[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Voxel-wise Brain Activation Group by Anticipation Interaction
Voxel-wise Brain Activation Group by Anticipation Interaction

Significant threshold for analysis of variance (ANOVA) clusters were determined at P < .05 (family-wise error corrected). The group-by-anticipation valence interaction found a significant cluster in the pregenual cingulate (pgCC) (F1,39 = 11.94, P = .001). Extracted contrast estimates from the pgCC cluster were used for post hoc comparisons and displayed in the histogram to the right. During the anticipation of losses, the low-risk group had significantly higher pgCC activation (P = .02) than the high-risk group. The high-risk group had significantly higher pgCC activation during anticipation of rewards compared with anticipation of losses (P = .02). Error bars indicate SE.

Figure 2.  Voxel-wise Brain Activation Group by Feedback Interaction
Voxel-wise Brain Activation Group by Feedback Interaction

Significant threshold for analysis of variance (ANOVA) clusters were determined at P < .05 (family-wise error corrected). The group by feedback valence interaction found a significant cluster in the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex (F1,39 = 23.28, P < .001). Extracted contrast estimates from the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex were used for post hoc comparisons and displayed in the histogram to the right. During the feedback of successful rewards, the high-risk group had higher activation in this region than the low-risk group (P = .004), whereas during the feedback of losses, the high-risk group had lower activation than the low-risk group (P = .001) in this region. The high-risk group had greater activation during feedback of rewards compared with losses (P = .002). The low-risk group had lower activation during feedback of rewards compared with losses (P = .001). Error bars indicate SE.

Figure 3.  Psychophysiologic Interaction (PPI) Pregenual Cingulate (pgCC) Connectivity Group by Anticipation Interaction
Psychophysiologic Interaction (PPI) Pregenual Cingulate (pgCC) Connectivity Group by Anticipation Interaction

The PPI analysis was conducted seeding the pgCC during anticipation. The pgCC seed along with an arrow indicating connectivity is displayed in green. Significant threshold for analysis of variance (ANOVA) clusters were determined at P < .05 (family-wise error corrected). The PPI group by anticipation interaction found a significant cluster in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) (F1,39 = 20.04, P < .001). Extracted connectivity estimates from the right VLPFC were used for post hoc comparisons and displayed in the histogram to the right. The pgCC connectivity associated with anticipation of rewards had lower right VLPFC connectivity in the high-risk group compared with the low-risk group (P < .001), whereas the high-risk group had higher connectivity compared with the low-risk group during anticipation of losses (P = .005). Error bars indicate SE.

