However, linkage studies of such pedigrees have yielded equivocal results, presumably because BP is genetically complex even within these families.3 The feasibility of identifying rare, high-impact variants through next-generation sequencing has stimulated renewed interest in pedigree studies; however, even with this technology, the etiological complexity of BP hinders the identification of risk variants. We hypothesize that BP results from the confluence of multiple etiological processes, each of which alone may be simpler to unravel. Investigation of quantitative component phenotypes in pedigrees from population isolates such as the genetically related isolates of the Central Valley of Costa Rica (CVCR) and Antioquia, Colombia (ANT),29-31 from which we recruited the pedigrees investigated herein, may lead to a better understanding of the heritable components of the disorder and at the same time simplify the search for specific genetic risk factors.
We report results from evaluations of the most extensive set of putative BP component phenotypes yet assessed within any study sample. For each measure, we describe its degree of familial aggregation (an indicator of heritability [h2]) and of association with BP-I. These results suggest multiple phenotypes for genetic investigations of BP-I across the domains of temperament, neurocognition, and neuroanatomy.
We investigated pedigrees from ANT (11) and CVCR (15), ascertained in previous genetic studies25-28,32-36 through hospitals and clinics in each country, using genealogic information to extend each pedigree. To prioritize pedigree branches for quantitative phenotyping, we recruited nuclear families including at least 1 member with known BP-I (based on the Diagnostic Interview for Genetics Studies37,38 and/or extensive medical records), available parents, and at least 2 siblings without BP-I (eAppendix 1 in Supplement). Families varied considerably in size (12-355 members; mean, 55 members) and in the number of individuals phenotyped in this study (3-177 individuals; mean, 29 individuals) (Table 1). Written informed consent was obtained from each participant. Institutional review boards at participating institutions approved all study procedures.
To establish DSM-IV diagnoses, we used a best-estimate process modified from previous procedures33 (eAppendix 1 in Supplement) and including diagnostic interviews using Spanish versions of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview39 and the Diagnostic Interview for Genetics Studies. Individuals designated as having BP-I had a best-estimate diagnosis of BP-I, unipolar mania, or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type, as in previous studies.27,33,40 The Young Mania Rating Scale41 and the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale42 were administered at the time of assessment and identified individuals with significant mood symptoms (Young Mania Rating Scale Score >14 or Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score >14), whom we excluded from analyses of temperament and neurocognitive measures.
Temperament and Neurocognitive Assessment
Temperament and neurocognitive measures, assessed in 738 subjects, had previously demonstrated heritability and association to BP13-16,22-24 (Table 2). The temperament battery, 15 measures generated from 7 instruments (eAppendix 1 in Supplement), included multiple dimensions categorized into 4 subdomains: affective temperament, impulsivity/risk taking, perceptual creativity, and delusion proneness (Table 2). The neurocognitive battery (eAppendix 1 in Supplement) included a computerized neuropsychological evaluation51 and paper-and-pencil measures of verbal abilities, inhibitory control,55 and declarative memory.52
We acquired T1-weighted structural neuroimages on 1.5-T scanners from 527 subjects (285 from CVCR and 242 from ANT) (eAppendix 1 in Supplement), implementing protocols for acquisition of DTIs in ANT only. We used Freesurfer software,57,58 with manual inspection of intermediate steps in the processing stream to correct common errors, to generate 96 structural MRI phenotypes, including measures of volume, surface area, and cortical thickness (Table 3, eTable 1 in Supplement).61,62
We determined DTI phenotypes (eAppendix 1 in Supplement) with Functional MRI of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library software59,60 using the Johns Hopkins University probabilistic tractography atlas63 to determine and customize regions of interest, which we limited to tracts previously associated with BP.64-66 In total, we generated 18 DTI phenotypes across 3 categories: fractional anisotropy, indicating the degree of anisotropy; axial diffusivity, or diffusivity along the major axis of diffusion; and radial diffusivity, an average of the diffusivities along the 2 minor axes67-70 (Table 3, eTable 1 in Supplement).
We assessed familial aggregation of traits using SOLAR version 6.3.6 software,71 which implements a variance component method to estimate the proportion of phenotypic variance due to additive genetic factors (narrow-sense heritability). This model partitions total variability into polygenic and environmental components. The environmental component is unique to individuals, while the polygenic component is shared between individuals as a function of their pedigree kinship. If the variance in phenotype Y due to the polygenic component is designated as σg2 and the environmental component as σe2, then in this model Var(Y) = σg2 + σe2, and the covariance between phenotype values of individuals i and j is Cov(Yi, Yj) = 2(φij)(σg2), where φij is the kinship between individuals i and j.
