[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Age vs cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) (A), gray matter (B), and white matter (C) volumes corrected for the effects of intracranial volume (ICV) for 82 healthy controls. The regression line (solid) for controls is shown together with lines demarcating 1 and 2 SDs above and below the regression line.

Age vs cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) (A), gray matter (B), and white matter (C) volumes corrected for the effects of intracranial volume (ICV) for 82 healthy controls. The regression line (solid) for controls is shown together with lines demarcating 1 and 2 SDs above and below the regression line.

Figure 2. 
Intracranial (ICV)-corrected brain measures for all 77 patients superimposed on control regression line (solid) and lines demarcating 1 and 2 SDs above and below the regression line. The patient group differed significantly from the control group on total cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) (A) and total gray matter (B) volumes, but not total white matter volume (C).

Intracranial (ICV)-corrected brain measures for all 77 patients superimposed on control regression line (solid) and lines demarcating 1 and 2 SDs above and below the regression line. The patient group differed significantly from the control group on total cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) (A) and total gray matter (B) volumes, but not total white matter volume (C).

Table 1. 
Characteristics of the Patient and Control Samples*
Characteristics of the Patient and Control Samples*
Table 2. 
Brain Measures for Patients With First Episode (FE) Psychosis and Age-Matched Controls Before and After Correction for Intracranial Volume (ICV) and Age*
Brain Measures for Patients With First Episode (FE) Psychosis and Age-Matched Controls Before and After Correction for Intracranial Volume (ICV) and Age*
1.
Johnstone  ECCrow  TJFrith  DCHusband  JKrel  L Cerebral ventricular size and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.  Lancet. 1976;2924- 926Google Scholar
2.
Weinberger  DRTorrey  EFNeophytides  ANWyatt  RJ Structural abnormalities in the cerebral cortex of chronic schizophrenic patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36935- 939Google Scholar
3.
Pfefferbaum  AZipursky  RBLim  KOZatz  LMStahl  SMJernigan  TL Computed tomographic evidence for generalized sulcal and ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45633- 640Google Scholar
4.
Pearlson  GDKim  WSKubos  KLMoberg  PJJayaram  GBascom  MJChase  GAGoldfinger  ADTune  LE Ventricle-brain ratio, computed tomographic density and brain area in 50 schizophrenics.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46690- 697Google Scholar
5.
Andreasen  NCSmith  MRJocoby  CGDennert  JWOlsen  SA Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: definition and prevalence.  Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139292- 302Google Scholar
6.
Zipursky  RBLim  KOSullivan  EVBrown  BWPfefferbaum  A Widespread cerebral gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49195- 205Google Scholar
7.
Harvey  IRon  MAdu Bouley  GWicks  DLewis  SWMurray  RM Reduction of cortical volume in schizophrenia on magnetic resonance imaging.  Psychol Med. 1993;23591- 604Google Scholar
8.
Schlaepfer  THarris  GJTien  AYPeng  LWLee  SFederman  EBChase  GABarta  PEPearlson  GD Decreased regional cortical gray matter volume in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151842- 848Google Scholar
9.
Lim  KOSullivan  EVZipursky  RBPfefferbaum  A Cortical gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia: a replication.  Schizophr Res. 1996;20157- 164Google Scholar
10.
Lim  KHarris  DBeal  MHoff  ALMinn  KCsernansky  JGFaustman  WOMarsh  LSullivan  EVPfefferbaum  A Gray matter deficits in young onset schizophrenia are independent of age of onset.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;404- 13Google Scholar
11.
Woods  BYurgelun-Todd  DGoldstein  JMSeidman  LJTsuang  MT MRI brain abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia: one process or more?  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40585- 596Google Scholar
12.
Zipursky  RSeeman  MVBury  ALangevin  RWortzman  GKatz  R Deficits in gray matter volume are present in schizophrenia but not bipolar disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1997;2685- 92Google Scholar
13.
Jernigan  TLZatz  LMMoses  JABerger  PA Computed tomography in schizophrenics and normal volunteers, I: fluid volume.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39765- 770Google Scholar
14.
Jernigan  TLZisook  SHeaton  RKMoranville  JTHesselink  JRBraff  DL Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48881- 890Google Scholar
15.
Buchanan  RWBreier  AKirkpatrick  BElkashef  AMunson  RCGellad  FCarpenter  WT Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;15059- 65Google Scholar
16.
Barta  PPearlson  GDBrill  LBRoyall  RMcGilchrist  IKPulver  AEPowers  RECasanova  MFTien  AYFrangou  SPetty  RG Planum temporale asymmetry reversal in schizophrenia: replication and relationship to gray matter abnormalities.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154661- 667Google Scholar
17.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-III-R).  Washington, D American Psychiatric Association1987;
18.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  MFirst  MB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R - Patient Edition (With Psychotic Screen).  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press Inc1990;
19.
McGuffin  PFarmer  AHarvey  I A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;47764- 770Google Scholar
20.
Cannon-Spoor  HEPotkin  SGWyatt  RJ Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1982;8470- 484Google Scholar
21.
Maxwell  ME Manual for the FIGS.  Bethesda, Md Clinical Neurogenetics Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health1992;
