[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1 
Means and SDs of heart rates in subjects with and without posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

Means and SDs of heart rates in subjects with and without posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).

Figure 2 
Frequency distribution of heart rate scores in the emergency department and 1 week later. PTSD indicates posttraumatic stress disorder.

Frequency distribution of heart rate scores in the emergency department and 1 week later. PTSD indicates posttraumatic stress disorder.

Table 1. 
Means and SDs of Demographic and Psychometric Variables in PTSD Subjects and Non-PTSD Subjects*
Means and SDs of Demographic and Psychometric Variables in PTSD Subjects and Non-PTSD Subjects*
Table 2. 
Means, SDs, and Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) of Psychometric Measures*
Means, SDs, and Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) of Psychometric Measures*
1.
Rothbaum  BOFoa  EB Subtypes of posttraumatic stress disorder and duration of symptoms. Davidson  JRTFoa  EBeds. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM-IV and Beyond Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1993;23- 35Google Scholar
2.
Kesslar  RCSonega  ABromet  EHughes  MNelson  CB Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521048- 1060Google Scholar
3.
Kolb  LCMultipassi  LR The conditioned emotional response: a subclass of the chronic and delayed posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatr Ann. 1982;12979- 987Google Scholar
4.
Pitman  RK Posttraumatic stress disorder, hormones, and memory.  Biol Psychiatry. 1989;26221- 223Google Scholar
5.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
6.
Mowrer  OH Learning Theory and Behavior.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1960;
7.
Keane  TMZimmering  RTCaddell  JM A behavioral formulation of post-traumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans.  Behav Ther. 1985;89- 12Google Scholar
8.
Foy  DWRueger  DBSipprelle  RCCarroll  EM Etiology of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans: analysis of premilitary, military, and combat exposure influences.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1984;5279- 87Google Scholar
9.
Breslau  NDavis  GC Posttraumatic stress disorder: the stressor criterion.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 1987;175255- 264Google Scholar
10.
Goldberg  JTrue  WREisen  SAHenderson  WG A twin study of the effects of the Vietnam war on posttraumatic stress disorder.  JAMA. 1990;2631227- 1232Google Scholar
11.
March  JS What constitutes a stressor? the criterion A issue in posttraumatic stress disorder. Davidson  JRTFoa  EBeds. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM-IV and Beyond Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1993;37- 54Google Scholar
12.
Feinstein  ADolan  R Predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder following physical trauma: an examination of the stressor criterion.  Psychol Med. 1991;2185- 91Google Scholar
13.
Perry  SDifede  JMusngi  GFrances  AJJacobsberg  L Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder after burn injury.  Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149931- 935Google Scholar
14.
Marmar  CRWeiss  DSSchlenger  WEFairbank  JAJordan  BKKulka  RAHough  RL Peritraumatic dissociation and posttraumatic stress in male Vietnam theater veterans.  Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151902- 907Google Scholar
15.
Shalev  AYPeri  TCanetti  LSchreiber  S Predictors of PTSD in injured trauma survivors: a prospective study.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153219- 225Google Scholar
16.
McGaugh  JLLiang  KCBennet  CSternberg  DB Adrenergic influence on memory storage: interaction of peripheral and central systems. Lynch  GMcGaugh  JLWeinberger  NMeds. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory New York, NY Guilford Press1984;313- 332Google Scholar
17.
Gold  PEMcCarthy  RC Stress regulation of memory processes: role of peripheral catecholamines and glucose. Friedman  MJCharney  DSDeutch  AYeds. Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1995;151- 162Google Scholar
18.
Resnick  HSYehuda  RPitman  RKFoy  DW Effect of previous trauma on acute plasma cortisol level following rape.  Am J Psychiatry. 1995;1521675- 1677Google Scholar
19.
Munck  AGuyre  PMHolbrook  NJ Physiological functions of glucocorticoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions.  Endocr Rev. 1984;939779- 9783Google Scholar
20.