Variance components analysis is sensitive to outliers and nonnormal trait distributions. To guard against potential statistical artifacts induced by skewed distributions, we used, prior to analysis, a rank-based procedure72 to inverse normal transform all phenotypes. This transformation, implemented within SOLAR, is standard in variance component analyses as it does not induce correlations between relatives or lead to inflated estimates of heritability.73
We regressed all phenotypes on 3 covariates (sex, age, and country). Additional covariates included years of education (temperament and neurocognitive measures), body weight (T1-weighted and DTI variables), intracranial volume (volume measurements from T1-weighted images), and total cortical surface area (regional surface area measures). We implemented regressions in SOLAR with pedigree structures using residuals from these models in all further analyses.
We tested for difference in trait means between individuals with and without a diagnosis of BP-I (BP-I association analyses), using SOLAR to account for dependencies among relatives. We controlled the family-wise error rate at the 0.05 level, using a Bonferroni-corrected threshold for each test (heritability and BP-I association; P < 2.96 × 10−4). We used published evidence to assign each trait an expected a priori direction of change, designating them as BP-I associated only if the difference was in the a priori assigned direction, therefore using a 1-tailed test (eTable 1 in Supplement).
We estimated phenotypic correlations for all trait pairs. Genetic correlations were estimated for all pairs in which both traits were significantly heritable using SOLAR.74 Graphs of the estimated correlation structures used methods described in eAppendix 1 in the Supplement.
Table 1 shows summary statistics for the sample by family; eTable 2 in the Supplement provides additional clinical characterization of the 181 participants who met best-estimate criteria for BP-I. We excluded 5 individuals with elevated Young Mania Rating Scale or Hamilton Depression Rating Scale scores from analyses of neurocognitive and temperament data, and we excluded 5 additional individuals from BP-I association analyses (but not from heritability analyses) because a BP-I diagnosis could be neither confirmed nor excluded.
Heritability and Association With BP-I
Of the 169 traits examined, 119 (70%) were significantly heritable, 51 (30%) were significantly associated with BP-I, and 38 (22%) were both heritable and associated with BP-I (Figure 1, eTable 1 in Supplement). These results were robust with respect to phenotype variations across pedigrees and countries (data not shown) and to outliers (eAppendix 2 and eFigure in Supplement); for secondary analyses of the effects of medications and duration of illness on trait values, see eAppendix 3 in the Supplement. Results within each domain are described here.
Six of the 15 temperament measures demonstrated significant heritability, although overall this domain showed the lowest estimates of additive genetic influence (h2 of approximately 0.18-0.30). In contrast, 3 temperament traits displayed the strongest BP-I associations of all 169 measures: Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego cyclothymia scale, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, and Peters et al Delusions Inventory. Delusion proneness (Peters et al Delusions Inventory) and perceptual creativity (Barron-Welsh Art Scale dislike subscale) were both heritable and associated with BP-I, while risk-taking propensity (Balloon Analogue Risk Task) was neither heritable nor associated with BP-I.
Some measures from all domains assessed showed significant heritability and BP-I associations. Most measures of processing speed, long-term memory, and verbal fluency were significantly heritable (13 of 19); within this heritable subset, most were associated with BP-I (9 of 13). Within working memory assessments, verbal but not spatial tasks showed evidence of heritability, and participants with BP-I showed significant impairment on measures of sustained attention (Identical Pairs Continuous Performance Test), spatial working memory (Spatial Capacity Delayed Response Test), and verbal working memory tasks (letter-number sequencing). Measures of inhibitory control (Stroop Color-Word Interference Test and Stop Signal Task) showed evidence for impairment in participants with BP-I; among these measures, the Stroop measures (Stroop Color-Word Interference Test trials, time, and number of errors) were also heritable. Nonverbal abstract reasoning measures (Abstraction, Inhibition, and Working Memory Task, Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, matrix reasoning) were neither significantly heritable nor associated with BP-I.
Most neuroimaging phenotypes (approximately 82%) were significantly heritable, and a substantial number of these measures were significantly associated with BP-I. Several global measures differed between participants with BP-I and their relatives without BP-I (decreased total cerebral gray and white matter and cerebellar volumes, with corresponding increases in third-ventricle volume). Localized reductions were also observed in several structures (Figure 2), including thalamus and ventral diencephalon (while amygdala and hippocampus showed a similar trend). The T1-weighted sequences also provided evidence for BP-I–related changes in white matter; participants with BP-I showed overall volumetric decreases in the corpus callosum and 4 of the 5 corpus callosum subdivisions.
Compared with relatives without BP-I, participants with BP-I displayed widespread reduction of cortical thickness in heteromodal association regions in most of the prefrontal and temporal cortex, including the superior temporal gyrus, fusiform, and lingual regions (Figure 2B). Most lateral prefrontal cortex regions, including all subregions of the inferior frontal gyrus, were significantly thinner in participants with BP-I. In contrast, the medial orbitofrontal region was neither heritable nor associated with BP-I. Another exception to the overall pattern of findings was the superior frontal gyrus, which showed BP-I–associated gray matter reduction but was not significantly heritable. Most measures of regional surface area were heritable but were not significantly associated with BP-I.
