[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Hippocampal Subfield Segmentation
Hippocampal Subfield Segmentation

A, FreeSurfer segmentation and location of different subfields within the hippocampus. CA indicates cornu ammonis; DG, dentate gyrus. Effect sizes per group according to the Cohen d color scale are shown for healthy controls (HCs) vs probands (B), HCs vs patients with schizophrenia (SZ) (C), HCs vs patients with schizoaffective disorder (SZA) (D), and HCs vs patients with psychotic bipolar disorder (BPP) (E).

Figure 2.  Relationship of Left Hippocampal Volume With Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) Composite and List-Learning Scores
Relationship of Left Hippocampal Volume With Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) Composite and List-Learning Scores

Scatterplot of individual-specific data for all participants and diagnostic group correlations. A, Correlation between BACS composite scores (z scale) and left hippocampal volume, with healthy controls (HCs) separate from probands. B, Correlation between list-learning scores (z scale) and left hippocampal volume, with HCs separate from probands. BPP indicates psychotic bipolar disorder; SZ, schizophrenia; and SZA, schizoaffective disorder.

Table 1.  Volume and Effect Size for Medial Temporal Regions
Volume and Effect Size for Medial Temporal Regions
Table 2.  Volume and Effect Size for Hippocampal Subfields
Volume and Effect Size for Hippocampal Subfields
Table 3.  Hippocampal Subfield Correlations With Symptom Severity and Cognition in Probands
Hippocampal Subfield Correlations With Symptom Severity and Cognition in Probands
1.
Keshavan  MS, Clementz  BA, Pearlson  GD, Sweeney  JA, Tamminga  CA.  Reimagining psychoses: an agnostic approach to diagnosis.  Schizophr Res. 2013;146(1-3):10-16. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.02.022.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Kraepelin  E. Gedanken über die Arbeitscurve. In: Kraepelin  E, ed.  Psychologische Arbeiten. Berlin, Germany: Julius Springer; 1894:535-547.
3.
Nelson  MD, Saykin  AJ, Flashman  LA, Riordan  HJ.  Hippocampal volume reduction in schizophrenia as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging: a meta-analytic study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(5):433-440.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Steen  RG, Mull  C, McClure  R, Hamer  RM, Lieberman  JA.  Brain volume in first-episode schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies.  Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:510-518. doi:10.1192/bjp.188.6.510.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Heckers  S, Heinsen  H, Geiger  B, Beckmann  H.  Hippocampal neuron number in schizophrenia: a stereological study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(11):1002-1008.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Swanson  LW, Köhler  C.  Anatomical evidence for direct projections from the entorhinal area to the entire cortical mantle in the rat.  J Neurosci. 1986;6(10):3010-3023.PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Arnold  OH.  Schizophrenia: a disturbance of signal interaction between the entorhinal cortex and the dentate gyrus? the contribution of experimental dibenamine psychosis to the pathogenesis of schizophrenia: a hypothesis.  Neuropsychobiology. 1999;40(1):21-32.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Keshavan  MS, Tandon  R, Boutros  NN, Nasrallah  HA.  Schizophrenia, “just the facts”: what we know in 2008 part 3: neurobiology.  Schizophr Res. 2008;106(2-3):89-107. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.07.020.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Strasser  HC, Lilyestrom  J, Ashby  ER,  et al.  Hippocampal and ventricular volumes in psychotic and nonpsychotic bipolar patients compared with schizophrenia patients and community control subjects: a pilot study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):633-639. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.009.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Szeszko  PR, Goldberg  E, Gunduz-Bruce  H,  et al.  Smaller anterior hippocampal formation volume in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(12):2190-2197.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Seidman  LJ, Pantelis  C, Keshavan  MS,  et al.  A review and new report of medial temporal lobe dysfunction as a vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric family study of the parahippocampal gyrus.  Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):803-830.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Pearlson  GD, Barta  PE, Powers  RE,  et al.  Ziskind-Somerfeld Research Award 1996: medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(1):1-14. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00373-3.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Narr  KL, Thompson  PM, Szeszko  P,  et al.  Regional specificity of hippocampal volume reductions in first-episode schizophrenia.  Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1563-1575. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.011.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Thermenos  HW, Keshavan  MS, Juelich  RJ,  et al.  A review of neuroimaging studies of young relatives of individuals with schizophrenia: a developmental perspective from schizotaxia to schizophrenia.  Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2013;162B(7):604-635. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32170.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Velakoulis  D, Wood  SJ, Wong  MTH,  et al.  Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(2):139-149. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.139.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Chakos  MH, Schobel  SA, Gu  H,  et al.  Duration of illness and treatment effects on hippocampal volume in male patients with schizophrenia.  Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:26-31. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.1.26.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Tamminga  CA, Stan  AD, Wagner  AD.  The hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(10):1178-1193. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081187.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Squire  LR, Zola-Morgan  S.  The medial temporal lobe memory system.  Science. 1991;253(5026):1380-1386.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Karnik-Henry  MS, Wang  L, Barch  DM, Harms  MP, Campanella  C, Csernansky  JG.  Medial temporal lobe structure and cognition in individuals with schizophrenia and in their non-psychotic siblings.  Schizophr Res. 2012;138(2-3):128-135. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.015.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cohen  NJ, Eichenbaum  H.  Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. Boston, MA: MIT Press; 1993.
21.
Davachi  L, Mitchell  JP, Wagner  AD.  Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(4):2157-2162. doi:10.1073/pnas.0337195100.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Aminoff  EM, Kveraga  K, Bar  M.  The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(8):379-390. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.009.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Aleman  A, Hijman  R, de Haan  EHF, Kahn  RS.  Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1358-1366.PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Cirillo  MA, Seidman  LJ.  Verbal declarative memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: from clinical assessment to genetics and brain mechanisms.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2003;13(2):43-77.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
McDonald  C, Zanelli  J, Rabe-Hesketh  S,  et al.  Meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometry studies in bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(6):411-417. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.021.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Kempton  MJ, Geddes  JR, Ettinger  U, Williams  SCR, Grasby  PM.  Meta-analysis, database, and meta-regression of 98 structural imaging studies in bipolar disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(9):1017-1032. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.9.1017.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Hajek  T, Kopecek  M, Höschl  C, Alda  M.  Smaller hippocampal volumes in patients with bipolar disorder are masked by exposure to lithium: a meta-analysis.  J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37(5):333-343. doi:10.1503/jpn.110143.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Van Leemput  K, Bakkour  A, Benner  T,  et al.  Automated segmentation of hippocampal subfields from ultra-high resolution in vivo MRI.  Hippocampus. 2009;19(6):549-557. doi:10.1002/hipo.20615.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Pfohl  B, Blum  N, Zimmermann  M.  Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality: SIDP-IV. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 1997.