22.
Ocarroll  RWalker  MDunan  JMurray  CBlackwood  DEbmeier  KPGoodwin  GM Selecting controls for schizophrenia research studies: the use of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) is a measure of premorbid ability.  Schizophr Res. 1992;8137- 141Google Scholar
23.
Nelson  HE National Adult Reading Test, NART.  Windsor, Ontario Nelson Publishing Co1982;
24.
Ammons  RBAmmons  CH The Quick Test (QT): Provisional Manual.  Psychological Rep. 1962;11111- 161Google Scholar
25.
Skinner  HA Development and validation of a lifetime alcohol consumption assessment procedure.  Toronto, Canada Addiction Research Foundation1982;
26.
American Psychiatric Association, Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;1541- 63Google Scholar
27.
Hollingshead  AB Two-Factor Index of Social Position.  New Haven, Conn Yale University1957;
28.
Reiss  ALHennessey  JSubramanium  BRubin  MBeach  L BRAINIMAGE.  Baltimore, Md Kennedy Krieger Institute Neuroimaging Research Center1995;
29.
Katzman  DBLambe  EKMikulis  DJRidgley  JNGoldbloom  DSZipursky  RB Cerebral gray matter and white matter volume deficits in adolescent girls with anorexia nervosa.  J Pediatr. 1996;129794- 803Google Scholar
30.
Lambe  EKKatzman  DKMikulis  DJKennedy  SHZipursky  RB Cerebral gray matter volume deficits after weight recovery from anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54537- 542Google Scholar
31.
Weinberger  DRDeLisi  LEPerman  GPTargum  SWyatt  RJ Computed tomography in schizophreniform disorder and other acute psychiatric disorders.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39778- 783Google Scholar
32.
Schulz  SCKoller  MMKishore  PRHamer  RM Ventricular enlargement in teenage patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1983;1401592- 1595Google Scholar
33.
Turner  SWToone  BKBrett-Jones  JR Computerized tomographic scan changes in early schizophrenia: preliminary findings.  Psychol Med. 1986;16219- 225Google Scholar
34.
Nyback  HWiesel  F-ABerggren  B-MHindmarsh  T Computed tomography of the brain in patients with acute psychosis and in healthy volunteers.  Acta Psychiat Scand. 1982;65403- 411Google Scholar
35.
MacDonald  HLBest  JJK The Scottish First Episode Schizophrenia Study, VI: computerised tomography brain scans in patients and controls.  Br J Psychiatry. 1989;154492- 498Google Scholar
36.
Nasrallah  HAOlson  SCMcCalley-Whitters  MChapman  SJacoby  CG Cerebral ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: a preliminary follow-up study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43157- 159Google Scholar
37.
Illowsky  BJuliano  DMBigelow  LBWeinberger  DR Stability of CT scan findings in schizophrenia: results of an 8-year follow-up study.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;51209- 213Google Scholar
38.
Reveley  MAChitkara  BLewis  S Ventricular and cranial size in schizophrenia: a 4- to 7-year follow-up.  Schizophr Res. 1988;1163Google Scholar
39.
Vita  ASacchetti  ECazzullo  CL A CT scan follow-up study of cerebral ventricular size in schizophrenia and major affective disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1988;1165- 166Google Scholar
40.
Pakkenberg  B Postmortem study of chronic schizophrenic brains.  Br J Psychiatry. 1987;151744- 752Google Scholar
41.
Selemon  LDRajkowska  GGoldman-Rakic  PS Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and occipital area 17.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52805- 818Google Scholar
42.
Lim  KOTew  WKushner  MChow  KMatsumoto  BDeLisi  LE Cortical gray matter deficit is present in first-episode schizophrenics.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;1531548- 1553Google Scholar
43.
Keshavan  MSPettegrew  JWBagwell  WWHaas  GLSweeney  JSchooler  NR Gray matter volume deficits in first-episode schizophrenia [abstract].  Biol Psychiatry. 1994;35713Google Scholar
44.
Sullivan  EVShear  PKLim  KOZipursky  RBPfefferbaum  A Cognitive and motor deficits impairments are related to gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39234- 240Google Scholar
45.
Andreasen  NCFlaum  MSwayze  V  IIO'Leary  DSAlliger  RCohen  GEhrhardt  JYuh  WTC Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150130- 134Google Scholar
46.
DeQuardo  JRGoldman  MTandon  R VBR in schizophrenia; relationship to family history of psychosis and season of birth.  Schizophr Res. 1996;20275- 285Google Scholar
47.
Farmer  AMcGuffin  PGottesman  II Problems and pitfalls of the family history positive and negative dichotomy: response to Dalen.  Schizophr Bull. 1990;16367- 370Google Scholar
48.
Rapaport  JGiedd  JKumra  SJacobsen  LSmith  ALee  PNelson  JHamburger  S Childhood-onset schizophrenia: progressive ventricular change during adolescence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54897- 903Google Scholar
49.
Roth  WTPfefferbaum  A Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe in schizophrenia.  N Engl J Med. 1992;3271689Google Scholar
Original Article
June 1998

Cerebral Gray Matter Volume Deficits in First Episode Psychosis

Author Affiliations

From the Clarke Institute of Psychiatry (Drs Zipursky and Kapur), The Toronto Hospital—Western Division (Dr Mikulis), and the Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Zipursky and Kapur) and Medical Imaging (Dr Mikulis), and the Institute of Medical Science (Ms Lambe), University of Toronto School of Medicine, Toronto, Ontario. Ms Lambe is now with the Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(6):540-546. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.540
Abstract

Background  Structural brain differences including decreased gray matter and increased cerebrospinal fluid volumes have been observed in the brains of chronically ill patients with schizophrenia. We hypothesized that deficits in gray matter volume would be present in patients presenting with a first episode of nonaffective psychosis.