Evaluation of Between-Trait Phenotypic and Genetic Correlations
Using false discovery rate methods, we determined thresholds (t) for rejecting the null hypothesis of correlation = 0; t = 2.59 SEs from 0 for phenotypic correlations (ρp) and 2.86 SEs from 0 for genetic correlations (ρg). About 19% of trait pairs (2024 of 10 585) exceeded t for ρp and 8% of heritable pairs (407 of 5050) exceeded t for ρg. Schematic representations (eAppendix 1 in Supplement) of the networks of phenotypic and genetic correlations (Figure 3) demonstrate the clustering of phenotypes by domain, showing no clear separation between heritable and nonheritable traits (circles and squares, respectively). Similarly, BP-I–associated traits showed no distinct clustering (nodes with a red border). The network structure of the genetic correlations was sparser than, but qualitatively similar to, that of phenotypic correlations. Traits mainly clustered within phenotypic domains, but some genetic correlations across domains were observed, such as Stroop errors with inferior parietal surface area (Figure 3B; nodes 34 and 87).
Through the most comprehensive evaluation to date of BP component phenotypes, we delineated measures that may help elucidate the genetic contribution to BP-I risk. Gauging the potential informativeness of traits based on their heritability and association with BP-I, we can divide them into 4 groups.
Measures that demonstrate both heritability and association with BP-I (group 1) are the most promising phenotypes for identifying loci contributing to disease risk, as shown for other neuropsychiatric disorders.75 Analyses at loci linked to and/or associated with both BP-I and a group 1 phenotype will suggest the degree of BP-I genetic risk directly attributable to that measure; some loci may, of course, contribute to trait variability but not to disease risk.
All domains that we assessed include group 1 phenotypes. Some phenotypes in this group, such as delusion proneness,76 appear broadly characteristic of the major psychoses. Others, such as perceptual creativity, appear specific to BP predisposition77-79; individuals diagnosed as having BP are overrepresented in creative occupations compared with individuals diagnosed as having other psychiatric disorders or with the general population.78,79 Many individuals with BP consider heightened creativity a positive aspect of their condition,80 which should fuel efforts to elucidate the mechanisms underlying this association.
Among the neurocognitive processes in group 1, the BP-I associations reflect impairments in processing speed, verbal learning and memory, category fluency, and inhibitory control, mirroring findings from previous BP and schizophrenia case-control, family, and pedigree studies.20,21,51,81-85 Such phenotypes could contribute to the shared risk between these disorders suggested by recent genome-wide association studies.86
Group 1 neuroimaging measures provide the first confirmation in families of BP-related anatomical variations previously identified through case-control studies.87-92 Although generally in accord with structural MRI findings from prior studies, our results identified larger zones of BP-I–associated gray matter reduction, which may reflect the greater size and reduced ethnic heterogeneity of the sample. We identified volume reduction and cortical thinning in 2 prefrontal systems implicated in BP pathogenesis: (1) a corticocognitive network anchored in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, including most subdivisions of the inferior frontal gyrus, which plays a role in attention, working memory, and inhibitory control and shows attenuated activation in functional MRI studies of individuals with BP93-98; and (2) a ventral-limbic system implicated in emotional reactivity, involving the hippocampus, amygdala, and orbitofrontal cortex.87,89-91 Further, the reduced corpus callosum volume aligns with twin studies suggesting genetically influenced alterations of this structure in BP.99,100 Gray matter reduction in temporal structures, including the superior temporal, lingual, and fusiform gyri, is noteworthy given the involvement of these structures in facial emotion identification, a process impaired in individuals with BP and adolescents at high risk.101-105
Numerous phenotypes, including most of the neuroimaging measures, were heritable but not associated with BP-I (group 2). The lack of difference in cortical surface area between participants with BP-I and their relatives without BP-I supports previous evidence dissociating this measure from cortical thickness abnormalities characteristic of the disorder.92 Similarly, neurocognitive traits in this category have consistently demonstrated heritability in twin and family samples84,106-113 but have shown inconsistent association with BP-I.20,21,81,114
A third set of phenotypes showed BP-I association but were not heritable (group 3), suggesting they may be predominantly influenced by environmental or disease-specific factors. Previous studies have proposed that temperament is a key contributor to BP genetic risk,115 but we found little evidence for heritability of several measures associated with emotional reactivity (cyclothymic, irritable, and depressive temperament, aggression, and impulsivity) that were elevated in our participants with BP-I.