Table.  Significant Clusters of Activation Using a 2-Way ANOVA
Significant Clusters of Activation Using a 2-Way ANOVA
1.
Perlis  RH, Dennehy  EB, Miklowitz  DJ,  et al.  Retrospective age at onset of bipolar disorder and outcome during two-year follow-up: results from the STEP-BD study.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(4):391-400.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Johnson  SL.  Mania and dysregulation in goal pursuit: a review.  Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25(2):241-262.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Galvan  A, Hare  TA, Parra  CE,  et al.  Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents.  J Neurosci. 2006;26(25):6885-6892.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Goodwin  FK, Jamison  KR.  Manic-Depressive Illness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1990.
5.
Pizzagalli  DA, Goetz  E, Ostacher  M, Iosifescu  DV, Perlis  RH.  Euthymic patients with bipolar disorder show decreased reward learning in a probabilistic reward task.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(2):162-168.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Mueller  SC, Ng  P, Temple  V,  et al.  Perturbed reward processing in pediatric bipolar disorder: an antisaccade study.  J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24(12):1779-1784.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Gorrindo  T, Blair  RJR, Budhani  S, Dickstein  DP, Pine  DS, Leibenluft  E.  Deficits on a probabilistic response-reversal task in patients with pediatric bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1975-1977.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Rich  BA, Schmajuk  M, Perez-Edgar  KE, Pine  DS, Fox  NA, Leibenluft  E.  The impact of reward, punishment, and frustration on attention in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(7):532-539.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Mason  L, O’Sullivan  N, Blackburn  M, Bentall  R, El-Deredy  W.  I want it now! neural correlates of hypersensitivity to immediate reward in hypomania.  Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(6):530-537.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Ernst  M, Dickstein  DP, Munson  S,  et al.  Reward-related processes in pediatric bipolar disorder: a pilot study.  J Affect Disord. 2004;82(suppl 1):S89-S101.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Nusslock  R, Almeida  JR, Forbes  EE,  et al.  Waiting to win: elevated striatal and orbitofrontal cortical activity during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder adults.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(3):249-260.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Abler  B, Greenhouse  I, Ongur  D, Walter  H, Heckers  S.  Abnormal reward system activation in mania.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(9):2217-2227.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Bermpohl  F, Kahnt  T, Dalanay  U,  et al.  Altered representation of expected value in the orbitofrontal cortex in mania.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(7):958-969.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Bebko  G, Bertocci  MA, Fournier  JC,  et al.  Parsing dimensional vs diagnostic category-related patterns of reward circuitry function in behaviorally and emotionally dysregulated youth in the Longitudinal Assessment of Manic Symptoms study.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(1):71-80.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Singh  MK, Chang  KD, Kelley  RG,  et al.  Reward processing in adolescents with bipolar I disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(1):68-83.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Wessa  M, Kanske  P, Linke  J.  Bipolar disorder: a neural network perspective on a disorder of emotion and motivation.  Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32(1):51-62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Linke  J, King  AV, Rietschel  M,  et al.  Increased medial orbitofrontal and amygdala activation: evidence for a systems-level endophenotype of bipolar I disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(3):316-325.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sripada  CS, Gonzalez  R, Phan  KL, Liberzon  I.  The neural correlates of intertemporal decision-making: contributions of subjective value, stimulus type, and trait impulsivity.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(10):1637-1648.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Cohen  JR, Asarnow  RF, Sabb  FW,  et al.  A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors.  Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(6):669-671.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Lombardo  LE, Bearden  CE, Barrett  J,  et al.  Trait impulsivity as an endophenotype for bipolar I disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(5):565-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Giovanelli  A, Hoerger  M, Johnson  SL, Gruber  J.  Impulsive responses to positive mood and reward are related to mania risk. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(6):1091-1104.PubMed
22.
Swann  AC, Lijffijt  M, Lane  SD, Steinberg  JL, Moeller  FG.  Increased trait-like impulsivity and course of illness in bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(3):280-288.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Mathias de Almeida  K, Nery  FG, Moreno  RA, Gorenstein  C, Lafer  B.  A sib-pair analysis of impulsivity in bipolar disorder type I.  Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54(8):1148-1152.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Chang  KD, Blasey  CM, Ketter  TA, Steiner  H.  Temperament characteristics of child and adolescent bipolar offspring.  J Affect Disord. 2003;77(1):11-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Knutson  B, Westdorp  A, Kaiser  E, Hommer  D.  FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task.  Neuroimage. 2000;12(1):20-27.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Gotlib  IH, Hamilton  JP, Cooney  RE, Singh  MK, Henry  ML, Joormann  J.  Neural processing of reward and loss in girls at risk for major depression.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):380-387.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Knutson  B, Fong  GW, Adams  CM, Varner  JL, Hommer  D.  Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI.  Neuroreport. 2001;12(17):3683-3687.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Knutson  B, Taylor  J, Kaufman  M, Peterson  R, Glover  G.  Distributed neural representation of expected value.  J Neurosci. 2005;25(19):4806-4812.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Haber  SN, Knutson  B.  The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):4-26.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Young  RC, Biggs  JT, Ziegler  VE, Meyer  DA.  A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.  Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Poznanski  EO, Grossman  JA, Buchsbaum  Y, Banegas  M, Freeman  L, Gibbons  R.  Preliminary studies of the reliability and validity of the Children’s Depression Rating Scale.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23(2):191-197.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
March  JS, Parker  JD, Sullivan  K, Stallings  P, Conners  CK.  The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): factor structure, reliability, and validity.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):554-565.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Shaffer  D, Gould  MS, Brasic  J,  et al.  A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS).  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(11):1228-1231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Cosi  S, Vigil-Colet  A, Canals  J, Lorenzo-Seva  U.  Psychometric properties of the Spanish adaptation of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-A for children.  Psychol Rep. 2008;103(2):336-346.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Windle  M, Lerner  R.  Reassessing the dimensions of temperament individuality across the life span: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R).  J Adolesc Res. 1986;1:213-229.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Cirino  PT, Chin  CE, Sevcik  RA, Wolf  M, Lovett  M, Morris  RD.  Measuring socioeconomic status: reliability and preliminary validity for different approaches.  Assessment. 2002;9(2):145-155.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Carskadon  MA, Acebo  C.  A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development.  J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190-195.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Psychological Corporation.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Co; 1999.
39.
Crovitz  HF, Zener  K.  A group-test for assessing hand- and eye-dominance.  Am J Psychol. 1962;75:271-276.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Cohen  MX, Heller  AS, Ranganath  C.  Functional connectivity with anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortices during decision-making.  Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;23(1):61-70.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Friston  KJ, Buechel  C, Fink  GR, Morris  J, Rolls  E, Dolan  RJ.  Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging.  Neuroimage. 1997;6(3):218-229.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
O’Sullivan  N, Szczepanowski  R, El-Deredy  W, Mason  L, Bentall  RP.  fMRI evidence of a relationship between hypomania and both increased goal-sensitivity and positive outcome-expectancy bias.  Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2825-2835.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Ernst  M, Nelson  EE, Jazbec  S,  et al.  Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents.  Neuroimage. 2005;25(4):1279-1291.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Ibanez  A, Cetkovich  M, Petroni  A,  et al.  The neural basis of decision-making and reward processing in adults with euthymic bipolar disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).  PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37306.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Amemori  K, Graybiel  AM.  Localized microstimulation of primate pregenual cingulate cortex induces negative decision-making.  Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):776-785.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Young  KD, Bellgowan  PSF, Bodurka  J, Drevets  WC.  Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in patients with depression and individuals at high risk for depression.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(7):698-708.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Mannie  ZN, Norbury  R, Murphy  SE, Inkster  B, Harmer  CJ, Cowen  PJ.  Affective modulation of anterior cingulate cortex in young people at increased familial risk of depression.  Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192(5):356-361.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Elliott  R, Dolan  RJ, Frith  CD.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies.  Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):308-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Urošević  S, Collins  P, Muetzel  R, Lim  K, Luciana  M.  Longitudinal changes in behavioral approach system sensitivity and brain structures involved in reward processing during adolescence.  Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1488-1500.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Albrecht  K, Volz  KG, Sutter  M, von Cramon  DY.  What do I want and when do I want it: brain correlates of decisions made for self and other.  PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e73531.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Longe  O, Senior  C, Rippon  G.  The lateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex work as a dynamic integrated system: evidence from FMRI connectivity analysis.  J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21(1):141-154.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Mitchell  DGV.  The nexus between decision making and emotion regulation: a review of convergent neurocognitive substrates.  Behav Brain Res. 2011;217(1):215-231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Greening  SG, Osuch  EA, Williamson  PC, Mitchell  DGV.  The neural correlates of regulating positive and negative emotions in medication-free major depression.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(5):628-637.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Morris  RW, Sparks  A, Mitchell  PB, Weickert  CS, Green  MJ.  Lack of cortico-limbic coupling in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia during emotion regulation.  Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e90.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Passarotti  AM, Ellis  J, Wegbreit  E, Stevens  MC, Pavuluri  MN.  Reduced functional connectivity of prefrontal regions and amygdala within affect and working memory networks in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Brain Connect. 2012;2(6):320-334.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Ladouceur  CD, Diwadkar  VA, White  R,  et al.  Fronto-limbic function in unaffected offspring at familial risk for bipolar disorder during an emotional working memory paradigm.  Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5:185-196.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Townsend  JD, Torrisi  SJ, Lieberman  MD, Sugar  CA, Bookheimer  SY, Altshuler  LL.  Frontal-amygdala connectivity alterations during emotion downregulation in bipolar I disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(2):127-135.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Price  JL, Drevets  WC.  Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(1):61-71.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Brotman  MA, Tseng  W-L, Olsavsky  AK,  et al.  Fronto-limbic-striatal dysfunction in pediatric and adult patients with bipolar disorder: impact of face emotion and attentional demands.  Psychol Med. 2014;44(8):1639-1651.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Yang  H, Lu  LH, Wu  M,  et al.  Time course of recovery showing initial prefrontal cortex changes at 16 weeks, extending to subcortical changes by 3 years in pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2013;150(2):571-577.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Simmons  RK, Howard  JL, Simpson  DN, Akil  H, Clinton  SM.  DNA methylation in the developing hippocampus and amygdala of anxiety-prone versus risk-taking rats.  Dev Neurosci. 2012;34(1):58-67.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Krebs  RM, Schott  BH, Düzel  E.  Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(2):103-110.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Eldreth  D, Hardin  MG, Pavletic  N, Ernst  M.  Adolescent transformations of behavioral and neural processes as potential targets for prevention.  Prev Sci. 2013;14(3):257-266.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
October 2014