Methods  We used magnetic resonance imaging to compare the brains of 77 patients assessed as having a first episode of psychosis (meeting DSM-III-R criteria for schizophrenia, schizophreniform disorder, schizoaffective disorder, delusional disorder, or psychotic disorder not otherwise specified) with those of 61 healthy controls matched for age, sex, race, and parental socioeconomic status. Axial, dual-echo scans of the whole brain were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid compartments using a computerized volumetric approach. These measures were corrected for the significant effects of intracranial volume and age prior to performing between-group comparisons.

Results  The first episode psychosis group had significantly smaller gray matter volume (t[136]=−2.2; P =.03) and greater cerebrospinal fluid volume (t[136]=2.5; P=.02) than normal controls. In the patient group, gray matter volumes were positively correlated with estimates of IQ but not with age of onset, duration of illness, or measures of premorbid functioning.

Conclusions  Deficits in gray matter volume are present in patients experiencing first episode nonaffective psychosis. The magnitude of these differences is smaller than has been described in more chronically ill patients.

INDIVIDUALS suffering from schizophrenia have been shown to have larger intracranial cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) volume1-5 and smaller cerebral gray matter volume,6-12 though some studies have failed to detect such differences.13-16 There is much interest in establishing when these differences arise and whether they progress over the course of the illness. It is also unclear whether these differences in brain structure are associated with clinical features of the illness such as presence of a family history of schizophrenia, response to treatment, duration of illness, cognitive deficits, exposure to antipsychotic medication, or long-term outcome.

The difficulties in establishing such relationships in part may be due to design limitations of previous studies. Difficulties arise in choosing optimal patient and control samples, in eliminating the possible confounding effects of substance abuse and antipsychotic medication, and in measurement of brain structures from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans.

Clinical correlates of brain structures in schizophrenia may be more readily identified in samples of patients experiencing their first episode of psychosis. The chance of identifying relationships between brain structures and outcome may be limited in studies of patients with chronic schizophrenia by the skewed range in outcome. As all patients with schizophrenia will have a first episode, the broadest range in outcome should be found in samples of patients identified at the time of their first episode.

This study was undertaken to determine whether structural brain abnormalities could be identified in patients experiencing first episode of nonaffective psychosis and to investigate whether significant clinical correlates could be identified.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

This study involved 3 subject groups: (1) a group of patients experiencing first episode psychosis, (2) a large control group used to calculate the correction factors for the effects of intracranial volume (ICV) and age on brain measures, and (3) a subset of this large control group selected as a matched comparison group for the patients who had first episode psychosis.

Consecutive referrals to the inpatient and outpatients services of the First Episode Psychosis Program at the Clarke Institute of Psychiatry were invited to participate in this study. The Clarke Institute is a tertiary care, university teaching hospital located in Toronto, Ontario. The protocol was approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the University of Toronto School of Medicine; all subjects provided written consent to participate. Patients were enrolled if they were experiencing a first episode of psychosis and were aged between16 and 50 years. For this study, patients were considered to have a first episode of psychosis if they were receiving treatment for the first time for an episode of psychosis. Efforts were made to study as many patients as possible prior to any exposure to antipsychotic medication.

Patients were excluded if the psychosis was determined to be due to an identifiable medical illness, drug use, or an underlying mood disorder; if they had a history of significant medical or neurological illness; or if they had ever met DSM-III-R17 criteria for substance dependence or abuse (other than for nicotine or caffeine). Subjects were also screened for any medical contraindications for MRI scanning.

A diagnosis was generated for each subject in the following way: A Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID)18 was completed for each subject based on an interview and review of clinical information by a trained research assistant, research fellow, or psychiatrist. The OPCRIT system19 was then used to document diagnostic criteria: to complete the OPCRIT forms, a process of consensus was used which included the principal investigator (R.B.Z.) and a research assistant or research fellow who had reviewed all clinical and research data. The OPCRIT program was then used to generate DSM-III-R diagnoses. This process was completed after each subject had undergone scanning as all efforts were made to study patients prior to antipsychotic treatment or as soon as possible in their treatment course.

In addition to the SCID, patients were also administered the Premorbid Adjustment Scale,20 the Family Interview for Genetic Studies,21 the National Adult Reading Test (NART)22,23 and the Quick Test24 to estimate intellectual ability. An estimate of lifetime alcohol consumption was obtained using the method described by Skinner.25 Total lifetime exposure to antipsychotic medication was estimated for all patients using medical records and patient self report. Dose for each antipsychotic agent received was multiplied by a conversion factor26 to yield dose expressed in milligrams of haloperidol per day and then multiplied by the number of days that dose was received; the sum of all antipsychotic medication ever received was then used as a measure of total lifetime antipsychotic exposure expressed in haloperidol equivalents. Age of onset was defined using the OPCRIT criteria ". . . the earliest age at which medical advice was sought for psychiatric reasons or at which symptoms began to cause subjective distress or impair functioning."19 Duration of illness was defined as the difference between the age at the MRI scan and the age of onset.

Eighty-four patients were recruited and underwent MRI scanning for this study. Seven patients were excluded from the analysis for the following reasons: 6 for a history of substance abuse and 1 in whom the diagnosis of bipolar disorder became clear shortly after the MRI scan was obtained.

The analysis included 77 patients experiencing first episode psychosis (Table 1). DSM-III-R diagnoses were as follows: 46 of the patients met criteria for schizophrenia, 16 for schizophreniform disorder, 10 for delusional disorder, 3 for psychotic disorder not otherwise specified, and 2 for schizoaffective disorder–depressive type. At the time of the MRI scan, 28 patients were neuroleptic-naive.

A group of 82 healthy control subjects (Table 1) were used to calculate the correction factors for the effects of ICV and age on the brain measures. Control subjects were administered the nonpatient version of the SCID and were excluded if they had a lifetime history of any major psychiatric disorder or a family history of schizophrenia. All other exclusion criteria were the same for controls as for patients. Eighty-five control subjects underwent MRI scanning and 3 were subsequently excluded from further study—1 subject for movement artifact, 1 for a positive family history of schizophrenia, and 1 for large patches of demyelination that subsequently led to a diagnosis of multiple sclerosis after the scan was read as being abnormal by a neuroradiologist (D.J.M.). These individuals were recruited from the community as normal control subjects for this study (n=52) and for concurrent studies in our program that involved an MRI scan (n=30).

From the large control group, 61 healthy adults were selected to form a comparison group, similar in sex, race, age, and parental socioeconomic status distribution to the group with first episode psychosis (Table 1). Parental socioeconomic status was determined using the 2-factor index (educational level and occupation) of social position developed by Hollingshead.27 The subject's father's educational level and occupation were used to calculate parental socioeconomic status unless the subject reported growing up with a single mother in the household.

Mri acquisition and analysis

All patients and controls were scanned using an MRI scanner (GE Signa 1.5 Tesla, General Electric Co, Milwaukee, Wis) at The Toronto Hospital—Western Division. Axial images were acquired using a dual echo spin-echo sequence (repetition time, 5000–5500 ms; echo time, 30-90 ms; field of view, 20 cm; and matrix, 256×192 pixels). The dual echo spin-echo sequence was required for carrying out the segmentation procedure as described later. The images were positioned perpendicular to the sagittal plane and parallel to a plane passing through both the anterior and posterior commissures6; between 40 and 43 contiguous sections of 3 mm thickness were collected. This analysis included all sections that did not have prominent areas of partially volumed white matter beginning at the inferior surface of the temporal lobes and extending to the vertex of the brain. The mean (SD) number of sections analyzed per subject did not differ for the patient group (mean, 32.6 [1.7]) and control group (mean, 32.4 [1.8]).

Images were transferred from MRI scanner to workstations (Macintosh Quadra 950, Apple, Cupertino, Calif) and reformatted to a 256×256-pixel matrix. The images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF compartments with BRAINIMAGE software.28 The steps involved in image preparation are described in detail in Katzman et al.29 Each brain was processed and segmented by 1 of 2 trained research assistants who have established high intrarater and interrater reliability; intraclass correlations ranged from 0.94 to 1.00 for all measures.30 Sections at and superior to the appearance of the frontal lobes were divided into an inner 55% portion and an outer 45% ring for more accurate segmentation.6 After the images were segmented, the cerebellum, brainstem, vermis, and surrounding cisterns were removed.29

Measurements from the outer rings, the inner portions, and the inferior whole sections were summed to give total CSF and gray matter and white matter volumes. The CSF measurements from the outer rings were added to those from the inferior whole sections and the inner portions superior to the ventricles to give the volume of "sulcal" CSF. The lateral ventricles, third ventricle, and temporal and posterior horns were then identified and summed to give the volume of ventricular CSF.

Statistical analysis

Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was first applied to the 82 control scans to determine whether scans from male and female subjects could be pooled. There was a significant effect of sex on ICV. When the main effect of ICV was entered into the model first, there was no effect of sex on CSF or gray matter or white matter volumes. Nor were there significant age-by-sex interactions in this model. Therefore, MRI scans from male and female controls were pooled to form the normal control group.

Analysis of covariance was then used to test for main effects of ICV, age,sex, and diagnosis on each of the brain measures, as well as interactions between diagnosis and the effects of these variables. Intracranial volume and age each yielded significant main effects consistent with the age regression model described later. Because sex did not, it was removed from the ANCOVA, as were the nonsignificant interaction terms between diagnosis and ICV, diagnosis and age, and diagnosis and sex. Group differences as assessed by ANCOVA were used to confirm the results from the age regression approach described later.

An age regression approach6 was adopted to assess group differences and to facilitate investigation of clinical correlates. In the large control group, ICV was significantly correlated with total CSF volume (r[81]=0.54, P≤.001), sulcal CSF volume (r[81]=0.52, P ≤.001), ventricular CSF volume (r[81]=0.25, P≤.05), total gray matter volume (r[81]=0.93, P≤.001), and total white matter volume (r[81]=0.91, P≤.001). After correcting for the effects of ICV on these measures, age remained significantly correlated with total CSF volume (r[81]=0.37, P≤.001), sulcal CSF volume (r[81]=0.36, P≤.001), total gray matter volume (r[81]= − 0.69, P≤.001), total white matter volume (r[81]=0.41, P≤.001) (Figure 1) but not ventricular volume (r[81]=0.20, P≤.10). ICV- and age-corrected residuals were calculated for all the brain measures based on linear regression analysis of the large control group.6 The z-scores were then calculated by dividing these residuals by the SD of the large control group.

Student t tests were used to assess significance when the z-scores for total gray matter, total CSF, and total white matter volumes were compared between the group with first episode psychosis (n=77) and the age-matched control group (n=61). As the primary objective of this study was to determine whether patients experiencing a first episode of psychosis had deficits in brain gray matter volume, this hypothesis was tested at the P=.05 level of statistical significance, 2 tailed. All other analyses were considered exploratory.

Within the group with first episode psychosis, we used Pearson product moment correlations to test for significant relationships between the gray matter z-scores and each of the following: age at onset, duration of illness, Premorbid Adjustment Scale subscales, NART scores, and Quick Test scores. Because we also had Quick Test and NART IQ measures for many (n=46) of the controls, we used ANCOVA to test for an interaction between the effects of diagnosis and IQ on the corrected gray matter volumes. IQ measures were only available for subjects who were proficient at English. Brain measures for patients considered to have a definite family history of schizophrenia in a first- or second-degree family member (n=13) were compared with those of patients with a negative family history (n=63) using the Student t test.

Results
Brain measures

The group with first episode psychosis had significantly smaller total gray matter volume (t[136]=−.2, P=.03) and significantly greater total CSF volume (t[136]=2.5, P=.02) when compared with the age-matched, normal control group. (Table 2, Figure 2) The patient group did not differ significantly from the control group on total white matter volume (t[136]= −0.4, P =.68). Further analysis of CSF volume showed that patients with first episode psychosis had significantly larger ventricular CSF volume (t[136]=2.9, P =.005) and showed a trend toward larger sulcal CSF volume (t[136]=1.7, P=.09).

The subset of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (n=46) also had significantly smaller total gray matter volume (t[105]= −2.8, P =.005) and significantly greater total CSF volume (t[105]=2.3, P=.03) when compared with the age-matched normal control group (Table 2). Lateral ventricles were significantly larger in the schizophrenia group (t[105]=3.3, P=.002) but sulcal volume did not differ significantly. White matter volume did not differ between the 2 groups (t[105]=.25, P =.8).

Significant differences in gray matter and CSF volumes but not white matter volume were confirmed using ANCOVA for both the total patient group and the subgroup diagnosed with schizophrenia.

Clinical correlates

In the patient group, there was a significant correlation between Quick Test IQ and the gray matter z-scores (r[52]=0.39, P=.004). Among the controls, however, we found no such correlation (r[45]= − 0.13, P=.38). Analysis of covariance revealed a significant interaction between the effects of diagnosis and IQ on the gray matter z-scores (F[98]=6.7, P=.002). There was also a trend for NART scores to be correlated with gray matter z-scores in patients (r[51]=0.24, P=.08) but not in controls (r[45]=0.14, P =.34); ANCOVA revealed a significant diagnosis by NART score interaction on gray matter z-scores (F[97]=7.5, P=.001). Quick Test IQ and NART scores were significantly correlated in both the patient (r[51]=0.58, P<.001) and control groups (r[45]=0.65, P<.001).

Gray matter z-scores were not significantly correlated with age of onset (r[76]=0.15, P=.20), duration of illness (r[76]= −0.10, P=.41) or with any of the subscales of the Premorbid Adjustment Scale. Patients with a definite family history of schizophrenia (n=13) did not differ significantly from those without (t[74]= − 1.1, P=.27). Estimates of lifetime alcohol consumption were not correlated with gray matter z-scores in the patient (r [74]=0.08, P =.51) or control groups (r [42]=0.10, P=.53).

Comment

The finding of significant deficits in gray matter volume in patients presenting with first episode nonaffective psychosis supports the view that the brain abnormalities found in patients with schizophrenia are present at the onset of psychotic symptoms. This is consistent with results of computed tomography and MRI studies in schizophrenia that have used first episode samples31-35 as well as longitudinal designs.36-39

The finding of smaller gray matter volume in patients with first episode nonaffective psychosis adds further support to the finding that deficits in gray matter volume may underlie the CSF volume increase reported in earlier computed tomography and pneumoencephalography studies. The finding of smaller gray matter volume has reported by many independent research groups in chronically ill samples.6,7,9,10,12 Using postmortem samples, Pakkenburg40 has described cortical volume deficits and Selemon et al41 have recently reported evidence for reduced cortical thickness (in the absence of reduced neuron number).

Significant deficits in gray matter volume in patients with a first episode of psychosis have also been described in recent reports by Lim et al42 and Keshavan et al.43 This study differs in important ways from the study by Lim et al. for clinical and MRI considerations. Lim et al42 studied a smaller group of patients (n=22), included comparison subjects who underwent MRI scanning for neurological complaints, and did not include neuroleptic-naive patients. Their image analysis was restricted to 7 noncontiguous MRI sections of 5 mm thickness. Keshavan et al43 studied patients with first episode nonaffective psychosis who were treatment-naive but the sample included only 13 patients.

Gray matter volume deficits were found to be correlated with 2 independent estimates of intellectual ability, the Quick Test and the NART, in the patient group but not the control group. Furthermore, we found significant diagnosis-by-Quick Test and diagnosis-by-NART interactions on gray matter z-scores suggesting that the relationship between cognitive functioning and gray matter volume is substantially different in patients from controls. It should be kept in mind that the NART and Quick Test provide only estimates of cognitive ability and do not provide information about specific deficits which may be of importance in schizophrenia. In keeping with our finding, Sullivan et al44 have also reported that gray matter z-scores in patients with chronic schizophrenia were significantly correlated with regional and global measures of cognitive function. Whether similar associations are present in healthy controls remains controversial.45

We did not detect other significant clinical correlates of gray matter deficits in this study. Deficits in gray matter volumes did not differ in those with a family history of schizophrenia or a history of major psychiatric illness compared with those without. In this study, family history was obtained whenever possible by interviewing a single first-degree family member; it would have been optimal to have interviewed all first-degree family members directly. However, studies relating the presence of a family history of schizophrenia to structural brain measures have not led to a consensus (for review see DeQuardo et al46). It may well be that there is little value in attempting to make such a distinction as has been suggested by Farmer et al.47

We did not find any associations between duration of illness or age of onset and the magnitude of gray matter deficits. Lim et al10 also failed to find these associations in a recent study that involved state hospital patients with a very broad range in the age at onset and duration of illness. Rapaport et,48 however, have found that during a 2-year period ventricular size increased to a greater extent in children and adolescents with schizophrenia than in healthy controls of similar age. Given the large sample size and the considerable range of durations of illness in both this study and the Lim et al10 study, it is unlikely that the magnitude of the deficits in gray matter volume increase during the long-term course of the illness. That some limited degree of change might occur in the early years of the illness cannot be ruled out.

Most patients in this study had little or no antipsychotic exposure and yet had brain abnormalities qualitatively similar to those reported in patients who have been treated for many years. Among those patients who met criteria for schizophrenia, those who were neuroleptic-naive had a mean gray matter effect size of − 0.42. It is, therefore, unlikely that the gray matter volume deficits and CSF volume increases reported in patients with schizophrenia are a consequence of antipsychotic treatment. The range of antipsychotic exposure in this study was, however, very limited. To conclusively rule out the possibility that antipsychotic exposure contributes to the magnitude of structural brain abnormalities, one would need to carry out a prospective longitudinal follow-up MRI study of patients with a first episode of psychosis as it is critical in addressing this question that there be both brain measures prior to any exposure as well as substantial variance in total antipsychotic exposure.

The magnitude of the gray matter deficits in this sample was much smaller than the effect sizes reported in patients with more chronic forms of the illness.6,9,10 The results from this study of a mean gray matter effect size of − 0.4 for all patients and − 0.6 for those meeting DSM-III-R criteria for schizophrenia is consistent with the recent MRI findings of Lim et al42 who found a gray matter effect size of approximately − 0.5 in a group of first episode patients who later had a diagnosis of schizophrenia confirmed. Zipursky et al6 found a gray matter effect size of − 1.15 in 22 patients with chronic schizophrenia from an American Veteran's Administration Medical Center; the results of this study were replicated by Lim et al9 who found an effect size of − 1.18. These findings suggest that more chronically ill patients have greater deficits in gray matter volume. Whether this reflects a process of deterioration or a selection bias in studies of patients with chronic schizophrenia remains to be clarified.

There are many considerations that limit the interpretation of this study. While none of the patients met criteria for a primary mood disorder at the time of the study, longitudinal follow-up is necessary to confirm the diagnoses. Longitudinal follow-up of these patients (in progress) will determine whether variability in diagnoses over time might also explain the smaller gray matter effect size reported in this sample. That those subjects who did not meet diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia had smaller gray matter deficits than those with schizophrenia is consistent with this possibility. Establishing the age of onset and in turn the duration of illness is an undertaking fraught with uncertainty; the error inherent in this determination may have contributed to our inability to detect associations between these variables and MRI measures. However, even when the duration of illness was calculated using the onset of the prodrome or the onset of psychotic symptoms as criteria, no associations with gray matter z -scores were detected. Measures of clinical symptoms were excluded in this analysis as patients were studied at different times in their treatment course ranging from acutely psychotic and untreated to fully recovered with treatment. It remains unclear how to meaningfully assess the relationship between clinical symptoms that are highly variable over short periods with brain measures that are relatively fixed over the same time span.49 Finally, this study does not provide information about the regional distribution of gray matter deficits. Previous studies6,8,9 have reported deficits in multiple cortical areas with a gradient in the magnitude of deficits reflecting more striking deficits in frontal and temporal areas. This is being explored using MRIs acquired from this sample of subjects using a 3-dimensional radiofrequency spoiled gradient recalled echo sequence.

This study confirms the presence of significant deficits in cerebral gray matter volume and increases in CSF volume in patients presenting with a first episode of nonaffective psychosis. It is unlikely that this finding can be attributed to either antipsychotic treatment or to the duration of time the illness has gone untreated. This finding is most consistent with the view that the structural brain abnormalities found in schizophrenia predate the onset of psychosis.

Accepted for publication January 14, 1998.

This study was support in part by the Medical Research Council of Canada, Ottawa, Ontario (Drs Zipursky and Mikulis) and the Canadian Psychiatric Research Foundation, Toronto, Ontario (Drs Zipursky and Mikulis).

We gratefully acknowledge the efforts of the nursing staff of the Clarke First Episode Psychosis Program whose great effort made this study possible. We also thank staff of the MRI Centre at the Toronto Hospital–Western Division. We are grateful to Edmond Chong, BSc, for his assistance with all aspects of image processing, to Chris Jaskulski, MA, and Yvonne Grot, BA, for the collection of clinical information, and to Brigid Swensen, BA, for analyzing many of the MRI scans. We also thank Jiahui Zhang-Wong, MD, Graham Bean, PhD, and Morton Beiser, MD, for their assistance in the diagnostic process and Allan L. Reiss, MD, for making BRAINIMAGE (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Human Brain Project, HD31715) available to us and assisting in its implementation.

Corresponding author: Robert B. Zipursky, MD, Clarke Institute of Psychiatry, 250 College St, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5T 1R8 (e-mail: rz@sig.clarke-inst.on.ca).

References
1.
Johnstone  ECCrow  TJFrith  DCHusband  JKrel  L Cerebral ventricular size and cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.  Lancet. 1976;2924- 926Google Scholar
2.
Weinberger  DRTorrey  EFNeophytides  ANWyatt  RJ Structural abnormalities in the cerebral cortex of chronic schizophrenic patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36935- 939Google Scholar
3.
Pfefferbaum  AZipursky  RBLim  KOZatz  LMStahl  SMJernigan  TL Computed tomographic evidence for generalized sulcal and ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45633- 640Google Scholar
4.
Pearlson  GDKim  WSKubos  KLMoberg  PJJayaram  GBascom  MJChase  GAGoldfinger  ADTune  LE Ventricle-brain ratio, computed tomographic density and brain area in 50 schizophrenics.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46690- 697Google Scholar
5.
Andreasen  NCSmith  MRJocoby  CGDennert  JWOlsen  SA Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: definition and prevalence.  Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139292- 302Google Scholar
6.
Zipursky  RBLim  KOSullivan  EVBrown  BWPfefferbaum  A Widespread cerebral gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49195- 205Google Scholar
7.
Harvey  IRon  MAdu Bouley  GWicks  DLewis  SWMurray  RM Reduction of cortical volume in schizophrenia on magnetic resonance imaging.  Psychol Med. 1993;23591- 604Google Scholar
8.
Schlaepfer  THarris  GJTien  AYPeng  LWLee  SFederman  EBChase  GABarta  PEPearlson  GD Decreased regional cortical gray matter volume in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151842- 848Google Scholar
9.
Lim  KOSullivan  EVZipursky  RBPfefferbaum  A Cortical gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia: a replication.  Schizophr Res. 1996;20157- 164Google Scholar
10.
Lim  KHarris  DBeal  MHoff  ALMinn  KCsernansky  JGFaustman  WOMarsh  LSullivan  EVPfefferbaum  A Gray matter deficits in young onset schizophrenia are independent of age of onset.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;404- 13Google Scholar
11.
Woods  BYurgelun-Todd  DGoldstein  JMSeidman  LJTsuang  MT MRI brain abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia: one process or more?  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40585- 596Google Scholar
12.
Zipursky  RSeeman  MVBury  ALangevin  RWortzman  GKatz  R Deficits in gray matter volume are present in schizophrenia but not bipolar disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1997;2685- 92Google Scholar
13.
Jernigan  TLZatz  LMMoses  JABerger  PA Computed tomography in schizophrenics and normal volunteers, I: fluid volume.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39765- 770Google Scholar
14.
Jernigan  TLZisook  SHeaton  RKMoranville  JTHesselink  JRBraff  DL Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48881- 890Google Scholar
15.
Buchanan  RWBreier  AKirkpatrick  BElkashef  AMunson  RCGellad  FCarpenter  WT Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;15059- 65Google Scholar
16.
Barta  PPearlson  GDBrill  LBRoyall  RMcGilchrist  IKPulver  AEPowers  RECasanova  MFTien  AYFrangou  SPetty  RG Planum temporale asymmetry reversal in schizophrenia: replication and relationship to gray matter abnormalities.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154661- 667Google Scholar
17.
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM-III-R).  Washington, D American Psychiatric Association1987;
18.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  MFirst  MB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R - Patient Edition (With Psychotic Screen).  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press Inc1990;
19.
McGuffin  PFarmer  AHarvey  I A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;47764- 770Google Scholar
20.
Cannon-Spoor  HEPotkin  SGWyatt  RJ Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1982;8470- 484Google Scholar
21.
Maxwell  ME Manual for the FIGS.  Bethesda, Md Clinical Neurogenetics Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health1992;
22.
Ocarroll  RWalker  MDunan  JMurray  CBlackwood  DEbmeier  KPGoodwin  GM Selecting controls for schizophrenia research studies: the use of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) is a measure of premorbid ability.  Schizophr Res. 1992;8137- 141Google Scholar
23.
Nelson  HE National Adult Reading Test, NART.  Windsor, Ontario Nelson Publishing Co1982;
24.
Ammons  RBAmmons  CH The Quick Test (QT): Provisional Manual.  Psychological Rep. 1962;11111- 161Google Scholar
25.
Skinner  HA Development and validation of a lifetime alcohol consumption assessment procedure.  Toronto, Canada Addiction Research Foundation1982;
26.
American Psychiatric Association, Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;1541- 63Google Scholar
27.
Hollingshead  AB Two-Factor Index of Social Position.  New Haven, Conn Yale University1957;
28.
Reiss  ALHennessey  JSubramanium  BRubin  MBeach  L BRAINIMAGE.  Baltimore, Md Kennedy Krieger Institute Neuroimaging Research Center1995;
29.
Katzman  DBLambe  EKMikulis  DJRidgley  JNGoldbloom  DSZipursky  RB Cerebral gray matter and white matter volume deficits in adolescent girls with anorexia nervosa.  J Pediatr. 1996;129794- 803Google Scholar
30.
Lambe  EKKatzman  DKMikulis  DJKennedy  SHZipursky  RB Cerebral gray matter volume deficits after weight recovery from anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54537- 542Google Scholar
31.
Weinberger  DRDeLisi  LEPerman  GPTargum  SWyatt  RJ Computed tomography in schizophreniform disorder and other acute psychiatric disorders.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39778- 783Google Scholar
32.
Schulz  SCKoller  MMKishore  PRHamer  RM Ventricular enlargement in teenage patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1983;1401592- 1595Google Scholar
33.
Turner  SWToone  BKBrett-Jones  JR Computerized tomographic scan changes in early schizophrenia: preliminary findings.  Psychol Med. 1986;16219- 225Google Scholar
34.
Nyback  HWiesel  F-ABerggren  B-MHindmarsh  T Computed tomography of the brain in patients with acute psychosis and in healthy volunteers.  Acta Psychiat Scand. 1982;65403- 411Google Scholar
35.
MacDonald  HLBest  JJK The Scottish First Episode Schizophrenia Study, VI: computerised tomography brain scans in patients and controls.  Br J Psychiatry. 1989;154492- 498Google Scholar
36.
Nasrallah  HAOlson  SCMcCalley-Whitters  MChapman  SJacoby  CG Cerebral ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: a preliminary follow-up study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43157- 159Google Scholar
37.
Illowsky  BJuliano  DMBigelow  LBWeinberger  DR Stability of CT scan findings in schizophrenia: results of an 8-year follow-up study.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;51209- 213Google Scholar
38.
Reveley  MAChitkara  BLewis  S Ventricular and cranial size in schizophrenia: a 4- to 7-year follow-up.  Schizophr Res. 1988;1163Google Scholar
39.
Vita  ASacchetti  ECazzullo  CL A CT scan follow-up study of cerebral ventricular size in schizophrenia and major affective disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1988;1165- 166Google Scholar
40.
Pakkenberg  B Postmortem study of chronic schizophrenic brains.  Br J Psychiatry. 1987;151744- 752Google Scholar
41.
Selemon  LDRajkowska  GGoldman-Rakic  PS Abnormally high neuronal density in the schizophrenic cortex: a morphometric analysis of prefrontal area 9 and occipital area 17.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52805- 818Google Scholar
42.
Lim  KOTew  WKushner  MChow  KMatsumoto  BDeLisi  LE Cortical gray matter deficit is present in first-episode schizophrenics.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;1531548- 1553Google Scholar
43.
Keshavan  MSPettegrew  JWBagwell  WWHaas  GLSweeney  JSchooler  NR Gray matter volume deficits in first-episode schizophrenia [abstract].  Biol Psychiatry. 1994;35713Google Scholar
44.
Sullivan  EVShear  PKLim  KOZipursky  RBPfefferbaum  A Cognitive and motor deficits impairments are related to gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39234- 240Google Scholar
45.
Andreasen  NCFlaum  MSwayze  V  IIO'Leary  DSAlliger  RCohen  GEhrhardt  JYuh  WTC Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150130- 134Google Scholar
46.
DeQuardo  JRGoldman  MTandon  R VBR in schizophrenia; relationship to family history of psychosis and season of birth.  Schizophr Res. 1996;20275- 285Google Scholar
47.
Farmer  AMcGuffin  PGottesman  II Problems and pitfalls of the family history positive and negative dichotomy: response to Dalen.  Schizophr Bull. 1990;16367- 370Google Scholar
48.
Rapaport  JGiedd  JKumra  SJacobsen  LSmith  ALee  PNelson  JHamburger  S Childhood-onset schizophrenia: progressive ventricular change during adolescence.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54897- 903Google Scholar
49.
Roth  WTPfefferbaum  A Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe in schizophrenia.  N Engl J Med. 1992;3271689Google Scholar
×