Our results for neurocognitive traits are remarkably similar to those reported in the only previously published study of such traits in BP pedigrees,51 with 3 exceptions. First, we did not find significant heritability for face memory (which was impaired in participants with BP-I in both studies). Second, we observed significant impairment in participants with BP-I on measures of sustained attention and spatial working memory. As deficits in these domains may index psychotic symptoms, regardless of diagnosis,116 this discordance may reflect the larger percentage of patients in our sample with a lifetime history of psychosis. Finally, we found lower heritability for nonverbal abstract reasoning. As we report heritability estimates corrected for demographic variables, comparisons with the prior study are with its similarly corrected estimates.
We identified extensive correlation among measures within each phenotypic domain, including phenotype clusters consistently implicated in BP pathology. Some such clusters also showed evidence of shared genetic influence (eg, limbic regions with the pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus98). This analysis also suggests shared genetic influence among select measures across domains, eg, that between Stroop test performance and surface area MRI measures.
Our ascertainment strategy emphasized close family relationships, enhancing the power for quantitative genetic analyses; however, the shared genetic and environmental backgrounds of our participants would tend to make them more similar to each other compared with cases and independently ascertained controls and reduce power to identify phenotypic associations with BP-I. Two scenarios may explain group differences observed for some phenotypes: participants with BP-I may carry risk alleles with strong and/or nonadditive phenotypic effects, and/or they may have experienced different environmental exposures, either prior to illness onset or as a consequence of the disorder. As the ascertainment of the pedigrees themselves and of the specific individuals evaluated within them were nonrandom with respect to clinical diagnosis, our data are not suitable for assessing the genetic relationship between these phenotypes and BP-I.
Although prior evidence supported the selection of each measure that we evaluated, the use of alternative measures could have yielded discrepant outcomes. While such discrepancies may reflect incompatibilities in the theoretical underpinnings of different instruments (eg, for temperament scales), identification of genetic coassociations between BP-I and specific component measures will accelerate the standardization of phenotyping.
Our findings establish a core set of measures across multiple domains as component phenotypes for identifying the genetic basis of BP-I risk. Overall, the profile of brain and behavioral impairments in these pedigrees is similar to those identified previously in case-control samples. We therefore anticipate that while specific genetic variants contributing to these phenotypes and to BP-I risk may be distinct to the CVCR and ANT population isolates, they could suggest genes that also influence disease risk in other populations.
Corresponding Author: Carrie E. Bearden, PhD, Center for Neurobehavioral Genetics, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, 695 Charles E. Young Dr S, Room 3506, Los Angeles, CA 90095 (cbearden@mednet.ucla.edu).
Submitted for Publication: June 5, 2013; final revision received September 19, 2013; accepted October 16, 2013.
Published Online: February 12, 2014. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4100.
Author Contributions: Drs Freimer and Bearden had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: Service, Kremeyer, Abaryan, Ericson, Altshuler, Escobar, Ospina-Duque, Risch, Ruiz-Linares, Lopez-Jaramillo, Macaya, Reus, Sabatti, Freimer, Bearden.
Acquisition of data: C. Araya, X. Araya, Bejarano, Ramirez, Castrillón, Gomez-Franco, Lopez, G. Montoya, P. Montoya, Aldana, Teshiba, Luykx, Tishler, Bartzokis, Escobar, Ospina-Duque, Lopez-Jaramillo, Macaya, Molina, Reus, Freimer, Bearden.
Analysis and interpretation of data: Fears, Service, Castrillón, Abaryan, Al-Sharif, Ericson, Jalbrzikowski, Navarro, Glahn, Risch, Thompson, Cantor, Reus, Sabatti, Freimer, Bearden.
Drafting of the manuscript: Fears, Ramirez, Castrillón, Lopez, G. Montoya, P. Montoya, Teshiba, Al-Sharif, Ericson, Glahn, Risch, Lopez-Jaramillo, Molina, Sabatti, Freimer, Bearden.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Fears, Service, Kremeyer, C. Araya, X. Araya, Bejarano, Gomez-Franco, Aldana, Abaryan, Jalbrzikowski, Luykx, Navarro, Tishler, Altshuler, Bartzokis, Escobar, Glahn, Ospina-Duque, Ruiz-Linares, Thompson, Cantor, Lopez-Jaramillo, Macaya, Reus, Sabatti, Freimer, Bearden.
Statistical analysis: Fears, Service, Castrillón, Abaryan, Ericson, Jalbrzikowski, Navarro, Glahn, Risch, Cantor, Lopez-Jaramillo, Sabatti, Freimer.
Obtained funding: Altshuler, Lopez-Jaramillo, Reus, Freimer, Bearden.