30.
Skudlarski  P, Schretlen  DJ, Thaker  GK,  et al.  Diffusion tensor imaging white matter endophenotypes in patients with schizophrenia or psychotic bipolar disorder and their relatives.  Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(8):886-898. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12111448.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Andreasen  NC, Pressler  M, Nopoulos  P, Miller  D, Ho  BC.  Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):255-262. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
First  MB, Spitzer  RL, Gibbon  M, Williams  JBW.  Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2012.
33.
Kay  SR, Fiszbein  A, Opler  LA.  The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261-276.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Montgomery  SA, Asberg  M.  A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change.  Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134:382-389.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Young  RC, Biggs  JT, Ziegler  VE, Meyer  DA.  A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.  Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Keefe  RSE, Goldberg  TE, Harvey  PD, Gold  JM, Poe  MP, Coughenour  L.  The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery.  Schizophr Res. 2004;68(2-3):283-297. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Desikan  RS, Ségonne  F, Fischl  B,  et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.  Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968-980. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Dixon  WJ, Tukey  JW.  Approximate behavior of the distribution of Winsorized t (Trimming/Winsorization 2).  Technometrics. 1968;10(1):83-98. doi:10.1080/00401706.1968.10490537.Google Scholar
39.
Hochberg  Y.  A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance.  Biometrika. 1988;75(4):800-802. doi:10.1093/biomet/75.4.800.Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Cohen  J.  Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
41.
Falkai  P, Bogerts  B.  Cell loss in the hippocampus of schizophrenics.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1986;236(3):154-161. doi:10.1007/BF00380943.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Harrison  PJ.  The hippocampus in schizophrenia: a review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;174(1):151-162. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1761-y.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Tamminga  CA, Holcomb  HH.  Phenotype of schizophrenia: a review and formulation.  Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(1):27-39. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001563.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Adriano  F, Caltagirone  C, Spalletta  G.  Hippocampal volume reduction in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis.  Neuroscientist. 2012;18(2):180-200. doi:10.1177/1073858410395147.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Strakowski  SM, Delbello  MP, Adler  CM.  The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: a review of neuroimaging findings.  Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(1):105-116. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001585.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Kühn  S, Musso  F, Mobascher  A, Warbrick  T, Winterer  G, Gallinat  J.  Hippocampal subfields predict positive symptoms in schizophrenia: first evidence from brain morphometry.  Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e127. doi:10.1038/tp.2012.51.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
O’Reilly  RC, McClelland  JL.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off.  Hippocampus. 1994;4(6):661-682. doi:10.1002/hipo.450040605.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Tamminga  CA, Southcott  S, Sacco  C, Wagner  AD, Ghose  S.  Glutamate dysfunction in hippocampus: relevance of dentate gyrus and CA3 signaling.  Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(5):927-935. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs062.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Grace  AA.  Dopamine system dysregulation by the ventral subiculum as the common pathophysiological basis for schizophrenia psychosis, psychostimulant abuse, and stress.  Neurotox Res. 2010;18(3-4):367-376. doi:10.1007/s12640-010-9154-6.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Herman  JP, Mueller  NK.  Role of the ventral subiculum in stress integration.  Behav Brain Res. 2006;174(2):215-224. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2006.05.035.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
O’Mara  S.  The subiculum: what it does, what it might do, and what neuroanatomy has yet to tell us.  J Anat. 2005;207(3):271-282. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00446.x.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Sapolsky  RM.  Glucocorticoid toxicity in the hippocampus: temporal aspects of neuronal vulnerability.  Brain Res. 1985;359(1-2):300-305.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Lodge  DJ, Grace  AA.  Hippocampal dysregulation of dopamine system function and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.  Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2011;32(9):507-513. doi:10.1016/j.tips.2011.05.001.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Libby  LA, Ekstrom  AD, Ragland  JD, Ranganath  C.  Differential connectivity of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices within human hippocampal subregions revealed by high-resolution functional imaging.  J Neurosci. 2012;32(19):6550-6560. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-11.2012.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Harrison  PJ, Eastwood  SL.  Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.  Hippocampus. 2001;11(5):508-519. doi:10.1002/hipo.1067.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Heckers  S, Konradi  C.  Hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia.  Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:529-553. doi:10.1007/7854_2010_43.PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Keshavan  MS.  Development, disease and degeneration in schizophrenia: a unitary pathophysiological model.  J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33(6):513-521.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Cherlyn  SYT, Woon  PS, Liu  JJ, Ong  WY, Tsai  GC, Sim  K.  Genetic association studies of glutamate, GABA and related genes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a decade of advance.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34(6):958-977. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.002.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Kraguljac  NV, White  DM, Reid  MA, Lahti  AC.  Increased hippocampal glutamate and volumetric deficits in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(12):1294-1302. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2437.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Harrison  PJ, Law  AJ, Eastwood  SL.  Glutamate receptors and transporters in the hippocampus in schizophrenia.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:94-101.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Kaymaz  N, van Os  J.  Heritability of structural brain traits an endophenotype approach to deconstruct schizophrenia.  Int Rev Neurobiol. 2009;89:85-130. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(09)89005-3.PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Francis  AN, Seidman  LJ, Tandon  N,  et al.  Reduced subicular subdivisions of the hippocampal formation and verbal declarative memory impairments in young relatives at risk for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1-3):154-157. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.002.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Meda  SA, Stevens  MC, Folley  BS, Calhoun  VD, Pearlson  GD.  Evidence for anomalous network connectivity during working memory encoding in schizophrenia: an ICA based analysis.  PLoS One. 2009;4(11):e7911. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007911.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Jovicich  J, Czanner  S, Han  X,  et al.  MRI-derived measurements of human subcortical, ventricular and intracranial brain volumes: reliability effects of scan sessions, acquisition sequences, data analyses, scanner upgrade, scanner vendors and field strengths.  Neuroimage. 2009;46(1):177-192. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.010.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
July 2014