Reward Processing in Healthy Offspring of Parents With Bipolar Disorder

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California
  • 2Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, California
JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(10):1148-1156. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1031
Abstract

Importance  Bipolar disorder (BD) is highly familial and characterized by deficits in reward processing. It is not known, however, whether these deficits precede illness onset or are a consequence of the disorder.

Objective  To determine whether anomalous neural processing of reward characterizes children at familial risk for BD in the absence of a personal history of a psychopathologic disorder.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This study compared neural activity and behaviors of children at high and low risk for mania while they anticipate and respond to reward and loss. The study was performed from September 15, 2009, through February 17, 2012, in a university functional magnetic resonance imaging facility and included 8- to 15-year-old children without disorders born to a parent with BD (n = 20 high-risk children) and demographically matched healthy comparison children (n = 25 low-risk children).

Main Outcomes and Measures  Neural activity, as measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging, during anticipation and receipt of reward and loss during a monetary incentive delay task.

Results  While anticipating losses, high-risk children had less activation in the pregenual cingulate than did their low-risk counterparts (t19 = −2.44, P = .02). When receiving rewards, high-risk children had greater activation in the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex than did low-risk children (t43 = −3.04, P = .004). High-risk children also had weaker functional connectivity between the pregenual cingulate and the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex while anticipating rewards than did low-risk children (t19 = −4.38, P < .001) but had a stronger connectivity between these regions while anticipating losses (t24 = 2.76, P = .01). Finally, in high- but not low-risk children, novelty seeking was associated with increased striatal and amygdalar activation in the anticipation of losses, and impulsivity was associated with increased striatal and insula activation in the receipt of rewards.

Conclusions and Relevance  Aberrant prefrontal activations and connectivities during reward processing suggest mechanisms that underlie early vulnerabilities for developing dysfunctional regulation of goal pursuit and motivation in children at high risk for mania. Longitudinal studies are needed to examine whether these patterns of neural activation predict the onset of mania and other mood disorders in high-risk children.

Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is a debilitating disorder of motivational functioning that commonly begins during adolescence.1 Manic states of BD are typically characterized by increased risk-taking, novelty seeking, and impulsivity.2 Clinicians are frequently challenged to distinguish children who exhibit normal variants of adolescent behavior3 from children who engage in maladaptive forms of reward processing that are associated with mania. Given that the strongest risk factor for developing mania is a family history of BD,4 disturbances in core reward processing in children at familial risk for BD may provide a basis for understanding the origins of manic symptoms.2 Few studies, however, have examined the neural aspects of these aberrations, particularly in young offspring of parents with BD who may be predisposed to reward dysfunction even before the onset of mania.

Adults5 and children6,7 with BD have impaired reward learning, increased reward reactivity and greater arousal in reward conditions,8 greater attentional bias toward immediate rewards,9 and greater satisfaction with winning.10 Neuroimaging studies of reward processing in BD have had mixed results. Although one study11 in adults with BD found expected increases in activation in the ventral striatum during reward anticipation, another study12 found reduced activation in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) on receipt of rewards. Other studies11,13,14 report increased prefrontal activations that may serve to regulate anticipation and response to reward. Singh et al15 found that adolescents with BD have decreased activation in the thalamus and inferior temporal gyrus while anticipating rewards and increased activations in the middle frontal gyrus and parietal cortices while anticipating losses. These studies suggest that adolescents and adults with BD have discordant behavioral and neural responses to reward, including enhanced motivation for seeking rewards and aberrant estimation of risks and punishments. However, these studies are confounded by comorbidities, medication exposures, and variable mood states. Emerging evidence also suggests that a neural network model provides the most comprehensive understanding of reward function in BD16; however, to our knowledge, no studies in BD have examined the connectivities among key regions during reward processing. Additional studies are clearly needed to gain a better understanding of dysfunctional reward processing in the development of mania.

It is not clear whether previously reported neurobehavioral patterns of reward response reflect a developmental process that is more typical of children than adults, play an etiologic role in BD, or are a consequence of multiple mood episodes or medications. Another study17 examined whether anomalous neural processing of rewards is a trait feature found in families with BD. In individuals with BD and their relatives, reward-related increases in activations were found in the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and were associated with heightened sensitivity in response to reward and deficient prediction error signaling.17 These findings raise the intriguing possibility that impaired reward processing represents an early risk factor for developing BD and is a potential therapeutic target even in the absence of overt symptoms. Indeed, investigators have linked aberrant reward processing to trait impulsivity18 and to approach or novelty-seeking behaviors,19 characteristics that have been posited to be associated with BD20,21 and can lead to a more severe illness course.22 No study, to our knowledge, however, has examined the neural correlates of reward processing in young offspring without disorders born to parents with BD; these children may be at risk for trait impulsivity23 and novelty-seeking24 behaviors before the onset of mania.21

The aim of the present study was to examine neural activations associated with reward processing in young offspring without disorders born to parents with BD. We used a monetary incentive delay task25 that has been used in children with and at risk for mood disorders15,26 and that reliably activates key reward neural circuitry, including medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices, OFC, dorsal and pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, and ventral striatum, during anticipation and receipt of rewards.27-29 On the basis of prior literature,11-15,17 we predicted that, compared with typically developing children at low risk for developing mania, offspring of parents with BD would have aberrations in frontostriatal activation and connectivity while processing rewards and losses. Given the likely relations between impulsivity and mania risk21 and between novelty seeking and mania risk,24 we also predicted that those high-risk participants with higher levels of trait impulsivity and novelty seeking would have greater activations in reward-related regions while processing rewards.

Methods
Participants

A total of 45 children 8 through 15 years of age with no current or past Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition, Text Revision) Axis I disorder participated in the study after they and their parents gave their assent and informed consent according to institutional guidelines for the protection of human subjects at Stanford University. Twenty children had one biological parent diagnosed as having bipolar I disorder (high risk), and 25 children had biological parents and first- and second-degree relatives with no history of any Axis I disorder (low risk). From September 15, 2009, through February 17, 2012, eligible children completed more extensive interviews and testing after parental written informed consent and child written assent (eMethods in the Supplement).

Assessment of Psychiatric Health

All participants were evaluated by semistructured clinical interviews by raters (M.K.S. and M.E.H.) masked to family history status and with established symptom and diagnostic reliability (κ>0.9) to rule out current and lifetime psychopathologic disorders (eMethods in the Supplement). To ensure that the 2 groups did not differ in levels of mania or depression in the absence of any psychiatric diagnoses, all youth were interviewed using the Young Mania Rating Scale30 and the Children’s Depression Rating Scale–Revised.31 Levels of anxiety were assessed by administering the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children32 to the parents. Global functioning was determined by the Children’s Global Assessment Scale.33 Level of trait impulsivity was assessed by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale,15,34 which yielded attentional, motor, and nonplanning subscales. The Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey35 was completed by all parents during euthymia about their offspring’s temperament. We focused on the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey approach-withdrawal score, which indexes the degree of novelty seeking (high scorers tend to approach or move toward new persons, objects, situations, or events) that may influence reward-related neural circuitry.24 Age, sex, socioeconomic status,36 pubertal stage,37 IQ,38 and handedness39 were also assessed.

Statistical Analysis

We administered the monetary incentive delay task25 during functional magnetic resonance imaging to participants, recording reaction time and accuracy on each trial. The monetary incentive delay task probes neural responses to anticipation and receipt of gains and losses using a set of cues to indicate whether participants can win or avoid losing money if they respond quickly enough to a target that follows a cue and anticipation period. Task design, functional magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition, preprocessing, and statistical analyses are detailed in the eMethods and eFigure in the Supplement.

Using a fixed-effects model in SPM8 statistical software (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging), we computed statistical contrasts for anticipation and feedback phases of reward and loss. For anticipation, we compared trials with reward or loss cues to corresponding nonreward and nonloss trials. For feedback, we compared trials in which participants gained money to nonreward feedback trials, and we compared trials in which participants avoided losing money to nonloss trials. To examine group differences in brain activation during reward processing, we conducted a 2-way (group [high risk or low risk] by valence [reward or loss]) voxel-wise analysis of covariance for anticipation and feedback contrasts after adjusting for Young Mania Rating Scale, Children’s Depression Rating Scale–Revised, and Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children scores (P < .05, family-wise error [FWE] corrected).

We used a psychophysiologic interaction analysis in SPM8 to evaluate functional connectivity between reward-related regions of interest from our voxel-wise analysis and the rest of the brain.40 We used 2-sample t tests to identify significant group differences in connectivity with the seed region at a cluster-level threshold (P < .05, FWE corrected) with a height threshold of P < .01 uncorrected.41

Finally, we explored within-group correlations to examine associations among neural activations in the bilateral amygdala, insula, and NAcc regions of interest during reward processing across all conditions and trait impulsivity and novelty seeking. These regions of interest were selected based on reward findings in BD11,17,42 and from the existing reward literature.28,43 The region of interest significance levels were Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons (0.05/3 = .02), and we used Fisher r-to-z transformations to determine whether the high- and low-risk groups differed significantly with respect to these within-group correlations.

Results
Participant Characteristics

Demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in the eTable in the Supplement. High-risk and low-risk children did not differ significantly with respect to age (P = .22), sex (P = .42), handedness (P = .23), IQ (P = .23), Young Mania Rating Scale score (P = .74), Children’s Depression Rating Scale–Revised score (P = .55), Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children score (P = .66), socioeconomic status (P = .66), or Tanner stage (P = .17).

Compared with low-risk children, high-risk children had lower Children’s Global Assessment Scale scores (t43 = 2.65, P = .01) and higher Barratt Impulsiveness Scale impulsivity subscale scores (P = .16 for the motor subscale, P = .60 for the nonplanning subscale, and P = .58 for the attention subscale), but the groups did not differ significantly with respect to these subscales. High-risk children also had higher Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey approach-withdrawal scores (t43 = 2.68, P = .01), reflecting higher levels of novelty seeking. Between-group imaging results remained significant after covarying for these characteristics.

Behavioral Results

Two-way group-by-valence analyses of covariance of reaction times and accuracy yielded no significant main effects or interactions (P = .64, .65, .50, .38, .18, and .44 for reaction time as the main effect of group, reaction time as the main effect of valence, reaction time group-by-valence interaction, accuracy as the main effect of group, accuracy as the main effect of valence, and accuracy group-by-valence interaction, respectively) (eTable in the Supplement).

Voxel-wise Neuroimaging Results

Two-way group-by-valence analyses of variance compared voxel-wise activity of high-risk and low-risk participants in response to rewards and losses during anticipation and feedback conditions (P = .05, FWE corrected); significant effects were followed by within- and between-group post hoc t tests. Voxel-wise main effects of group, main effects of valence, and the interaction between group and valence during anticipation and feedback conditions are presented in the Table.

Anticipation

A significant interaction of group and valence was found in the pregenual cingulate during anticipation (F1,39 = 11.94, P = .001). Whereas low-risk children had greater pregenual cingulate activation during anticipation of loss than during anticipation of reward (t24 = –2.04, P = .05), high-risk children had the opposite result, with less activation in the same region during anticipation of loss than during anticipation of reward (t19 = −2.44, P = .02) (Figure 1). Low-risk children had greater pregenual cingulate activation than did high-risk children during the anticipation of loss (t43 = 2.35, P = .02) (Figure 1).

Feedback

A significant interaction of group and valence was found in the left lateral OFC during feedback (F1,39 = 23.28, P < .001) and in the bilateral OFC at a lower threshold (P < .01, uncorrected). Whereas high-risk children had greater left lateral OFC activation during feedback of successful rewards than during feedback of avoided losses (t19 = 3.68, P = .002), low-risk children had less activation in the same region during successful rewards than during avoided losses (t24 = −3.72, P = .001) (Figure 2). Moreover, high-risk children had greater left lateral OFC activation in response to successful rewards than did low-risk children (t43 = −3.04, P = .004); in contrast, low-risk children had greater activation in response to avoided losses in the same region than did high-risk children (t43 = 3.56, P = .001) (Figure 2).

Functional Connectivity Results

Group and valence effects and interactions of voxel-wise connectivity with the pregenual cingulate during anticipation and with the left lateral OFC during feedback are presented in the Table.

Anticipation

Connectivity with the pregenual cingulate during anticipation yielded a significant interaction in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) (F1,39 = 20.04, P < .001). Whereas high-risk children had less connectivity between the pregenual cingulate and right VLPFC during anticipation of reward than during anticipation of loss (t19 = –4.38, P < .001), low-risk children had greater connectivity between the same regions during anticipation of reward than during anticipation of loss (t24 = 2.76, P = .01). In addition, high-risk children had significantly greater connectivity between the pregenual cingulate and right VLPFC than did low-risk children during anticipation of loss (t43 = −2.94, P = .005) and weaker connectivity between the same regions during anticipation of gain (t43 = 4.49, P < .001) (Figure 3).

Feedback

No significant group or valence effects were found.

Correlations

Within high-risk children, significant positive correlations were found between Barratt Impulsiveness Scale attentional impulsivity and activations while receiving rewards in the NAcc (r = 0.62, P = .005) and in bilateral insula (r = 0.68, P = .001); these correlations were not found in low-risk children (Fisher r-to-z transformations: z = 2.82 for NAcc and 2.84 for insula; P = .005 for both). In high-risk children, there were significant positive correlations between increased novelty seeking and activation in the NAcc (r = 0.59, P = .006) and in bilateral amygdala (r = 0.57, P = .009) while anticipating losses; these correlations were not evident in low-risk children (Fisher r-to-z transformations: z = 1.83, P = .03, for NAcc; z = 1.75, P = .04, for amygdala).

Discussion

Aberrant reward function may be a critical vulnerability factor for developing mania. In the current study, we documented empirical support for our hypotheses that, compared with their low-risk peers, children without disorders born to parents with BD have aberrant neural responses to reward, aberrant connectivities among reward-related regions, and neural correlates in mesolimbic regions to noveltyseeking and impulsive traits, all of which may contribute to an increased risk of developing mania.

Neural activations in response to reward and loss found in high-risk children in the current study are consistent with previous investigations in patients with and at risk for BD, with some notable differences. Decreased activation in the pregenual cingulate during loss anticipation in high-risk children parallels deficits found in cingulate function during reward anticipation in euthymic adults with BD44 and during reward feedback in healthy offspring of mothers with depression.26 The pregenual cingulate typically functions in the regulation of emotion and to weigh cost against benefit in situations that require approach-avoidance decision-making.45 Thus, reduced pregenual cingulate activation in high-risk youth may represent a neurobiological vulnerability that predisposes high-risk children to impaired hedonic function.46,47

When high-risk children received feedback about receiving rewards, they activated the lateral OFC to a greater degree than did low-risk children, indicating an exaggerated prefrontal response during reward outcome. In healthy individuals, the OFC is involved in monitoring reward values, and the lateral OFC is especially likely to be activated when a response previously associated with a reward has to be suppressed,48 supporting its regulatory or inhibitory control function. Adults with mania have significant increases in activation in the left lateral OFC (Brodmann areas 11 and 47) while anticipating increasing rewards11,13 but not during reward outcome. Activation in the OFC, but in different conditions (anticipation vs outcome), may occur because we assessed children in this study before they experienced the typical increases in reward sensitivity during anticipation that are first observed in adolescence.49 Alternatively, increased activation in the lateral OFC during reward outcome in the high-risk offspring may represent an immature or maladaptive engagement in immediate gratification.50 Distinguishing between this explanation and an account that suggests a regulatory response by the lateral OFC requires further investigation of how the prefrontal cortex is functionally connected during reward processing.

In our examination of prefrontal functional connectivity, we found that high-risk children aberrantly regulate their affective responses to reward. Specifically, compared with low-risk children, high-risk children had weaker connectivity between the pregenual cingulate and VLPFC during reward anticipation but stronger connectivity between these regions during anticipation of loss, suggesting impaired regulation of affect while anticipating rewards29,51 but excessive regulation while anticipating losses. The VLPFC may be functioning in synchrony with the pregenual cingulate to regulate emotional response to loss or to reinforce inhibitory control in the context of deficient pregenual cingulate activation while anticipating losses to facilitate optimum behavior.52,53 In contrast, VLPFC dysfunction may decrease pregenual cingulate–VLPFC connectivity, resulting in a failure to regulate emotion during reward anticipation. This explanation is consistent with other studies in individuals with54,55 and at risk56 for BD that have found VLPFC dysfunction to be associated with emotional dysregulation and mood shifts characteristic of BD.57

In addition, high- and low-risk groups did not differ in ventrostriatal activation during reward anticipation, suggesting a lack of differential regard for reward magnitude during this condition12,13,28 but exaggerated prefrontal regulatory control during reward outcome.49 These findings suggest that the affective component of reward that relates to reward magnitude is less relevant to high-risk children than is prefrontally mediated reward probability and regulation. Furthermore, prefrontal dysfunction occurs in the absence of any symptoms and may precede striatal and limbic dysfunction commonly associated with BD.58,59 This finding has important implications for treatment, particularly with early-onset BD.60

High-risk children had higher levels of trait novelty seeking than did low-risk children, suggesting heightened reward sensitivity, and trends for elevated attention, motor, and nonplanning impulsivity compared with low-risk children. In high-risk children, trait novelty seeking was associated with increased striatal and amygdalar activation during loss anticipation, and trait impulsivity was associated with increased striatal and insula activation during receipt of rewards. Excessive striatal activations with reward value (anticipation) and prediction error (outcome) in children with higher trait novelty seeking parallel a study42 in adults with hypomania and suggest a mania-related enhanced perception of the value of goals that may lead to reward. Rodent models suggest that amygdala hyperactivity with novelty seeking represents a developmental vulnerability toward psychopathologic disorders.61 Finally, insula hyperactivity in response to reward outcome in more impulsive high-risk children is consistent with a bias toward an expectation of positive outcomes in decision-making situations.42 Together, these findings suggest that high-risk children with high trait novelty seeking and impulsivity have enhanced perception and representation of goal value coupled with a positive outcome expectancy bias, which could increase their risk of developing mania-related insatiable and indiscriminate reward seeking.

We should note a number of study limitations. First, we had a modest sample size; nevertheless, we found robust activation differences between groups. Second, to minimize motion artifact, only 9 replications of each trial type were presented, which may have reduced power to obtain significant effects. Third, our cross-sectional design without a bipolar comparison group did not allow us to determine whether findings in the high-risk group represented neural vulnerability (risk) vs neural adaptation (resilience). We tried to avoid comparisons in children who are in grossly different developmental stages on the bipolar continuum because of the potential for confounding from age, medication exposure, comorbidities, or mood state. Prospective studies are needed to determine reward-related vulnerabilities that predict clinical outcome. Fourth, self-report and parent questionnaires, rather than laboratory procedures, were used to assess impulsivity and temperament in children. Although there is a rich literature on how these traits impair neural responses to reward,18,62,63 few studies have directly examined their effect on neural predispositions for mania. Our study is the first, to our knowledge, to demonstrate the influence of trait novelty seeking and impulsivity on neural response to reward in children at risk for BD.

Conclusions

In this study, we present evidence that children without disorders born to parents with BD exhibit anomalous prefrontal function during reward processing that may represent a biomarker for developing mania. Future studies should examine the longitudinal trajectory of this dysfunction and its ability to predict the clinical onset of mania. Such research may facilitate the development of intervention strategies that use adaptive reward responses that could prevent the onset of mania.

Back to top
Article Information

Submitted for Publication: December 6, 2013; final revision received April 30, 2014; accepted May 6, 2014.

Corresponding Author: Manpreet K. Singh, MD, MS, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, 401 Quarry Rd, Stanford, CA 94305 (mksingh@stanford.edu).

Published Online: August 20, 2014. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1031.

Author Contributions: Dr Singh had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Singh, Howe, Reiss, Gotlib, Chang.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Singh, Kelley, Chang.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Singh, Howe, Reiss, Gotlib, Chang.

Statistical analysis: Singh, Kelley, Gotlib.

Obtained funding: Singh.

Administrative, technical, or material support: All authors.

Study supervision: Singh, Reiss, Gotlib, Chang.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Drs Singh and Gotlib reported receiving research funding from the National Institute of Mental Health. Dr Reiss reported serving as a consultant for Novartis and receiving research funding from the National Institutes of Health. Dr Chang reported receiving research funding from GlaxoSmithKline, Merck, National Institute of Mental Health, and Brain and Behavior Research Foundation and serving as an unpaid consultant for GlaxoSmithKline, Eli Lilly and Company, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Merck, and Sunovion. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This research was supported by grant MH085919 from the National Institute of Mental Health (Dr Singh).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding source had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: Elizabeth Adams, MSN, Rosie Shoemaker, BA, Erica Marie Sanders, BA, and Jennifer Kallini, BS, assisted with assessment, recruitment, data collection, and data entry. Mss Adams, Shoemaker, and Sanders were paid research assistants funded by the grant from the National Institute of Mental Health.

References
1.
Perlis  RH, Dennehy  EB, Miklowitz  DJ,  et al.  Retrospective age at onset of bipolar disorder and outcome during two-year follow-up: results from the STEP-BD study.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(4):391-400.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Johnson  SL.  Mania and dysregulation in goal pursuit: a review.  Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25(2):241-262.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Galvan  A, Hare  TA, Parra  CE,  et al.  Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents.  J Neurosci. 2006;26(25):6885-6892.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Goodwin  FK, Jamison  KR.  Manic-Depressive Illness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1990.
5.
Pizzagalli  DA, Goetz  E, Ostacher  M, Iosifescu  DV, Perlis  RH.  Euthymic patients with bipolar disorder show decreased reward learning in a probabilistic reward task.  Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(2):162-168.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Mueller  SC, Ng  P, Temple  V,  et al.  Perturbed reward processing in pediatric bipolar disorder: an antisaccade study.  J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24(12):1779-1784.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Gorrindo  T, Blair  RJR, Budhani  S, Dickstein  DP, Pine  DS, Leibenluft  E.  Deficits on a probabilistic response-reversal task in patients with pediatric bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(10):1975-1977.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Rich  BA, Schmajuk  M, Perez-Edgar  KE, Pine  DS, Fox  NA, Leibenluft  E.  The impact of reward, punishment, and frustration on attention in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(7):532-539.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Mason  L, O’Sullivan  N, Blackburn  M, Bentall  R, El-Deredy  W.  I want it now! neural correlates of hypersensitivity to immediate reward in hypomania.  Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(6):530-537.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Ernst  M, Dickstein  DP, Munson  S,  et al.  Reward-related processes in pediatric bipolar disorder: a pilot study.  J Affect Disord. 2004;82(suppl 1):S89-S101.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Nusslock  R, Almeida  JR, Forbes  EE,  et al.  Waiting to win: elevated striatal and orbitofrontal cortical activity during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder adults.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(3):249-260.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Abler  B, Greenhouse  I, Ongur  D, Walter  H, Heckers  S.  Abnormal reward system activation in mania.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(9):2217-2227.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Bermpohl  F, Kahnt  T, Dalanay  U,  et al.  Altered representation of expected value in the orbitofrontal cortex in mania.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(7):958-969.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Bebko  G, Bertocci  MA, Fournier  JC,  et al.  Parsing dimensional vs diagnostic category-related patterns of reward circuitry function in behaviorally and emotionally dysregulated youth in the Longitudinal Assessment of Manic Symptoms study.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(1):71-80.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Singh  MK, Chang  KD, Kelley  RG,  et al.  Reward processing in adolescents with bipolar I disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(1):68-83.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Wessa  M, Kanske  P, Linke  J.  Bipolar disorder: a neural network perspective on a disorder of emotion and motivation.  Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32(1):51-62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Linke  J, King  AV, Rietschel  M,  et al.  Increased medial orbitofrontal and amygdala activation: evidence for a systems-level endophenotype of bipolar I disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(3):316-325.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Sripada  CS, Gonzalez  R, Phan  KL, Liberzon  I.  The neural correlates of intertemporal decision-making: contributions of subjective value, stimulus type, and trait impulsivity.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(10):1637-1648.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Cohen  JR, Asarnow  RF, Sabb  FW,  et al.  A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors.  Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(6):669-671.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Lombardo  LE, Bearden  CE, Barrett  J,  et al.  Trait impulsivity as an endophenotype for bipolar I disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(5):565-570.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Giovanelli  A, Hoerger  M, Johnson  SL, Gruber  J.  Impulsive responses to positive mood and reward are related to mania risk. Cogn Emot. 2013;27(6):1091-1104.PubMed
22.
Swann  AC, Lijffijt  M, Lane  SD, Steinberg  JL, Moeller  FG.  Increased trait-like impulsivity and course of illness in bipolar disorder.  Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(3):280-288.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Mathias de Almeida  K, Nery  FG, Moreno  RA, Gorenstein  C, Lafer  B.  A sib-pair analysis of impulsivity in bipolar disorder type I.  Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54(8):1148-1152.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Chang  KD, Blasey  CM, Ketter  TA, Steiner  H.  Temperament characteristics of child and adolescent bipolar offspring.  J Affect Disord. 2003;77(1):11-19.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Knutson  B, Westdorp  A, Kaiser  E, Hommer  D.  FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task.  Neuroimage. 2000;12(1):20-27.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Gotlib  IH, Hamilton  JP, Cooney  RE, Singh  MK, Henry  ML, Joormann  J.  Neural processing of reward and loss in girls at risk for major depression.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):380-387.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Knutson  B, Fong  GW, Adams  CM, Varner  JL, Hommer  D.  Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI.  Neuroreport. 2001;12(17):3683-3687.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Knutson  B, Taylor  J, Kaufman  M, Peterson  R, Glover  G.  Distributed neural representation of expected value.  J Neurosci. 2005;25(19):4806-4812.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Haber  SN, Knutson  B.  The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):4-26.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Young  RC, Biggs  JT, Ziegler  VE, Meyer  DA.  A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.  Br J Psychiatry. 1978;133:429-435.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Poznanski  EO, Grossman  JA, Buchsbaum  Y, Banegas  M, Freeman  L, Gibbons  R.  Preliminary studies of the reliability and validity of the Children’s Depression Rating Scale.  J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23(2):191-197.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
March  JS, Parker  JD, Sullivan  K, Stallings  P, Conners  CK.  The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): factor structure, reliability, and validity.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):554-565.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Shaffer  D, Gould  MS, Brasic  J,  et al.  A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS).  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(11):1228-1231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Cosi  S, Vigil-Colet  A, Canals  J, Lorenzo-Seva  U.  Psychometric properties of the Spanish adaptation of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-A for children.  Psychol Rep. 2008;103(2):336-346.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Windle  M, Lerner  R.  Reassessing the dimensions of temperament individuality across the life span: the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R).  J Adolesc Res. 1986;1:213-229.Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Cirino  PT, Chin  CE, Sevcik  RA, Wolf  M, Lovett  M, Morris  RD.  Measuring socioeconomic status: reliability and preliminary validity for different approaches.  Assessment. 2002;9(2):145-155.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Carskadon  MA, Acebo  C.  A self-administered rating scale for pubertal development.  J Adolesc Health. 1993;14(3):190-195.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Psychological Corporation.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Co; 1999.
39.
Crovitz  HF, Zener  K.  A group-test for assessing hand- and eye-dominance.  Am J Psychol. 1962;75:271-276.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Cohen  MX, Heller  AS, Ranganath  C.  Functional connectivity with anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortices during decision-making.  Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;23(1):61-70.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Friston  KJ, Buechel  C, Fink  GR, Morris  J, Rolls  E, Dolan  RJ.  Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging.  Neuroimage. 1997;6(3):218-229.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
O’Sullivan  N, Szczepanowski  R, El-Deredy  W, Mason  L, Bentall  RP.  fMRI evidence of a relationship between hypomania and both increased goal-sensitivity and positive outcome-expectancy bias.  Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2825-2835.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Ernst  M, Nelson  EE, Jazbec  S,  et al.  Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents.  Neuroimage. 2005;25(4):1279-1291.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Ibanez  A, Cetkovich  M, Petroni  A,  et al.  The neural basis of decision-making and reward processing in adults with euthymic bipolar disorder or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).  PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e37306.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Amemori  K, Graybiel  AM.  Localized microstimulation of primate pregenual cingulate cortex induces negative decision-making.  Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):776-785.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Young  KD, Bellgowan  PSF, Bodurka  J, Drevets  WC.  Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of autobiographical memory deficits in patients with depression and individuals at high risk for depression.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(7):698-708.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Mannie  ZN, Norbury  R, Murphy  SE, Inkster  B, Harmer  CJ, Cowen  PJ.  Affective modulation of anterior cingulate cortex in young people at increased familial risk of depression.  Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192(5):356-361.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Elliott  R, Dolan  RJ, Frith  CD.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies.  Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):308-317. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Urošević  S, Collins  P, Muetzel  R, Lim  K, Luciana  M.  Longitudinal changes in behavioral approach system sensitivity and brain structures involved in reward processing during adolescence.  Dev Psychol. 2012;48(5):1488-1500.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Albrecht  K, Volz  KG, Sutter  M, von Cramon  DY.  What do I want and when do I want it: brain correlates of decisions made for self and other.  PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e73531.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Longe  O, Senior  C, Rippon  G.  The lateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex work as a dynamic integrated system: evidence from FMRI connectivity analysis.  J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21(1):141-154.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Mitchell  DGV.  The nexus between decision making and emotion regulation: a review of convergent neurocognitive substrates.  Behav Brain Res. 2011;217(1):215-231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Greening  SG, Osuch  EA, Williamson  PC, Mitchell  DGV.  The neural correlates of regulating positive and negative emotions in medication-free major depression.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(5):628-637.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Morris  RW, Sparks  A, Mitchell  PB, Weickert  CS, Green  MJ.  Lack of cortico-limbic coupling in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia during emotion regulation.  Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e90.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Passarotti  AM, Ellis  J, Wegbreit  E, Stevens  MC, Pavuluri  MN.  Reduced functional connectivity of prefrontal regions and amygdala within affect and working memory networks in pediatric bipolar disorder.  Brain Connect. 2012;2(6):320-334.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Ladouceur  CD, Diwadkar  VA, White  R,  et al.  Fronto-limbic function in unaffected offspring at familial risk for bipolar disorder during an emotional working memory paradigm.  Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5:185-196.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Townsend  JD, Torrisi  SJ, Lieberman  MD, Sugar  CA, Bookheimer  SY, Altshuler  LL.  Frontal-amygdala connectivity alterations during emotion downregulation in bipolar I disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(2):127-135.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Price  JL, Drevets  WC.  Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders.  Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(1):61-71.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Brotman  MA, Tseng  W-L, Olsavsky  AK,  et al.  Fronto-limbic-striatal dysfunction in pediatric and adult patients with bipolar disorder: impact of face emotion and attentional demands.  Psychol Med. 2014;44(8):1639-1651.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Yang  H, Lu  LH, Wu  M,  et al.  Time course of recovery showing initial prefrontal cortex changes at 16 weeks, extending to subcortical changes by 3 years in pediatric bipolar disorder.  J Affect Disord. 2013;150(2):571-577.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Simmons  RK, Howard  JL, Simpson  DN, Akil  H, Clinton  SM.  DNA methylation in the developing hippocampus and amygdala of anxiety-prone versus risk-taking rats.  Dev Neurosci. 2012;34(1):58-67.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Krebs  RM, Schott  BH, Düzel  E.  Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area.  Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65(2):103-110.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Eldreth  D, Hardin  MG, Pavletic  N, Ernst  M.  Adolescent transformations of behavioral and neural processes as potential targets for prevention.  Prev Sci. 2013;14(3):257-266.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×