Administrative, technical, and material support: Fears, Kremeyer, C. Araya, X. Araya, Bejarano, Ramirez, Castrillón, Gomez-Franco, Lopez, G. Montoya, P. Montoya, Aldana, Teshiba, Abaryan, Al-Sharif, Jalbrzikowski, Luykx, Tishler, Altshuler, Bartzokis, Escobar, Ospina-Duque, Thompson, Lopez-Jaramillo, Macaya, Molina, Freimer.
Study supervision: Abaryan, Bartzokis, Escobar, Ruiz-Linares, Lopez-Jaramillo, Macaya, Reus, Freimer, Bearden.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Altshuler has received advisory board honoraria from Sepracor, Takeda Pharmaceuticals North America, H. Lundbeck A/S, and Sunovion Pharmaceuticals and has been a consultant for Eli Lilly. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by grants R01MH075007, R01MH095454, P30NS062691 (Dr Freimer), K23MH074644-01 (Dr Bearden), K08MH086786 (Dr Fears), and R01HG006695 (Dr Sabatti) from the National Institutes of Health and by Colciencias and Codi–University of Antioquia (Dr Lopez-Jaramillo).
Role of the Sponsor: The funding organizations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Correction: This article was corrected on April 3, 2014, for an omission in Funding/Support and on May 18, 2016, for errors in data in the Abstract; Results and Discussion sections of the text; Figures 1, 2, and 3; and eTable 1 in the Supplement.
1.Goodwin
FK, Jamison
KR. Manic-Depressive Illness: Bipolar Disorders and Recurrent Depression. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007.
2.McGuffin
P, Rijsdijk
F, Andrew
M, Sham
P, Katz
R, Cardno
A. The heritability of bipolar affective disorder and the genetic relationship to unipolar depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(5):497-502.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Fears
SC, Mathews
CM, Freimer
NF. Genetic linkage analysis of psychiatric disorders. In: Kaplan and Sadock's Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2009:320-332.
4.Ferreira
MA, O’Donovan
MC, Meng
YA,
et al; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium. Collaborative genome-wide association analysis supports a role for
ANK3 and
CACNA1C in bipolar disorder.
Nat Genet. 2008;40(9):1056-1058.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Sklar
P, Smoller
JW, Fan
J,
et al. Whole-genome association study of bipolar disorder.
Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(6):558-569.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Sullivan
PF, Daly
MJ, O’Donovan
M. Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications.
Nat Rev Genet. 2012;13(8):537-551.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Bearden
CE, Freimer
NB. Endophenotypes for psychiatric disorders: ready for primetime?
Trends Genet. 2006;22(6):306-313.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Cannon
TD, Keller
MC. Endophenotypes in the genetic analyses of mental disorders.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2006;2:267-290.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Gottesman
II, Gould
TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.
Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636-645.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Preston
GA, Weinberger
DR. Intermediate phenotypes in schizophrenia: a selective review.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2005;7(2):165-179.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Akiskal
HS, Kilzieh
N, Maser
JD,
et al. The distinct temperament profiles of bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar patients.
J Affect Disord. 2006;92(1):19-33.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Karam
EG, Salamoun
MM, Yeretzian
JS,
et al. The role of anxious and hyperthymic temperaments in mental disorders: a national epidemiologic study.
World Psychiatry. 2010;9(2):103-110.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Vázquez
GH, Kahn
C, Schiavo
CE,
et al. Bipolar disorders and affective temperaments: a national family study testing the “endophenotype” and “subaffective” theses using the TEMPS-A Buenos Aires.
J Affect Disord. 2008;108(1-2):25-32.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Srivastava
S, Childers
ME, Baek
JH,
et al. Toward interaction of affective and cognitive contributors to creativity in bipolar disorders: a controlled study.
J Affect Disord. 2010;125(1-3):27-34.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Santosa
CM, Strong
CM, Nowakowska
C, Wang
PW, Rennicke
CM, Ketter
TA. Enhanced creativity in bipolar disorder patients: a controlled study.
J Affect Disord. 2007;100(1-3):31-39.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Glahn
DC, Bearden
CE, Niendam
TA, Escamilla
MA. The feasibility of neuropsychological endophenotypes in the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder.
Bipolar Disord. 2004;6(3):171-182.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Arts
B, Jabben
N, Krabbendam
L, van Os
J. Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives.
Psychol Med. 2008;38(6):771-785.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Bora
E, Yucel
M, Pantelis
C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives.
J Affect Disord. 2009;113(1-2):1-20.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Fusar-Poli
P, Howes
O, Bechdolf
A, Borgwardt
S. Mapping vulnerability to bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies.
J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37(3):170-184.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Foland-Ross
LC, Thompson
PM, Sugar
CA,
et al. Investigation of cortical thickness abnormalities in lithium-free adults with bipolar I disorder using cortical pattern matching.
Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(5):530-539.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.McInnes
LA, Escamilla
MA, Service
SK,
et al. A complete genome screen for genes predisposing to severe bipolar disorder in two Costa Rican pedigrees.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93(23):13060-13065.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Service
S, Molina
J, Deyoung
J,
et al. Results of a SNP genome screen in a large Costa Rican pedigree segregating for severe bipolar disorder.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141B(4):367-373.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Herzberg
I, Jasinska
A, García
J,
et al. Convergent linkage evidence from two Latin-American population isolates supports the presence of a susceptibility locus for bipolar disorder in 5q31-34.
Hum Mol Genet. 2006;15(21):3146-3153.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Kremeyer
B, García
J, Müller
H,
et al. Genome-wide linkage scan of bipolar disorder in a Colombian population isolate replicates loci on chromosomes 7p21-22, 1p31, 16p12 and 21q21-22 and identifies a novel locus on chromosome 12q.
Hum Hered. 2010;70(4):255-268.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Carvajal-Carmona
LG, Ophoff
R, Service
S,
et al. Genetic demography of Antioquia (Colombia) and the Central Valley of Costa Rica.
Hum Genet. 2003;112(5-6):534-541.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Service
S, DeYoung
J, Karayiorgou
M,
et al. Magnitude and distribution of linkage disequilibrium in population isolates and implications for genome-wide association studies.
Nat Genet. 2006;38(5):556-560.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Jasinska
AJ, Service
S, Jawaheer
D,
et al. A narrow and highly significant linkage signal for severe bipolar disorder in the chromosome 5q33 region in Latin American pedigrees.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B(7):998-1006.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Freimer
NB, Reus
VI, Escamilla
M,
et al. An approach to investigating linkage for bipolar disorder using large Costa Rican pedigrees.
Am J Med Genet. 1996;67(3):254-263.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Escamilla
MA, Spesny
M, Reus
VI,
et al. Use of linkage disequilibrium approaches to map genes for bipolar disorder in the Costa Rican population.
Am J Med Genet. 1996;67(3):244-253.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Freimer
NB, Reus
VI, Escamilla
MA,
et al. Genetic mapping using haplotype, association and linkage methods suggests a locus for severe bipolar disorder (BPI) at 18q22-q23.
Nat Genet. 1996;12(4):436-441.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Ophoff
RA, Escamilla
MA, Service
SK,
et al. Genomewide linkage disequilibrium mapping of severe bipolar disorder in a population isolate.
Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71(3):565-574.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Nurnberger
JI
Jr, Blehar
MC, Kaufmann
CA,
et al; NIMH Genetics Initiative. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies: rationale, unique features, and training.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(11):849-859, discussion 863-864.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Palacio
CA, García
J, Arbeláez
MP,
et al. Validation of the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS) in Colombia [in Spanish].
Biomedica. 2004;24(1):56-62.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Sheehan
DV, Lecrubier
Y, Sheehan
KH,
et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for
DSM-IV and
ICD-10.
J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):22-33.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Hong
KS, McInnes
LA, Service
SK,
et al. Genetic mapping using haplotype and model-free linkage analysis supports previous evidence for a locus predisposing to severe bipolar disorder at 5q31-33.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;125B(1):83-86.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Young
RC, Biggs
JT, Ziegler
VE, Meyer
DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.
Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Peters
E, Joseph
S, Day
S, Garety
P. Measuring delusional ideation: the 21-item Peters et al Delusions Inventory (PDI).
Schizophr Bull. 2004;30(4):1005-1022.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Akiskal
HS, Akiskal
KK. TEMPS: Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego.
J Affect Disord. 2005;85(1-2):1-2.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Patton
JH, Stanford
MS, Barratt
ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale.
J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768-774.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Lejuez
CW, Read
JP, Kahler
CW,
et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART).
J Exp Psychol Appl. 2002;8(2):75-84.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Glahn
DC, Almasy
L, Barguil
M,
et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder identified in multiplex multigenerational families.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):168-177.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Wechsler
D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2008.
53.Glahn
DC, Cannon
TD, Gur
RE, Ragland
JD, Gur
RC. Working memory constrains abstraction in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(1):34-42.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Kurtz
MM, Ragland
JD, Moberg
PJ, Gur
RC. The Penn Conditional Exclusion Test: a new measure of executive-function with alternate forms of repeat administration.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(2):191-201.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Stroop
JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
J Exp Psychol Gen. 1992;121(1):15-23.
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Brown
L, Sherbenou
RJ, Johnsen
SK. Test of Nonverbal Intelligence: A Language-Free Measure of Cognitive Ability. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1997.
57.Dale
AM, Fischl
B, Sereno
MI. Cortical surface-based analysis, I: segmentation and surface reconstruction.
Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):179-194.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Fischl
B, Sereno
MI, Dale
AM. Cortical surface-based analysis, II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system.
Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):195-207.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Smith
SM, Jenkinson
M, Woolrich
MW,
et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL.