Medial Temporal Lobe Structures and Hippocampal Subfields in Psychotic Disorders: Findings From the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) Study

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 2Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas
  • 3School of Social Work, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
  • 4Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
  • 5Massachusetts General Hospital/Massachusetts Institute of Technology/Harvard Medical School Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston
  • 6Department of Psychology, BioImaging Research Center, University of Georgia, Athens
  • 7Department of Neuroscience, BioImaging Research Center, University of Georgia, Athens
  • 8Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Institute of Living, Hartford, Connecticut
  • 9Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut
  • 10University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas
JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(7):769-777. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.453
Abstract

Importance  Structural alterations in the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe regions have been observed in schizophrenia. How these alterations and hippocampal subfields might differ across the psychosis spectrum remains unclear.

Objectives  To characterize medial temporal lobe structures, including hippocampal subfields, using magnetic resonance imaging and to examine their relation to psychosis and cognitive function across the psychosis spectrum.

Design, Setting, and Participants  Case-control, cross-sectional neuroimaging study in a large series of probands with psychotic disorders and healthy volunteers as part of the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP). Patients with psychotic disorders (schizophrenia, n = 219; schizoaffective disorder, n = 142; and psychotic bipolar disorder, n = 188) and healthy controls (n = 337) were recruited across ambulatory clinics at university health centers in the B-SNIP consortium.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Medial temporal lobe and hippocampal subfields were quantified with an automated parcellation approach using FreeSurfer software. Memory and other cognitive parameters were assessed using standardized neuropsychological tests.

Results  Hippocampal volume reductions were seen in all 3 diagnostic groups when compared with healthy controls; alterations in the entorhinal cortex and parahippocampal regions were limited to schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorders (P < .001). Smaller volumes across the hippocampal subfields were seen in all 3 psychotic disorders, with the most prominent differences being in cornu ammonis 2/3 (P < .001). Hippocampal volumes were positively correlated with psychosis severity, declarative memory, and overall cognitive performance (P < .05).

Conclusions and Relevance  Alterations in the hippocampus were evident across psychotic disorders. Hippocampal subfields that participate in memory-related processes supporting pattern separation and pattern completion might be abnormal and may underlie the pathophysiology of psychosis.

Despite more than a century of research, the pathophysiology of psychotic disorders remains unclear.1-4 Neuropathological1-5 and functional (in vivo neuroimaging) studies have identified the medial temporal lobe (MTL) as a key area of alteration in psychoses, especially schizophrenia (SZ).6-10 Volumetric alterations in the hippocampus have been characterized as a hallmark feature of SZ, although alterations have been observed in the parahippocampal gyrus and the entorhinal cortex (EC) as well.3,4,11,12 These alterations appear in first-episode SZ10,13 and in those at familial risk for this illness11,14 and may progress in severity during the course of illness.15,16

Psychosis has been theorized to result from cognitive mistakes that may stem from a hippocampally mediated failure to discriminate between present and past memory experiences17,18; the MTL and its subregions, which have been classically implicated in declarative memory19,20 (which is impaired in SZ), are therefore of particular relevance in studies of pathophysiology of psychoses. The hippocampus comprises cornu ammonis 1 through 4 (CA1-4), dentate gyrus (DG), and subiculum. The parahippocampal cortex (PHC) and EC surround the hippocampus and serve as its primary inputs. Subfields in the hippocampal formation such as the DG and CA3 play key roles in encoding distinct representations (pattern separation) and associating previous memory traces with current input (pattern completion).17 The EC stores multimodal sensory input, while the aforementioned input is compared with older memory traces to distinguish between novel and familiar information.6,7 The parahippocampal region assimilates novel information with longer-term contextual information.19,21,22 It has been suggested that deficits in these regions, ie, a failure of DG-mediated pattern separation, and increased CA3-mediated pattern completion may result in spurious associations of memories, ultimately manifesting as psychosis.17 Verbal declarative or explicit memory (the conscious recollection of words, stories, or events), known to be impaired in SZ, is of importance in understanding the neural substrates of psychoses.23,24 Investigating structural alterations among the subfields of the hippocampal complex may therefore help in understanding the pathophysiology of psychoses.

Alterations in the MTL in patients with bipolar disorder are inconsistent, with most studies showing either modest or no change.3,25,26 However, smaller hippocampi have been observed among patients with psychotic bipolar disorder (BPP) but not in patients with nonpsychotic bipolar disorder.9 This raises the question of whether MTL and hippocampal alterations cut across a range of psychosis spectrum disorders or whether they align with the traditional separation between SZ and bipolar disorder, a dichotomy whose biological validity remains to be established after more than a century of study.1 Interestingly, there appears to be a medication effect, with patients with bipolar disorder taking lithium showing no hippocampal reductions.27 Larger well-characterized samples are clearly needed to address this question.

The Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) consortium, which assessed endophenotypes across the psychotic spectrum, provided a unique opportunity to study the relationship between psychosis, cognition, MTL, and hippocampal subfields in a large sample of individuals with psychotic disorders. Recent advances in image processing28 allow precise parcellation of the hippocampal subfields. We hypothesized that (1) the hippocampal subfield alterations would be most prominent in SZ and schizoaffective disorder (SZA) and least in BPP, (2) patients with psychosis would demonstrate significant alterations in the volumes of the DG, CA2/3, PHC, and EC, with the most widespread alterations in SZ, and (3) deficits in these regions would correlate with positive symptoms and declarative memory. To test these hypotheses, we examined volumes of the hippocampal subfields, PHC, and EC in patients with SZ, SZA, and BPP.

Methods
Study Participants

This study included clinically stable probands with SZ (n = 219), SZA (n = 142), or BPP (n = 188) as well as 337 healthy controls (HCs) recruited as part of the B-SNIP consortium, a 6-site collaboration (Wayne State University, Harvard University, Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, University of Chicago/University of Illinois at Chicago, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, and the Institute of Living/Yale University).

Inclusion criteria were the following: (1) ages 15 to 65 years; (2) sufficient proficiency in English at the sixth-grade level or higher; (3) no significant neurologic disorders including those secondary to head injury; (4) no history of substance abuse within the last month or substance dependence within the last 6 months; and (5) negative urine toxicology screening results on the day of testing. The HCs met the following additional criteria: (1) no personal or family history (first degree) of psychotic or bipolar disorders; (2) no personal history of recurrent mood disorder; (3) no lifetime history of substance dependence; and (4) no history of any significant cluster A Axis II personality features defined by meeting full criteria or within 1 criterion of a cluster A diagnosis using the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality.29 Institutional review boards at each of the 6 sites approved the study, and all sites used identical diagnostic, clinical, and recruitment techniques.30 All participants provided written informed consent. All but 88 patients (SZ, n = 17; SZA, n = 19; BPP, n = 52) were taking antipsychotics and 82 patients (SZ, n = 13; SZA, n = 16; BPP, n = 53) were taking lithium. Antipsychotic dosing equivalents were computed using the method of Andreasen et al.31

Diagnoses were based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders.32 Symptom ratings were completed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS),33 the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale,34 and the Young Mania Rating Scale.35 Cognition was evaluated using the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS).36 The BACS composite score indexed overall cognition, and the list-learning measure was used to assess verbal declarative memory.

Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging

High-resolution isotropic T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient echo scans were performed (scanning parameters are listed in eAppendix in Supplement). All images underwent rigorous data quality control, which was performed blind to participant identity. Fifty-one participants were excluded from this analysis owing to severe motion artifacts or scanner inhomogeneity (HC, n = 10; SZ, n = 22; SZA, n = 10; BPP, n = 9). Images were converted to Neuroimaging Informatics Technology Initiative format and checked for scanner artifacts by trained raters. Images were then run through a first-level autoreconstruction in FreeSurfer version 5.1 software.37 The skull-stripped brains were checked for remaining dura or sinuses that could interfere with accurate segmentation. When nonbrain tissue was found, trained raters blinded to clinical data edited images manually. All raters (I.M., N.T., A.N.F.) had interrater reliabilities and intrarater reliability (intraclass r) greater than 95%. When deemed sufficiently clean for segmentation by an independent rater, images were run through second- and third-level autoreconstruction, in which gray matter surface area, thickness, and volume measures were extracted.

Automated hippocampal subfield segmentation was carried out using a separate processing pipeline included in the FreeSurfer software package.37 The subfield regional volumes that are extracted include CA1, CA2/3, CA4/DG, presubiculum, subiculum, hippocampal fissure, and fimbria.28 This method, which uses a Bayesian probabilistic model to automatically segment the hippocampus, has been validated with manual morphometric measurements of ultra–high-resolution magnetic resonance imaging scans. The model uses prior distributions from hippocampal image data to generate predictions about where neuroanatomical labels typically occur throughout the image.28 Voxel measurements of the subfields were 0.5 × 0.5 × 0.5 mm3.

Statistical Analysis

Outliers were handled by removing participants with data showing more than 4 SDs from the mean (HC, n = 5; SZ, n = 5; SZA, n = 0; BPP, n = 2). Participants between the third and fourth SD (HC, n = 18; SZ, n = 16; SZA, n = 7; BPP, n = 11) were winsorised to the third SD.38 When appropriate, a 1-way analysis of variance and χ2 tests were used to test for differences between groups in demographic and clinical variables. First, contrasts comparing HCs with probands were run on the MTL region using age, sex, site, race, and intracranial volume as covariates. If the resulting P value of a composite structure was less than .05, subregions within the composite structure were tested, adjusting the number of regions within each composite structure with the Hochberg method.39 For regions showing a trending difference (P < .05) at the proband level, a contrast comparing HCs with each diagnostic group was run, adjusting for number of contrasts with the Hochberg method.39 Regions assessed bilaterally within the hippocampus include CA1, CA2/3, CA4/DG, presubiculum, and subiculum. The parahippocampal region included the parahippocampal gyrus and EC. As shown by Van Leemput et al,28 measurements of smaller subfields may be unreliable; therefore, we did not include the hippocampal fissure, fimbria, hippocampal tail, and regions where subfields are not discernable. Effect sizes (Cohen d) were calculated using pooled standard deviations and residualized means adjusted for covariates.40

Partial Spearman rank correlations were used to investigate the relationships of symptom severity and cognition with hippocampal volumes, controlling for age, sex, intracranial volume, and site. Relationships with symptom severity were assessed using the PANSS. Measures used for analysis were PANSS positive subscale, item 1 (delusions), and item 3 (hallucinations). Relationships with cognition were assessed using the z-scaled BACS composite and sum of list-learning tasks. A 1-way analysis of variance was used to test for differences between HCs and probands in the BACS composite and list-learning tasks while controlling for age, sex, site, and intracranial volume.

Results

Our final sample included 886 participants (HC, n = 337; SZ, n = 219; SZA, n = 142; BPP, n = 188), consisting of 416 males (HC, n = 152; SZ, n = 145; SZA, n = 62; BPP, n = 57) and 470 females (HC, n = 185; SZ, n = 74; SZA, n = 80; BPP, n = 131) (eTable in Supplement). The mean age of our sample was 37.3 years (HC, 37.2 years; SZ, 35.1 years; SZA, 35.7 years; BPP, 36.1 years). The groups did not differ by age (F = 1.32; P = .27) but showed significant sex (F = 54.6; P < .001), race (F = 48.1; P < .001), and site (F = 62.1; P < .001) effects; thus, these variables were included as covariates in our analysis. There were group differences in intracranial volume (F = 6.5; P < .001). No site by group or sex by group interactions were seen for any regional gray matter measures.

MTL Volumes

Hippocampal volumes were significantly reduced bilaterally when comparing all probands and HCs (Table 1). Bilateral hippocampal volume reductions were also present between individual diagnostic groups and HCs. Somewhat smaller bilateral reductions in mean parahippocampal gyral volume were observed in all probands when compared with HCs. The SZ and SZA groups showed a bilateral reduction in parahippocampal gyrus volume but the BPP group showed no difference when compared with HCs. Reductions in mean EC volumes were observed only on the left side for the SZA group compared with HCs. No differences in bilateral EC volumes were observed when SZ and BPP groups were compared with HCs.

Hippocampal Subfields

Having observed significant group differences in the hippocampus, we proceeded with subfield analyses. Bilateral hippocampal subfields showed widespread differences across all probands with statistically significant reductions of mean volume in CA1, CA2/3, CA4/DG, presubiculum, and subiculum (Table 2 and Figure 1).

When individual diagnostic groups were compared with HCs, significant volume reductions were found in the SZ group bilaterally in CA2/3, CA4/DG, presubiculum, subiculum, and CA1. Compared with HCs, the SZA group displayed statistically significant volume reductions bilaterally in CA1, CA2/3, CA4/DG, presubiculum, and subiculum. The most prominent reductions were in SZ bilaterally in CA2/3, subiculum, and CA4/DG. Compared with HCs, the BPP group displayed bilateral volume reductions in CA2/3. The presubiculum was significant only on the left side. The CA4/DG and the subiculum were significant only on the right side.

Clinical and BACS Correlations

We did not find any correlations between chlorpromazine equivalent antipsychotic dosage and hippocampal subfield volumes (r = −0.02 to 0.007). A total of 495 probands (SZ, n = 194; SZA, n = 129; BPP, n = 172) had imaging and PANSS data (Table 3 and Figure 2). We observed significant (Hochberg-corrected) negative correlations between hippocampal volume and symptom severity. The left hippocampus, CA4/DG, presubiculum, and subiculum correlated negatively with PANSS positive subscale and hallucinations item scale scores (r = −0.16 to −0.11). There was a significant negative correlation between the left CA2/3 and hallucinations item scale score (r = −0.11). The right hippocampus and subiculum were also negatively correlated with PANSS positive subscale score (r = −0.12 and −0.13, respectively). The right subiculum negatively correlated with delusions item scale score (r = −0.12).

A total of 472 probands (SZ, n = 192; SZA, n = 119; BPP, n = 161) and 286 HCs had imaging and BACS data. We observed a significant difference between HCs and probands in BACS composite (P < .001) and BACS list-learning (P < .001) scores. Positive significant correlations between hippocampal volumes, BACS composite score, and BACS list-learning score were seen in probands but not HCs. The hippocampus and all hippocampal subfields demonstrated significant positive correlations with the BACS composite score (r = 0.09 to 0.19). Bilaterally, the hippocampus positively correlated with BACS list-learning score (r = 0.14). The left CA1, CA2/3, CA4/DG, and subiculum also positively correlated with BACS list-learning score (r = 0.11 to 0.14).

Discussion

We observed widespread volumetric reductions in the hippocampus and its subfields in probands with psychosis compared with HCs, consistent with and extending numerous previous observations.41-43 Widespread MTL volumetric reductions were seen in SZ and SZA consistent with prior literature.42,44 While patients with BPP did not show alteration in other MTL regions, hippocampal volume reductions were evident; this observation in a large sample is particularly important and helps address the inconsistent findings in previous BPP literature.45

The observed volume reductions in the hippocampus and hippocampal subfields across diagnostic groups suggest that smaller hippocampal volumes may be a common trait shared across the psychosis spectrum. Consistent with a recent study by Kühn et al,46 we observed a significant relationship between hippocampal subfield volumes and psychotic symptoms. However, these correlations were weak, perhaps resulting from the relatively small variance in psychotic symptoms in our sample, which consisted mainly of patients stabilized with antipsychotics.

As predicted, we noted an association, albeit modest, between bilateral hippocampal volumes and composite cognitive score as well as verbal declarative memory in probands. The association was strongest for the overall hippocampus and for the subiculum. However, all subregions other than the presubiculum showed significant associations in the predicted direction in the probands (smaller volumes, worse performance) and not in HCs, suggesting that hippocampal volumes are linked to the cognitive deficits associated with psychosis.

The observed relationships between hippocampal structure and psychosis on the one hand and with declarative memory on the other hand are consistent with the view that psychosis may be related to alterations in a number of nodes of the associative memory network in the hippocampal formation. While all subfields showed structural volume reductions across probands, these were most prominent in the CA2/3, DG, and subicular regions. The hippocampus mediates the encoding of new memories20,47 and the retrieval of old memories.47 Distinct hippocampal structures may play different roles in the process of memory encoding and retrieval. Pattern separation, the flexible distinction of multiple memory constructs from highly similar stimuli, is thought to be carried out by the DG and CA2/3 and establishes distinct memory.17 Pattern completion involves retrieval of full memory from partial stimuli cues via the process of associative recognition thought to be mediated through CA2/3 and CA1 augmented by the trisynaptic pathway. Spurious associations may arise owing to a partial failure of the DG, thus leading to increased and inaccurate CA2/3 activity and consequently to the production of psychotic symptoms.17,48 The subiculum is thought to serve 2 functions salient to the manifestation of psychosis: (1) hippocampal gating of dopamine-mediated information flow from the prefrontal cortex,49 and (2) inhibition of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.50,51 Both systems have been described extensively elsewhere.49,52 Thus, altered subiculum function might lead to (1) failure of dopaminergic gating with a net increase in mesolimbic dopaminergic activity, and (2) dysregulation (exaggeration) of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis–mediated stress response. Excessive hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity may in turn contribute to hippocampal atrophy.52,53 These changes together may be part of the causative chain leading to psychosis in the context of spurious information processing and the attendant misinterpretation of neutral sensory inputs caused by the EC, PHC, DG/CA4, and CA2/3 regions. Structural alterations in the parahippocampus and EC, which provide most of the input to the hippocampus,48,54 may also be pertinent in the manifestation of psychosis, although their precise role remains to be understood.

Causal mechanisms involved in the pathogenesis of the observed hippocampal abnormalities remain unclear. We found that bilateral volume reductions in CA2/3 were shared across all diagnostic groups, with the SZ and SZA groups showing the most impairment. Hippocampal alterations have been thought to result from developmentally mediated disruptions in glutamatergic excitatory and/or inhibitory interneurons leading to synaptic or dendritic pathology.55-57 Recent reports identify glutamate genes to be implicated in both SZ and BPP.58 Interestingly, another recent study59 suggests that hippocampal volume reduction in SZ is correlated with increases in glutamatergic metabolites (glutamine/glutamate). While focused investigations into hippocampal glutamate among patients with BPP are still needed, perturbations in hippocampal glutamate may play a part in the shared CA2/3 volume reductions observed across the psychosis spectrum. Imaging studies in SZ have identified increases in baseline blood perfusion and decreases in task-related activation in the hippocampus, thought to be related to impaired glutamatergically mediated synaptic plasticity.48 Such neuropathological alterations could potentially result from genetic factors interacting with environmental influences.60

Few studies have definitively examined whether in vivo hippocampal subfield morphology can serve as an endophenotypic marker. Evidence for hippocampal alterations in unaffected relatives,18,48 evidence for moderate heritability of hippocampal volumes,61 and our recent observation of hippocampal subfield alterations among first-degree relatives of patients with SZ62 support this view. Investigation of genomic correlates of brain structure can help further examine this question.63

To our knowledge, this is the first report of changes in hippocampal subfield volumes and related MTL structures across the psychotic spectrum. A major strength is the use of an automated parcellation approach. While manual region-of-interest tracing remains the gold standard for morphometric studies, it is time-consuming, making it impractical for large-sample studies. Semiautomated FreeSurfer whole-brain and hippocampal subfield segmentations have been rigorously tested and proven to produce reliable measurements.28,64 Larger subfields such as CA2/3 and CA4/DG correlate well with manual volume segmentations; smaller subfields produced less reliable segmentations and were not considered for analysis.28 Another strength is the large, clinically well-characterized sample of patients across DSM diagnoses within the psychosis spectrum.

Our study was cross-sectional and most patients were receiving medications. Because medication effects may not necessarily be linear, use of chlorpromazine equivalents alone as a way to address this confound has limitations. Future studies need to examine hippocampal subfield data prospectively before and after introduction of antipsychotics in previously untreated patients. We did not have a sample of patients with nonpsychotic bipolar disorder, which could have more definitively clarified the nature of hippocampal alterations in bipolar disorders. Finally, our imaging parameters were not optimized for imaging hippocampal subfields as in the study by Van Leemput et al,28 which used long acquisition times and frequent averaging to yield high signal-noise ratios. Future studies of hippocampal structure need to use more rigorous image acquisition and multimodal techniques to elucidate structure-function relationships. Despite these limitations, our samples are some of the largest to date, we tested specificity among the psychoses, and we used the most sophisticated tool for subdividing the hippocampus to date.

Conclusions

This study firmly establishes the hippocampus as one of the key nodes in the pathway to psychosis. Understanding the functional consequences and etiological underpinnings of these alterations will likely facilitate better prediction and targeted intervention in psychoses.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author: Matcheri S. Keshavan, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard Medical School, 75 Fenwood Rd, Boston, MA 02115 (maugusta@bidmc.harvard.edu).

Submitted for Publication: November 22, 2013; final revision received February 10, 2014; accepted March 6, 2014.

Published Online: May 14, 2014. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.453.

Author Contributions: Mr Mathew had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Clementz, Sweeney, Tamminga, Keshavan.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Mathew, Gardin, Tandon, Eack, Seidman, Clementz, Keshavan.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Mathew, Gardin, Tandon, Francis, Seidman, Clementz, Pearlson, Sweeney, Tamminga, Keshavan.

Statistical analysis: Mathew, Gardin, Tandon, Eack, Francis, Clementz, Keshavan.

Obtained funding: Pearlson, Sweeney, Tamminga, Keshavan.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Tandon, Francis, Seidman, Tamminga, Keshavan.

Study supervision: Pearlson, Sweeney, Tamminga, Keshavan.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Pearlson has served on an advisory panel for Bristol-Myers Squibb. Dr Sweeney has served on advisory boards for Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Pfizer, Roche, and Takeda and has received grant support from Janssen. Dr Tamminga has served on the advisory board for drug development for Intra-Cellular Therapies, Inc; has served as an ad hoc consultant for PureTech Ventures, Eli Lilly Pharmaceuticals, Sunovion, Astellas, Cypress Bioscience, and Merck; has served as an organizer and unpaid volunteer for the International Congress on Schizophrenia Research; has been a council member and unpaid volunteer for the National Alliance on Mental Illness; and has served as deputy editor for the American Psychiatric Association. Dr Keshavan has received research support from Sunovion and GlaxoSmithKline. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This research was supported in part by grants MH078113, MH077945, MH077852, MH077851, MH077862, MH072767, and MH083888 from the National Institute of Mental Health and grant SCDMH82101008006 from the Commonwealth Research Center.

Role of the Sponsor: The funders reviewed the manuscript but had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: Gunvant Thaker, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, Nashaat Boutros, MD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan, and Melvin McKinnis, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, provided collaboration on this project, and William Carpenter, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland, Baltimore, made helpful suggestions. They received no compensation from the funders for these contributions.

References
1.
Keshavan  MS, Clementz  BA, Pearlson  GD, Sweeney  JA, Tamminga  CA.  Reimagining psychoses: an agnostic approach to diagnosis.  Schizophr Res. 2013;146(1-3):10-16. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.02.022.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Kraepelin  E. Gedanken über die Arbeitscurve. In: Kraepelin  E, ed.  Psychologische Arbeiten. Berlin, Germany: Julius Springer; 1894:535-547.
3.
Nelson  MD, Saykin  AJ, Flashman  LA, Riordan  HJ.  Hippocampal volume reduction in schizophrenia as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging: a meta-analytic study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(5):433-440.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Steen  RG, Mull  C, McClure  R, Hamer  RM, Lieberman  JA.  Brain volume in first-episode schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies.  Br J Psychiatry. 2006;188:510-518. doi:10.1192/bjp.188.6.510.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Heckers  S, Heinsen  H, Geiger  B, Beckmann  H.  Hippocampal neuron number in schizophrenia: a stereological study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(11):1002-1008.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Swanson  LW, Köhler  C.  Anatomical evidence for direct projections from the entorhinal area to the entire cortical mantle in the rat.  J Neurosci. 1986;6(10):3010-3023.PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Arnold  OH.  Schizophrenia: a disturbance of signal interaction between the entorhinal cortex and the dentate gyrus? the contribution of experimental dibenamine psychosis to the pathogenesis of schizophrenia: a hypothesis.  Neuropsychobiology. 1999;40(1):21-32.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Keshavan  MS, Tandon  R, Boutros  NN, Nasrallah  HA.  Schizophrenia, “just the facts”: what we know in 2008 part 3: neurobiology.  Schizophr Res. 2008;106(2-3):89-107. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.07.020.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Strasser  HC, Lilyestrom  J, Ashby  ER,  et al.  Hippocampal and ventricular volumes in psychotic and nonpsychotic bipolar patients compared with schizophrenia patients and community control subjects: a pilot study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):633-639. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.009.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Szeszko  PR, Goldberg  E, Gunduz-Bruce  H,  et al.  Smaller anterior hippocampal formation volume in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(12):2190-2197.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Seidman  LJ, Pantelis  C, Keshavan  MS,  et al.  A review and new report of medial temporal lobe dysfunction as a vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric family study of the parahippocampal gyrus.  Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(4):803-830.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Pearlson  GD, Barta  PE, Powers  RE,  et al.  Ziskind-Somerfeld Research Award 1996: medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(1):1-14. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00373-3.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Narr  KL, Thompson  PM, Szeszko  P,  et al.  Regional specificity of hippocampal volume reductions in first-episode schizophrenia.  Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1563-1575. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.011.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Thermenos  HW, Keshavan  MS, Juelich  RJ,  et al.  A review of neuroimaging studies of young relatives of individuals with schizophrenia: a developmental perspective from schizotaxia to schizophrenia.  Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2013;162B(7):604-635. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32170.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Velakoulis  D, Wood  SJ, Wong  MTH,  et al.  Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(2):139-149. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.139.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Chakos  MH, Schobel  SA, Gu  H,  et al.  Duration of illness and treatment effects on hippocampal volume in male patients with schizophrenia.  Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:26-31. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.1.26.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Tamminga  CA, Stan  AD, Wagner  AD.  The hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(10):1178-1193. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081187.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Squire  LR, Zola-Morgan  S.  The medial temporal lobe memory system.  Science. 1991;253(5026):1380-1386.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Karnik-Henry  MS, Wang  L, Barch  DM, Harms  MP, Campanella  C, Csernansky  JG.  Medial temporal lobe structure and cognition in individuals with schizophrenia and in their non-psychotic siblings.  Schizophr Res. 2012;138(2-3):128-135. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.015.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cohen  NJ, Eichenbaum  H.  Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. Boston, MA: MIT Press; 1993.
21.
Davachi  L, Mitchell  JP, Wagner  AD.  Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(4):2157-2162. doi:10.1073/pnas.0337195100.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Aminoff  EM, Kveraga  K, Bar  M.  The role of the parahippocampal cortex in cognition.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(8):379-390. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.009.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Aleman  A, Hijman  R, de Haan  EHF, Kahn  RS.  Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.  Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1358-1366.PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Cirillo  MA, Seidman  LJ.  Verbal declarative memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: from clinical assessment to genetics and brain mechanisms.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2003;13(2):43-77.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
McDonald  C, Zanelli  J, Rabe-Hesketh  S,  et al.  Meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging brain morphometry studies in bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56(6):411-417. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.021.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Kempton  MJ, Geddes  JR, Ettinger  U, Williams  SCR, Grasby  PM.  Meta-analysis, database, and meta-regression of 98 structural imaging studies in bipolar disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(9):1017-1032. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.9.1017.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Hajek  T, Kopecek  M, Höschl  C, Alda  M.  Smaller hippocampal volumes in patients with bipolar disorder are masked by exposure to lithium: a meta-analysis.  J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2012;37(5):333-343. doi:10.1503/jpn.110143.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Van Leemput  K, Bakkour  A, Benner  T,  et al.  Automated segmentation of hippocampal subfields from ultra-high resolution in vivo MRI.  Hippocampus. 2009;19(6):549-557. doi:10.1002/hipo.20615.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Pfohl  B, Blum  N, Zimmermann  M.  Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality: SIDP-IV. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing; 1997.
30.
Skudlarski  P, Schretlen  DJ, Thaker  GK,  et al.  Diffusion tensor imaging white matter endophenotypes in patients with schizophrenia or psychotic bipolar disorder and their relatives.  Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(8):886-898. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12111448.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Andreasen  NC, Pressler  M, Nopoulos  P, Miller  D, Ho  BC.  Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs.  Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):255-262. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
First  MB, Spitzer  RL, Gibbon  M, Williams  JBW.  Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2012.
33.
Kay  SR, Fiszbein  A, Opler  LA.  The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261-276.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Montgomery  SA, Asberg  M.  A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change.  Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134:382-389.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Young  RC, Biggs  JT, Ziegler  VE, Meyer  DA.  A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.  Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Keefe  RSE, Goldberg  TE, Harvey  PD, Gold  JM, Poe  MP, Coughenour  L.  The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery.  Schizophr Res. 2004;68(2-3):283-297. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Desikan  RS, Ségonne  F, Fischl  B,  et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.  Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968-980. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Dixon  WJ, Tukey  JW.  Approximate behavior of the distribution of Winsorized t (Trimming/Winsorization 2).  Technometrics. 1968;10(1):83-98. doi:10.1080/00401706.1968.10490537.Google Scholar
39.
Hochberg  Y.  A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance.  Biometrika. 1988;75(4):800-802. doi:10.1093/biomet/75.4.800.Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Cohen  J.  Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
41.
Falkai  P, Bogerts  B.  Cell loss in the hippocampus of schizophrenics.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1986;236(3):154-161. doi:10.1007/BF00380943.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Harrison  PJ.  The hippocampus in schizophrenia: a review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;174(1):151-162. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1761-y.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Tamminga  CA, Holcomb  HH.  Phenotype of schizophrenia: a review and formulation.  Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(1):27-39. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001563.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Adriano  F, Caltagirone  C, Spalletta  G.  Hippocampal volume reduction in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: a review and meta-analysis.  Neuroscientist. 2012;18(2):180-200. doi:10.1177/1073858410395147.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Strakowski  SM, Delbello  MP, Adler  CM.  The functional neuroanatomy of bipolar disorder: a review of neuroimaging findings.  Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(1):105-116. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001585.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Kühn  S, Musso  F, Mobascher  A, Warbrick  T, Winterer  G, Gallinat  J.  Hippocampal subfields predict positive symptoms in schizophrenia: first evidence from brain morphometry.  Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e127. doi:10.1038/tp.2012.51.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
O’Reilly  RC, McClelland  JL.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off.  Hippocampus. 1994;4(6):661-682. doi:10.1002/hipo.450040605.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Tamminga  CA, Southcott  S, Sacco  C, Wagner  AD, Ghose  S.  Glutamate dysfunction in hippocampus: relevance of dentate gyrus and CA3 signaling.  Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(5):927-935. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs062.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Grace  AA.  Dopamine system dysregulation by the ventral subiculum as the common pathophysiological basis for schizophrenia psychosis, psychostimulant abuse, and stress.  Neurotox Res. 2010;18(3-4):367-376. doi:10.1007/s12640-010-9154-6.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Herman  JP, Mueller  NK.  Role of the ventral subiculum in stress integration.  Behav Brain Res. 2006;174(2):215-224. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2006.05.035.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
O’Mara  S.  The subiculum: what it does, what it might do, and what neuroanatomy has yet to tell us.  J Anat. 2005;207(3):271-282. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00446.x.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Sapolsky  RM.  Glucocorticoid toxicity in the hippocampus: temporal aspects of neuronal vulnerability.  Brain Res. 1985;359(1-2):300-305.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Lodge  DJ, Grace  AA.  Hippocampal dysregulation of dopamine system function and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.  Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2011;32(9):507-513. doi:10.1016/j.tips.2011.05.001.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Libby  LA, Ekstrom  AD, Ragland  JD, Ranganath  C.  Differential connectivity of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices within human hippocampal subregions revealed by high-resolution functional imaging.  J Neurosci. 2012;32(19):6550-6560. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-11.2012.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Harrison  PJ, Eastwood  SL.  Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.  Hippocampus. 2001;11(5):508-519. doi:10.1002/hipo.1067.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Heckers  S, Konradi  C.  Hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia.  Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:529-553. doi:10.1007/7854_2010_43.PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Keshavan  MS.  Development, disease and degeneration in schizophrenia: a unitary pathophysiological model.  J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33(6):513-521.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Cherlyn  SYT, Woon  PS, Liu  JJ, Ong  WY, Tsai  GC, Sim  K.  Genetic association studies of glutamate, GABA and related genes in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a decade of advance.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34(6):958-977. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.002.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Kraguljac  NV, White  DM, Reid  MA, Lahti  AC.  Increased hippocampal glutamate and volumetric deficits in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(12):1294-1302. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2437.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Harrison  PJ, Law  AJ, Eastwood  SL.  Glutamate receptors and transporters in the hippocampus in schizophrenia.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:94-101.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Kaymaz  N, van Os  J.  Heritability of structural brain traits an endophenotype approach to deconstruct schizophrenia.  Int Rev Neurobiol. 2009;89:85-130. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(09)89005-3.PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Francis  AN, Seidman  LJ, Tandon  N,  et al.  Reduced subicular subdivisions of the hippocampal formation and verbal declarative memory impairments in young relatives at risk for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1-3):154-157. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.10.002.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Meda  SA, Stevens  MC, Folley  BS, Calhoun  VD, Pearlson  GD.  Evidence for anomalous network connectivity during working memory encoding in schizophrenia: an ICA based analysis.  PLoS One. 2009;4(11):e7911. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007911.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Jovicich  J, Czanner  S, Han  X,  et al.  MRI-derived measurements of human subcortical, ventricular and intracranial brain volumes: reliability effects of scan sessions, acquisition sequences, data analyses, scanner upgrade, scanner vendors and field strengths.  Neuroimage. 2009;46(1):177-192. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.010.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×