Bohus  B Humoral modulation of learning and memory processes: physiological significance of brain and peripheral mechanisms. Delacour  Jed. The Memory System of the Brain Singapore World Scientific1984;337- 364Google Scholar
21.
Shalev  AYFreedman  SPeri  TBrandes  DSahar  T Predicting PTSD in civilian trauma survivors: prospective evaluation of self report and clinician administered instruments.  Br J Psychiatry. 1997;170558- 564Google Scholar
22.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
23.
Horowitz  MWilner  NAlvarez  W Impact of Events Scale: a measure of subjective stress.  Psychosom Med. 1979;41209- 218Google Scholar
24.
Keane  TMCaddell  JMTaylor  KL Mississippi Scale for Combat-Related Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: three studies in reliability and validity.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;5685- 90Google Scholar
25.
Speilberger  CD Manual for State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.  Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1983;
26.
Green  BL Psychometric review of Trauma History Questionnaires (Self Report). Stamm  BHed. Measurement of Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation Lutherville, Md Sidran Press1996;Google Scholar
27.
Pitman  RKOrr  SPForgue  DFdeJong  JBClaiborn  JM Psychophysiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in Vietnam combat veterans.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44970- 975Google Scholar
28.
Shalev  AYOrr  SPPitman  RK Psychophysiologic assessment of traumatic imagery in Israeli civilian patients with post-traumatic stress disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;50620- 624Google Scholar
29.
Hamilton  M A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;2356- 62Google Scholar
30.
Blake  DDWeathers  FWNagy  LMKaloupek  DGKlauminzer  GCharney  DSKeane  TM A clinician rating scale for assessing current and lifetime PTSD: the CAPS-1.  Behav Ther. 1990;13187- 188Google Scholar
31.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBAGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R: Patient Version.  New York, NY New York State Psychiatric Institute1987;
32.
Solomon  ZBenbenishty  RNeria  YAbramowitz  MGinzburg  KOhry  A Assessment of PTSD: validation of the revised PTSD Inventory.  Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 1993;30110- 115Google Scholar
33.
Shalev  AYOrr  SPPeri  TSchreiber  SPitman  RK Physiologic responses to loud tones in Israeli posttraumatic stress disorder patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49870- 875Google Scholar
34.
Orr  SPLasko  NShalev  APitman  RK Physiologic responses to loud tones in Vietnam veterans with PTSD.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;10475- 82Google Scholar
35.
Prins  AKaloupek  DGKeane  TM Psychophysiological evidence for autonomic arousal and startle in traumatized adult populations. Friedman  MJCharney  DSDeutch  AYeds. Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1995;291- 314Google Scholar
36.
Murburg  MMMcFall  MEVeith  RC Basal sympathoadrenal function in patients with PTSD and depression. Murburg  MMed. Cathecholamine Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Emerging Concepts Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1994;175- 188Google Scholar
37.
McFall  MEMurburg  MM Psychophysiological studies of combat-related PTSD: an integrated view. Murburg  MMed. Cathecholamine Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Emerging Concepts Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1994;161- 173Google Scholar
38.
Geraradi  RJKeane  TMCahoon  BJKlauminzer  GW An in vivo assessment of physiological arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103825- 827Google Scholar
39.
Southwick  SMKrystal  JHMorgan  CAJohnson  DNagy  LMNicolaou  AHeninger  GRCharney  DS Abnormal noradrenergic function in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50266- 274Google Scholar
40.
True  WRRice  JEisen  SAHeath  ACGoldberg  JLyons  MJNowak  J A twin study of genetic and environmental contributions to liability for posttraumatic stress symptoms.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50257- 264Google Scholar
41.
Breslau  NDavis  GCAndreski  PPeterson  E Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48216- 222Google Scholar
42.
Hobfoll  SE The Ecology of Stress.  New York, NY Hemisphere Publishing Corporation1988;
43.
McFall  MEVeith  RCMurburg  MM Basal sympathoadrenal function in PTSD.  Biol Psychiatry. 1992;311050- 1056Google Scholar
44.
Williams  CLMcGaugh  JL Reversible inactivation of the solitary tract impairs retention performance in an inhibitory avoidance task.  Behav Neural Biol. 1992;58204- 210Google Scholar
Original Article
June 1998

A Prospective Study of Heart Rate Response Following Trauma and the Subsequent Development of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Author Affiliations

From The Center for Traumatic Stress, Department of Psychiatry, Hadassah University Hospital, Jerusalem, Israel (Drs Shalev, Peri, and Glick and Mss Sahar, Freedman, and Brandes); and Manchester Veterans Affairs Research Service, Harvard Medical School, Manchester, NH (Drs Orr and Pitman).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(6):553-559. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.553
Abstract

Background  Physiological arousal during traumatic events may trigger the neurobiological processes that lead to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This study prospectively examined the relationship between heart rate and blood pressure recorded immediately following a traumatic event and the subsequent development of PTSD.

Methods  Eighty-six trauma survivors who presented at the emergency department of a general hospital were followed up for 4 months. Heart rate and blood pressure were recorded on arrival at the emergency department. Heart rate, anxiety, depression, and PTSD symptoms were assessed 1 week, 1 month, and 4 months later. The clinician-administered PTSD scale defined PTSD status at 4 months.

Results  Twenty subjects (23%) met PTSD diagnostic criteria at the 4-month assessment (PTSD group), and 66 (77%) did not (non-PTSD group). Subjects who developed PTSD had higher heart rates at the emergency department (95.5±13.9 vs 83.3±10.9 beats per minute, t=4.4, P<.001) and 1 week later (77.8±11.9 vs 72.0±9.5 beats per minute, t=2.25, P<.03), but not after 1 and 4 months. The groups did not differ in initial blood pressure measurement. Repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for heart rate showed a significant group effect (P<.02), time effect (P<.001), and group × time interaction (P<.001). The time effect and group × time interaction remained significant when adjusted for sex, age, trauma severity, immediate response, and dissociation during the traumatic event.

Conclusion  Elevated heart rate shortly after trauma is associated with the later development of PTSD.

POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS disorder (PTSD) is a pervasive anxiety disorder that occurs in 15% to 25% of trauma survivors. Symptoms of PTSD have been observed in up to 94% of trauma survivors 1 week following trauma.1 Yet the intensity of such symptoms decreases in most survivors, leaving only a few with chronic PTSD.2 The mechanism that links exposure to a traumatic event with the development of PTSD remains largely unidentified.

According to classical conditioning theory, the traumatic event serves as an unconditioned stimulus that evokes an immediate and "hardwired" response from the organism (unconditioned response).3,4 Stimuli associated with the traumatic event then become conditioned stimuli, capable of subsequently eliciting conditioned responses in the form of PTSD symptoms (eg, reactivity "upon exposure to cues that symbolize or resemble an aspect of the traumatic event").5 Additionally, the avoidance of stimuli associated with the traumatic event may be rewarded by a reduction in distress, and thereby be reinforced and fail to extinguish with time.6,7

The intensity of the traumatic event (unconditioned stimulus) has been shown to correlate with the subsequent development of PTSD.8-11 However, it is the reaction to the traumatic event at the time of its occurrence (unconditioned response) that may hold the key to the pathogenesis of PTSD.12,13 The psychological component of the immediate reaction has been the object of a few studies,14,15 and is generally believed to contribute to the development of PTSD above and beyond the intensity of the exposure. The physiological component of the response to trauma, however, has received relatively little attention.

Physiological activation during stressful events may play a central role in the pathogenesis of PTSD. Peripheral administration of epinephrine immediately after aversive training enhances the consolidation of amygdala-mediated learning in animal models.16,17 Low cortisol levels following rape have been found to be associated with a higher risk for developing PTSD,18 and low corticosterone levels may prolong the adrenergic response to stress19 and enhance the effect of catecholamines on memory consolidation in animals.20

Studying bodily responses to traumatic stress is therefore of particular interest. It is also important to know whether measurable dimensions of such responses can improve the prediction of PTSD over and above predictions made from psychometrics. Our study prospectively evaluates the relationship between 2 measures of physiological activation recorded immediately following the trauma, heart rate and blood pressure, and the subsequent development of PTSD.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

The results presented here are part of a large-scale prospective study of the effect of trauma (see Shalev et al21 for discussion of the parent project). Patients arriving at the emergency department of Hadassah University Hospital, Jerusalem, Israel, were recruited into the study during a period of 2 years. Patients were examined by a research psychiatrist and considered for the study if they were between 16 and 65 years of age and had experienced a traumatic event meeting the criterion A of the DSM-III-R.22 Subject candidates received information about the study, were invited to participate, and gave written informed consent. They were subsequently interviewed 1 week, 1 month, and 4 months after the traumatic event. The follow-up interviews included self-reported psychometrics, structured clinical interviews, and assessments of resting heart rate. Blood pressure was measured in the emergency department but not during follow-up.

To reduce the heart rate and blood pressure variability that might result from severe physical injuries, treatment provided on the way to the hospital (eg, fluid substitution, medication), and different methods of vital sign monitoring in the emergency department (severely injured patients are monitored on-line), only patients with mild injuries who did not need surgical intervention and who were released to their homes within 12 hours of emergency department arrival were included. Other exclusion criteria were head injury, current or lifetime abuse of alcohol or illicit drugs (relatively rare in Israel), past or present psychosis, a life-threatening medical illness, and burn injury.

A total of 239 subjects agreed to participate in the parent study and 191 (79.9%) completed all 3 follow-up interviews. Of these, 105 failed to meet the more stringent exclusion criteria of the current study, leaving a total of 86 participants (45% of all completers). Subjects who were excluded from this study did not differ from those included in type of traumatic events (83% motor vehicle crashes; χ2 test, 0.02; P=.9) and in prior and event-related variables (ie, age [29.3±10.4 years among excluded patients], number of past traumatic events [4.2±1.8 among excluded patients], event severity [5.0±1.6], Peritraumatic Dissociation Responses Questionnaire14(PDEQ) score [18.7±7.6] and immediate response scores [73.2±27.7]; multivariate analysis of variance [MANOVA] F5,186=1.56; P=.2). The subgroups did not differ in the prevalence of 4-month PTSD (17% in the parent sample; χ2 test, 2.54; P=.12), in 1-week and 1-month scores on the Impact of Events Scale23(IES) (36.4±15.5 and 24.8±15.4, Fs<1), in 1-month scores on the Mississippi Scale for Combat-Related PTSD–Civilian Version24(MISS) (70.6±20.7; F<1) and in 1-month scores on the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) (23.9±26.2, F<1). Finally, the subgroups did not differ in heart rate measures at 1 week (75.1±10.3 among those excluded, t=0.41), 1 month (72.6±9.4, t=0.67) and 4 months (73.0±11.2, t=0.44).

Psychometrics

Self-report instruments included the IES, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,25 (STAI), MISS, PDEQ, an immediate-response questionnaire, and a trauma history questionnaire.

The IES, STAI, and MISS have been used in numerous studies of PTSD and other disorders and will not be described here (see Shalev21 for detailed discussion of psychometric properties and predictive power) in this study's sample. The PDEQ is an 8-item questionnaire about experiences related to the construct of dissociation, validated by Marmar et al14 with 238 male Vietnam war veterans and by Shalev et al15,21 in civilian trauma survivors.

The immediate-response questionnaire included 14 items that assessed the intensity of physical (eg, pain), emotional (eg, fear, anger), and negative cognitive (eg, expectation of doom) experiences during the event. Each item was rated from 1 to 10 (1=none, 10=highest possible intensity), yielding response intensity scores ranging from 10 to 140.

The trauma history questionnaire26 assessed the nature and number of previously experienced traumatic events (eg, natural and man-made disasters, war, motor vehicle crashes, rape, physical assault, and others). The number of distinct traumatic events endured by each subject constituted the questionnaire's score.

In addition to the above, 12 mental health professionals, blinded as to subjects' PTSD status, listened to audiotaped scripts describing each of the traumatic events (for detailed procedure see21,26-28) and independently rated the severity of the traumatic event on a 1 to 10 scale (1=not severe at all, 10=extreme severity). The average score of the 12 observers is reported.

Structured clinical instruments

These included Hebrew versions of the Hamilton Depression and Hamilton Anxiety scales,29 CAPS,30 and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III31 (SCID). All instruments had been previously validated and used in studies of PTSD,15,28,32,33 and all were administered by clinicians (D.B., S.F., T.P., and A.S.) with extensive experience in PTSD diagnosis and treatment. The PTSD status at 4 months was determined according to DSM-III-R criteria as measured by the CAPS. Current and lifetime diagnoses of other mental disorders were identified via the SCID.

Heart rate and blood pressure

Heart rate and blood pressure were obtained from all patients on presentation to the emergency department. A registered nurse, using a vital signs monitor (Critikon Dynamap, Tampa, Fla), measured and then immediately transcribed these measures into the patient's medical record. During subsequent assessments, resting heart rate was recorded as part of a larger study of responses to auditory startle in the psychophysiological laboratory of the Center for Traumatic Stress, Hadassah University Hospital, according to a previously described procedure.33,34 A Coulbourn Modular Instrument System (Coulbourn Inc, Allentown, Pa), interfaced with a personal computer through a Coulbourn Lablinc Computer Interface was used to record heart rate via standard limb electrocardiogram leads connected to a high-gain bioamplifier and inputting to a Coulbourn Tachometer. The analog signal was sampled and digitized by a Coulbourn Lablinc Analog-to-Digital Converter at a rate of 2 Hz. The laboratory sessions took place in an 3.2×2.4-m humidity- and temperature-controlled room, connected via wires to an adjoining portion of the laboratory in which the experimental apparatus was located. The subject was seated in a comfortable armchair. After the subject was familiarized with the laboratory, the electrodes were attached, and he or she was asked to sit quietly for 5 minutes. Heart rates were sampled continuously during this period and averaged to yield a measure of resting heart rate.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses follow the general linear model for group comparisons (ANOVAs and MANOVAs), group × time comparisons (repeated-measures ANOVA), control of confounds (analysis of covariance [ANCOVA]), and analyses of multiple correlations (multiple regression). Logistic regression was used to assess the prediction of PTSD. An α level of .05 conferred statistical significance. All values are given as mean±SD unless otherwise indicated.

Results
Diagnostic classification and demographic data

Eighty-six trauma survivors (34 women and 52 men) were included in this study. Thirty-three (38%) met diagnostic criteria for PTSD at 1 month, and 20 (23%) had PTSD at the 4-month assessment (PTSD group). Sixty-six (77%) did not have PTSD at 4 months (non-PTSD group). Traumatic events in the PTSD group included motor vehicle crashes (n=17, 85%), terrorist attacks (n=2, 10%) and a mechanical accident occurring at home (n=1, 5%). Traumatic events in the non-PTSD group included motor vehicle crashes (n=53, 80%), work-related accidents (n=5, 7.5%) terrorist attacks (n=4, 6%), mechanical accidents occurring at home (n=3, 4.5%), and witnessing violence (n=1, 1.5%). The difference between the groups in type of traumatic events (motor vehicle crashes vs all others) was not statistically significant (χ2 test, 0.22; P=.64).

Eight PTSD subjects (40%) and 16 non-PTSD subjects (24%) had at least 1 DSM-III-R Axis I disorder prior to the traumatic event (χ2 test, 1.89; P=.16). Lifetime diagnoses in the PTSD group (more than one per subject allowed) included simple phobia (n=6), social phobia (n=3), major depression (n=3), panic disorder (n=1), bipolar disorder (n=1), and agoraphobia without panic attacks (n=1). Lifetime diagnoses in the non-PTSD group included major depression (n=6), social phobia (n=5), somatoform disorders (n=4), simple phobia (n=1), and panic disorder (n=1).

Psychometrics

Table 1 provides demographic and psychometric information for the 2 groups. The groups did not differ in the average number of past traumatic events, but did differ in objective ratings of current event severity, self-reported severity of the immediate response, and PDEQ (MANOVA F3,81=3.56, P<.02). Group differences at 1 and 4 months are consistent with the group selection procedure and reflect the presence vs absence of PTSD. More importantly, these differences significantly increased with time (Table 2).

Heart rate and blood pressure
Differences Between PTSD Subjects and Non-PTSD Subjects

Repeated-measures ANOVA for heart rate revealed a significant main effect of diagnostic group (F1,84=5.73, P<.02), a significant main effect of time (F3,252=62.2, P <.001), and a significant group × time interaction (F3,252=8.71, P<.001). Figure 1 illustrates changes in mean heart rate over time.

The PTSD group had higher heart rates in the emergency department (96.4±13.9 vs 83.3±10.9 beats per minute; t [84]=4.40; P<.001) and higher 1-week heart rates (77.8±12.9 vs 72.0±9.46 beats per minute; t [84]=2.26; P<.03) than the non-PTSD group. Figure 2 illustrates the distribution of individual heart rate measurements in the emergency department and 1 week later, showing an overlap between the groups and the lack of remote outliers in the PTSD group. The groups did not differ in 1-month heart rate measurements (74.06±12.8 vs 74.02±10.52 beats per minute; t [84]=0.02) or in 4-month heart rate measurements (72.7±13.3 vs 72.2±9.78 beats per minute; t [84]=0.21). The groups did not differ in emergency department systolic (139.8±24.9 vs 128.2±17.8 mm Hg; t [84]=0.53) or diastolic (79.8±12.4 vs 78.5±9.04 mm Hg, t=0.52) blood pressure.

Controlling for Prior Variables, Event Severity, and Immediate Responses

Repeated-measures ANCOVA for heart rate (emergency department, 1 week, 1 month, and 4 months), using PTSD as grouping factor and controlling for sex, age, past Axis I disorders (as a dummy 0,1 variable), event severity, response severity, and peritraumatic dissociation, yielded a nearly significant group effect (F1,77=2.94, P =.09), a significant main effect of time (F3,249=61.06, P<.001), and a significant group × time interaction (F3,249=8.63, P<.001). The adjusted mean heart rate in the emergency department was 95.1 beats per minute for PTSD subjects and 84.7 beats per minute for non-PTSD subjects (F1,78=10.92, P<.002). The adjusted mean heart rate at 1 week was 77.3 beats per minute for PTSD subjects and 72.6 beats per minute for non-PTSD subjects (F1,78=2.78, P=.09).

Correlations Between Emergency Department Heart Rate and Other Measures
Prior Variables

Heart rate levels in the emergency department were higher in male (88.6±13.6 beats per minute) than in female subjects (82.9±11.03 beats per minute). Two-way ANOVA failed to show a sex × PTSD interaction (F1,82=0.11), while showing a significant main effect of PTSD (F1,82=16.75, P<.001) and a trend toward a main effect of sex (F1,82=3.39, P<.07). The correlations with age and with peritraumatic dissociation were not significant.

Trauma-Related Variables

Heart rate levels in the emergency department showed a small but significant correlation with systolic blood pressure (r=0.24, P<.03), diastolic blood pressure (r=0.32, P<.01), event severity (r=0.36, P<.001), and immediate response (r=0.26, P<.02).

Continuous Outcome Variables

Heart rate scores in the emergency department produced significant correlation with 4-month IES Intrusion subscale scores (r=0.43, P<.001) IES Avoidance subscale scores (r=0.28, P=.02), STAI-State scores (r=0.31, P<.01), MISS scores (r=0.24, P=.03), and total CAPS score (r=0.31, P<.005). Systolic and diastolic blood pressure did not correlate significantly with any of the 4-month variables.

Predicting PTSD From Prior Variables, Immediate Responses, and Heart Rate

Stepwise hierarchical logistic regression, estimating the effects of sex, age, trauma history, event severity, immediate response, dissociation, and initial (emergency department) heart rate (in that order) on 4-month PTSD status showed a significant effect of event severity (χ2 test, 0.01; P<.04) and a significant additional effect (increase in χ2 test, 9.97; P =.002) of initial heart rate (total χ2 test, 20.37; P=.005), with no other variable contributing significantly to 4-month PTSD.

Comment

The results of this study showed that on arrival to an emergency department following a traumatic event, and regardless of whether PTSD developed, survivors of traumatic events showed elevated heart rates, which later normalized. Survivors who subsequently developed PTSD showed higher emergency department and 1-week heart rates than those who did not. The heart rate difference between diagnostic groups was not accounted for by rated intensity of the trauma nor by self-reported responses. This finding suggests that heart rate expresses a dimension of the response to traumatic events that is not entirely captured by psychometrics. Initial blood pressure measurements did not differentiate the groups, nor did they correlate with any of the outcome measures.

There are several limitations to this study, mostly resulting from its "naturalistic" design. First, while all subjects had come to the emergency department directly from the site of their trauma, the exact timing of their arrival relative to the time of their traumatic incident was unavailable. However, traumatic events are not limited to the impact phase but often include the evacuation phase as well. Second, heart rate levels in the emergency department were obtained by single measurement, while those obtained at 1 week represent an average of 5 minutes of continuous recording. However, the persistence of higher heart rate levels at 1 week in PTSD supports the validity of the study's main finding. Data on blood pressure, in contrast, rely on a single measurement.

Within the above-mentioned conditioning model, the finding of elevated heart rate in the emergency department in the PTSD group may be interpreted as reflecting higher intensity of the unconditioned response. Theoretically, excessive adrenergic activation may contribute to the development of PTSD through enhanced memory consolidation of the traumatic event (unconditioned stimulus).4,19 The lack of blood pressure elevation in this study, despite heart rate increase, suggests a mainly adrenergic as opposed to noradrenergic activation.

Our study does not exclude a possible effect of a prior trait (eg, hyperresponsiveness) on both heart rate response and the development of PTSD. Posttraumatic stress disorder has been associated with increased physiological responses to trauma-related cues3,27,28,35-37 and to other stimuli, including loud tones33,34; nontraumatic stressors, such as waiting to receive medical treatment38; and chemical stimulation, such as yohimbine administration.39 Hypothetically, increased physiological responsiveness may precede the traumatic event, be expressed during the event, and become part of PTSD after the trauma. The source of such hyperresponsiveness may be genetic. A twin study of Vietnam veterans has shown that about 30% of the liability to developing PTSD symptoms of hyperarousal and avoidance are accounted for by heredity.40 Alternatively, the elevated heart rate responses in PTSD patients may reflect the priming effect of earlier traumatization or a combination of inherited vulnerability and lifetime exposure. A family history of mental disorders has been associated with increased likelihood of both exposure to traumatic events and of developing PTSD on such exposure.41

Interestingly, the heart rate responses of the 2 groups overlapped (Figure 2) and were not extreme. This finding does not support theories postulating that PTSD typically results from "extreme" or "catastrophic" responses to stress.42 Indeed, these results are in line with previous research showing that moderate sympathetic activation has a greater effect on learning and memory than more extreme activation.17

Despite increasing differences in symptom intensity, the 2 study groups had similar resting heart rates 1 and 4 months after trauma. This may reflect a slow return to baseline following the trauma and/or habituation to the laboratory setting. Studies of resting heart rate in PTSD37 are often confounded by an expectation of a stressful task, such as psychophysiological testing. Conversely, when PTSD subjects were maintained in supine position for 30 minutes without an expectation of a challenge, their heart rate, blood pressure, and plasma norepinephrine levels were not found to differ from those of controls.43 Accordingly, Prins et al35 concluded that baseline heart rate may not be higher in PTSD. It is possible that in the emergency department and during the first visit to the laboratory, PTSD subjects reacted as toward a challenge, and therefore showed higher heart rate levels. By their second and third visits to the laboratory, however, they may have became accustomed to the setting and learned that they were not in a threatening environment, so that no differences were seen.

Previous studies have suggested that an alteration in noradrenergic brain functions may exist in chronic PTSD.36,37,39 This study does not address central noradrenergic mechanisms, nor those through which peripheral hormones affect central catecholamines and memory consolidation. The effect of peripheral epinephrine on memory may be mediated through the brainstem nucleus of the solitary tract, projecting into the locus ceruleus,44 or through epinephrine-related increase in circulating glucose levels.17 Future prospective studies should explore such endocrine and metabolic variables shortly after trauma. They should also address the interaction between circulating catecholamines and cortisol during acute stress43 and its relationship with subsequent PTSD.

Accepted for publication September 4, 1997.

This research was supported by grant MH-50379 from the US Public Health Service, Washington, DC.

Reprints: Arieh Y. Shalev, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Hadassah University Hospital, PO Box 12000, Jerusalem 91120, Israel.

References
1.
Rothbaum  BOFoa  EB Subtypes of posttraumatic stress disorder and duration of symptoms. Davidson  JRTFoa  EBeds. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM-IV and Beyond Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1993;23- 35Google Scholar
2.
Kesslar  RCSonega  ABromet  EHughes  MNelson  CB Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521048- 1060Google Scholar
3.
Kolb  LCMultipassi  LR The conditioned emotional response: a subclass of the chronic and delayed posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatr Ann. 1982;12979- 987Google Scholar
4.
Pitman  RK Posttraumatic stress disorder, hormones, and memory.  Biol Psychiatry. 1989;26221- 223Google Scholar
5.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
6.
Mowrer  OH Learning Theory and Behavior.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1960;
7.
Keane  TMZimmering  RTCaddell  JM A behavioral formulation of post-traumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans.  Behav Ther. 1985;89- 12Google Scholar
8.
Foy  DWRueger  DBSipprelle  RCCarroll  EM Etiology of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans: analysis of premilitary, military, and combat exposure influences.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1984;5279- 87Google Scholar
9.
Breslau  NDavis  GC Posttraumatic stress disorder: the stressor criterion.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 1987;175255- 264Google Scholar
10.
Goldberg  JTrue  WREisen  SAHenderson  WG A twin study of the effects of the Vietnam war on posttraumatic stress disorder.  JAMA. 1990;2631227- 1232Google Scholar
11.
March  JS What constitutes a stressor? the criterion A issue in posttraumatic stress disorder. Davidson  JRTFoa  EBeds. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM-IV and Beyond Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1993;37- 54Google Scholar
12.
Feinstein  ADolan  R Predictors of post-traumatic stress disorder following physical trauma: an examination of the stressor criterion.  Psychol Med. 1991;2185- 91Google Scholar
13.
Perry  SDifede  JMusngi  GFrances  AJJacobsberg  L Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder after burn injury.  Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149931- 935Google Scholar
14.
Marmar  CRWeiss  DSSchlenger  WEFairbank  JAJordan  BKKulka  RAHough  RL Peritraumatic dissociation and posttraumatic stress in male Vietnam theater veterans.  Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151902- 907Google Scholar
15.
Shalev  AYPeri  TCanetti  LSchreiber  S Predictors of PTSD in injured trauma survivors: a prospective study.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153219- 225Google Scholar
16.
McGaugh  JLLiang  KCBennet  CSternberg  DB Adrenergic influence on memory storage: interaction of peripheral and central systems. Lynch  GMcGaugh  JLWeinberger  NMeds. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory New York, NY Guilford Press1984;313- 332Google Scholar
17.
Gold  PEMcCarthy  RC Stress regulation of memory processes: role of peripheral catecholamines and glucose. Friedman  MJCharney  DSDeutch  AYeds. Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1995;151- 162Google Scholar
18.
Resnick  HSYehuda  RPitman  RKFoy  DW Effect of previous trauma on acute plasma cortisol level following rape.  Am J Psychiatry. 1995;1521675- 1677Google Scholar
19.
Munck  AGuyre  PMHolbrook  NJ Physiological functions of glucocorticoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions.  Endocr Rev. 1984;939779- 9783Google Scholar
20.
Bohus  B Humoral modulation of learning and memory processes: physiological significance of brain and peripheral mechanisms. Delacour  Jed. The Memory System of the Brain Singapore World Scientific1984;337- 364Google Scholar
21.
Shalev  AYFreedman  SPeri  TBrandes  DSahar  T Predicting PTSD in civilian trauma survivors: prospective evaluation of self report and clinician administered instruments.  Br J Psychiatry. 1997;170558- 564Google Scholar
22.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
23.
Horowitz  MWilner  NAlvarez  W Impact of Events Scale: a measure of subjective stress.  Psychosom Med. 1979;41209- 218Google Scholar
24.
Keane  TMCaddell  JMTaylor  KL Mississippi Scale for Combat-Related Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: three studies in reliability and validity.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;5685- 90Google Scholar
25.
Speilberger  CD Manual for State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.  Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1983;
26.
Green  BL Psychometric review of Trauma History Questionnaires (Self Report). Stamm  BHed. Measurement of Stress, Trauma, and Adaptation Lutherville, Md Sidran Press1996;Google Scholar
27.
Pitman  RKOrr  SPForgue  DFdeJong  JBClaiborn  JM Psychophysiological assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in Vietnam combat veterans.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44970- 975Google Scholar
28.
Shalev  AYOrr  SPPitman  RK Psychophysiologic assessment of traumatic imagery in Israeli civilian patients with post-traumatic stress disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;50620- 624Google Scholar
29.
Hamilton  M A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;2356- 62Google Scholar
30.
Blake  DDWeathers  FWNagy  LMKaloupek  DGKlauminzer  GCharney  DSKeane  TM A clinician rating scale for assessing current and lifetime PTSD: the CAPS-1.  Behav Ther. 1990;13187- 188Google Scholar
31.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBAGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R: Patient Version.  New York, NY New York State Psychiatric Institute1987;
32.
Solomon  ZBenbenishty  RNeria  YAbramowitz  MGinzburg  KOhry  A Assessment of PTSD: validation of the revised PTSD Inventory.  Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 1993;30110- 115Google Scholar
33.
Shalev  AYOrr  SPPeri  TSchreiber  SPitman  RK Physiologic responses to loud tones in Israeli posttraumatic stress disorder patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49870- 875Google Scholar
34.
Orr  SPLasko  NShalev  APitman  RK Physiologic responses to loud tones in Vietnam veterans with PTSD.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;10475- 82Google Scholar
35.
Prins  AKaloupek  DGKeane  TM Psychophysiological evidence for autonomic arousal and startle in traumatized adult populations. Friedman  MJCharney  DSDeutch  AYeds. Neurobiological and Clinical Consequences of Stress Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1995;291- 314Google Scholar
36.
Murburg  MMMcFall  MEVeith  RC Basal sympathoadrenal function in patients with PTSD and depression. Murburg  MMed. Cathecholamine Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Emerging Concepts Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1994;175- 188Google Scholar
37.
McFall  MEMurburg  MM Psychophysiological studies of combat-related PTSD: an integrated view. Murburg  MMed. Cathecholamine Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Emerging Concepts Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1994;161- 173Google Scholar
38.
Geraradi  RJKeane  TMCahoon  BJKlauminzer  GW An in vivo assessment of physiological arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103825- 827Google Scholar
39.
Southwick  SMKrystal  JHMorgan  CAJohnson  DNagy  LMNicolaou  AHeninger  GRCharney  DS Abnormal noradrenergic function in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50266- 274Google Scholar
40.
True  WRRice  JEisen  SAHeath  ACGoldberg  JLyons  MJNowak  J A twin study of genetic and environmental contributions to liability for posttraumatic stress symptoms.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50257- 264Google Scholar
41.
Breslau  NDavis  GCAndreski  PPeterson  E Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48216- 222Google Scholar
42.
Hobfoll  SE The Ecology of Stress.  New York, NY Hemisphere Publishing Corporation1988;
43.
McFall  MEVeith  RCMurburg  MM Basal sympathoadrenal function in PTSD.  Biol Psychiatry. 1992;311050- 1056Google Scholar
44.
Williams  CLMcGaugh  JL Reversible inactivation of the solitary tract impairs retention performance in an inhibitory avoidance task.  Behav Neural Biol. 1992;58204- 210Google Scholar
×