Neuroimage. 2004;23(suppl 1):S208-S219.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Panizzon
MS, Fennema-Notestine
C, Eyler
LT,
et al. Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness.
Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(11):2728-2735.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Oishi
K, Zilles
K, Amunts
K,
et al. Human brain white matter atlas: identification and assignment of common anatomical structures in superficial white matter.
Neuroimage. 2008;43(3):447-457.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Mahon
K, Burdick
KE, Ikuta
T,
et al. Abnormal temporal lobe white matter as a biomarker for genetic risk of bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(2):177-182.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Sexton
CE, Mackay
CE, Ebmeier
KP. A systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in affective disorders.
Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(9):814-823.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Sprooten
E, Sussmann
JE, Clugston
A,
et al. White matter integrity in individuals at high genetic risk of bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(4):350-356.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Bartzokis
G, Lu
PH, Heydari
P,
et al. Multimodal magnetic resonance imaging assessment of white matter aging trajectories over the lifespan of healthy individuals.
Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72(12):1026-1034.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Budde
MD, Xie
M, Cross
AH, Song
SK. Axial diffusivity is the primary correlate of axonal injury in the experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis spinal cord: a quantitative pixelwise analysis.
J Neurosci. 2009;29(9):2805-2813.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Song
SK, Sun
SW, Ramsbottom
MJ, Chang
C, Russell
J, Cross
AH. Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water.
Neuroimage. 2002;17(3):1429-1436.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Song
SK, Sun
SW, Ju
WK, Lin
SJ, Cross
AH, Neufeld
AH. Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia.
Neuroimage. 2003;20(3):1714-1722.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Almasy
L, Blangero
J. Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees.
Am J Hum Genet. 1998;62(5):1198-1211.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Van der Waerden
BL. Order tests for the two-sample problem and their power.
Indag Math. 1952;14:453-458.
Google ScholarCrossref 73.Pilia
G, Chen
WM, Scuteri
A,
et al. Heritability of cardiovascular and personality traits in 6148 Sardinians.
PLoS Genet. 2006;2(8):e132.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Williams
JT, Van Eerdewegh
P, Almasy
L, Blangero
J. Joint multipoint linkage analysis of multivariate qualitative and quantitative traits, I: likelihood formulation and simulation results.
Am J Hum Genet. 1999;65(4):1134-1147.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Cruchaga
C, Kauwe
JS, Harari
O,
et al; GERAD Consortium; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI); Alzheimer Disease Genetic Consortium (ADGC). GWAS of cerebrospinal fluid tau levels identifies risk variants for Alzheimer’s disease.
Neuron. 2013;78(2):256-268.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Schürhoff
F, Szöke
A, Méary
A,
et al. Familial aggregation of delusional proneness in schizophrenia and bipolar pedigrees.
Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(7):1313-1319.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Kyaga
S, Lichtenstein
P, Boman
M, Hultman
C, Långström
N, Landén
M. Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300 000 people with severe mental disorder.
Br J Psychiatry. 2011;199(5):373-379.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 79.Kyaga
S, Landén
M, Boman
M, Hultman
CM, Långström
N, Lichtenstein
P. Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study.
J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(1):83-90.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Parker
G, Paterson
A, Fletcher
K, Blanch
B, Graham
R. The “magic button question” for those with a mood disorder: would they wish to re-live their condition?
J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):419-424.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Balanzá-Martínez
V, Rubio
C, Selva-Vera
G,
et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of relatives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(8):1426-1438.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 82.Robinson
LJ, Thompson
JM, Gallagher
P,
et al. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.
J Affect Disord. 2006;93(1-3):105-115.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Torres
IJ, Boudreau
VG, Yatham
LN. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2007;(434):17-26.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 84.Greenwood
TA, Braff
DL, Light
GA,
et al. Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(11):1242-1250.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 85.Gur
RE, Nimgaonkar
VL, Almasy
L,
et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(5):813-819.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 86.Steinberg
S, de Jong
S, Mattheisen
M,
et al; GROUP; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2. Common variant at 16p11.2 conferring risk of psychosis.
Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(1):108-114.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Arnone
D, Cavanagh
J, Gerber
D, Lawrie
SM, Ebmeier
KP, McIntosh
AM. Magnetic resonance imaging studies in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: meta-analysis.
Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(3):194-201.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 88.Bora
E, Fornito
A, Yücel
M, Pantelis
C. Voxelwise meta-analysis of gray matter abnormalities in bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(11):1097-1105.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 89.Hallahan
B, Newell
J, Soares
JC,
et al. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in bipolar disorder: an international collaborative mega-analysis of individual adult patient data.
Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(4):326-335.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 90.Kempton
MJ, Geddes
JR, Ettinger
U, Williams
SC, Grasby
PM. Meta-analysis, database, and meta-regression of 98 structural imaging studies in bipolar disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(9):1017-1032.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 91.McDonald
C, Zanelli
J, Rabe-Hesketh
S,
et al. Meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometry studies in bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(6):411-417.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 92.Rimol
LM, Hartberg
CB, Nesvåg
R,
et al. Cortical thickness and subcortical volumes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(1):41-50.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 93.Houenou
J, Frommberger
J, Carde
S,
et al. Neuroimaging-based markers of bipolar disorder: evidence from two meta-analyses.
J Affect Disord. 2011;132(3):344-355.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 94.Chen
CH, Suckling
J, Lennox
BR, Ooi
C, Bullmore
ET. A quantitative meta-analysis of fMRI studies in bipolar disorder.
Bipolar Disord. 2011;13(1):1-15.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 95.Cusi
AM, Nazarov
A, Holshausen
K, Macqueen
GM, McKinnon
MC. Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders.
J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37(3):154-169.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 96.Kupferschmidt
DA, Zakzanis
KK. Toward a functional neuroanatomical signature of bipolar disorder: quantitative evidence from the neuroimaging literature.
Psychiatry Res. 2011;193(2):71-79.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 97.Townsend
J, Altshuler
LL. Emotion processing and regulation in bipolar disorder: a review.
Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(4):326-339.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 98.Roberts
G, Green
MJ, Breakspear
M,
et al. Reduced inferior frontal gyrus activation during response inhibition to emotional stimuli in youth at high risk of bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(1):55-61.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 99.Bearden
CE, van Erp
TG, Dutton
RA,
et al. Mapping corpus callosum morphology in twin pairs discordant for bipolar disorder.
Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(10):2415-2424.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 100.van der Schot
AC, Vonk
R, Brans
RG,
et al. Influence of genes and environment on brain volumes in twin pairs concordant and discordant for bipolar disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(2):142-151.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 101.Brotman
MA, Guyer
AE, Lawson
ES,
et al. Facial emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for bipolar disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(3):385-389.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 102.Brotman
MA, Skup
M, Rich
BA,
et al. Risk for bipolar disorder is associated with face-processing deficits across emotions.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(12):1455-1461.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 103.Mercer
L, Becerra
R. A unique emotional processing profile of euthymic bipolar disorder? a critical review.
J Affect Disord. 2013;146(3):295-309.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 104.Samamé
C, Martino
DJ, Strejilevich
SA. Social cognition in euthymic bipolar disorder: systematic review and meta-analytic approach.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;125(4):266-280.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 105.Adleman
NE, Kayser
RR, Olsavsky
AK,
et al. Abnormal fusiform activation during emotional-face encoding assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Psychiatry Res. 2013;212(2):161-163.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 107.Corvin
A, Donohoe
G, Hargreaves
A, Gallagher
L, Gill
M. The cognitive genetics of neuropsychiatric disorders.
Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2012;12:579-613.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 108.Donohoe
G, Deary
IJ, Glahn
DC, Malhotra
AK, Burdick
KE. Neurocognitive phenomics: examining the genetic basis of cognitive abilities.
Psychol Med. 2013;43(10):2027-2036.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 109.Greenwood
TA, Beeri
MS, Schmeidler
J,
et al. Heritability of cognitive functions in families of successful cognitive aging probands from the Central Valley of Costa Rica.
J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;27(4):897-907.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 110.Hervey
AS, Greenfield
K, Gualtieri
CT. Heritability in cognitive performance: evidence using computer-based testing.
J Genet Psychol. 2012;173(1):112-118.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 111.Lee
T, Mosing
MA, Henry
JD,
et al; OATS Research Team. Genetic influences on four measures of executive functions and their covariation with general cognitive ability: the Older Australian Twins Study.
Behav Genet. 2012;42(4):528-538.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 112.Oliver
BR, Plomin
R. Twins’ Early Development Study (TEDS): a multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems from childhood through adolescence.
Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10(1):96-105.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 113.van Soelen
IL, Brouwer
RM, van Leeuwen
M, Kahn
RS, Hulshoff Pol
HE, Boomsma
DI. Heritability of verbal and performance intelligence in a pediatric longitudinal sample.
Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14(2):119-128.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 114.Bearden
CE, Hoffman
KM, Cannon
TD. The neuropsychology and neuroanatomy of bipolar affective disorder: a critical review.
Bipolar Disord. 2001;3(3):106-150, discussion 151-153.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 115.Evans
L, Akiskal
HS, Keck
PE
Jr,
et al. Familiality of temperament in bipolar disorder: support for a genetic spectrum.
J Affect Disord. 2005;85(1-2):153-168.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 116.Glahn
DC, Bearden
CE, Barguil
M,
et al. The neurocognitive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(8):910-916.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref