Background
Symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been associated with frontal lobe deficits. We used a novel brain electrical imaging method to investigate rapid and continuous changes in brain activity during the continuous performance task (CPT) in normal boys and in boys with ADHD. The amplitude and latency topography of the steady-state visually evoked potential (SSVEP) were examined while subjects performed the "X" version of the CPT (CPT-X; the reference task) and the "A-X" version of the CPT (CPT-AX).
Methods
Seventeen boys meeting DSM-III-R criteria for ADHD and 17 age-matched controls participated in the study. Brain electrical activity was recorded from 64 scalp sites. During the reference task, subjects pressed a microswitch on the unpredictable appearance of the letter X. During the CPT-AX, subjects were required to press the microswitch on the appearance of the letter X only if an A had preceded it.
Results
In the interval between the appearances of the A and the X of the correct trials of the CPT-AX, control boys showed transient reductions in SSVEP latency at right prefrontal sites. By contrast, boys with ADHD showed no change or an increase in prefrontal SSVEP latency at right prefrontal sites.
Conclusion
Our results suggest increased speed of prefrontal neural processing in children without ADHD following a priming stimulus, and a deficit in such processes in children with ADHD.
ATTENTION-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most commonly diagnosed psychiatric conditions of childhood, with an estimated incidence of 3% to 6%.1 Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by developmentally inappropriate levels of inattentiveness, impulsivity, and overactivity. Neuropsychological findings suggest that a failure to inhibit motor responses may constitute the primary deficit in ADHD.2 Such findings have been interpreted in terms of prefrontal lobe deficits.3-5
Functional brain imaging studies have also pointed to prefrontal lobe abnormalities. Single photon emission computed tomographic studies in ADHD have demonstrated hypoperfusion of the central frontal lobes and caudate nucleus and reduced prefrontal blood flow in response to cognitive tasks.6-8 In a positron emission tomographic (PET) study of adults with a history of ADHD, Zametkin et al9 reported reduced increases in premotor and sensorimotor glucose metabolism when subjects performed an auditory continuous performance task (CPT). However, a subsequent study of teenagers with ADHD failed to statistically differentiate the ADHD group from the control group.10
Some of the disparate PET neuroimaging findings could be the result of several factors, such as small population size and different cognitive activation tasks, but one important feature could be the poor temporal resolution of the brain imaging methods used. 18F-fluoro-2-deoxy-D-glucose PET has a temporal resolution of 40 minutes typically, while oxygen 15–labeled water enables a resolution of approximately 60 seconds.11,12 By contrast, a cognitive activation task may make demands on compromised cortical regions for only certain brief intervals. Thus, PET imaging may only yield an averaged representation of cortical activity that may grossly underestimate important regional changes of a transient nature.
In evoked potential studies of ADHD, normal control subjects have demonstrated evoked potential differences between attended and nonattended stimuli, whereas subjects with ADHD show smaller or nonsignificant evoked potential differences.13 While evoked potential findings point to deficits in a range of attentional mechanisms in ADHD, it is more difficult to determine the neurophysiological and neuroanatomical substrates of the disorder from evoked potential recordings alone.
Previous work by 1 of us (R.B.S.) suggests that steady-state probe topography, a novel method using the steady-state visually evoked potential (SSVEP), offers the opportunity of high temporal resolution analysis of brain electrical correlates of extended tasks coupled with noise resistance.14,15 We have reported strong cognitive task effects on the SSVEP when the eliciting stimulus comprises a uniform visual flicker superimposed on the computer monitor used to present the cognitive task. Increases in visual vigilance, for example, were associated with SSVEP amplitude reductions at occipitoparietal sites while a planning task yielded SSVEP amplitude reductions at prefrontal sites.15 Findings yielded by this technique appear analogous to the regional reductions in alpha activity associated with cognitive tasks.16 Such transient reductions in alpha activity have been used as indicators of increased regional activity,16 and the corresponding reductions in SSVEP amplitude may also indicate increased regional activity.
In a steady-state probe topographic study of the A-X version of the CPT (CPT-AX), where subjects are required to make a response on the unpredictable appearance of the letter X if preceded by the letter A, it was found that the appearances of the A and X were associated with transient reductions in prefrontal SSVEP amplitude and latency.17
While the amplitude reduction is consistent with increased regional activity, the latency reductions suggest increased neural information processing speed, possibly due to an increase in the efficiency of interactions between prefrontal neural networks. In light of our previous CPT findings and other studies suggesting CPT performance deficits in ADHD (for a review see Corkum and Siegel18), we undertook a steady-state probe topographic study of attentional processes in ADHD using the CPT-X (response on the unpredictable appearance of X) and CPT-AX. We reasoned that, when using the CPT-X as a reference task, the appearance of the A in the CPT-AX would act to prime attentional processes in anticipation of the possible appearance of a target X. We hypothesized that this priming effect would be apparent in the prefrontal region and that subjects with ADHD would demonstrate reduced prefrontal priming that would be most apparent in the interval between the appearances of the A and X.
The study was approved by the Human Experimentation Ethics Committee of Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia, and by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Twin Registry. For each individual subject, informed consent was obtained from a parent.
All subjects who participated in this study were male, right-handed as assessed using the Edinburgh Inventory,19 and had a full-scale IQ of at least 80 as estimated with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition20 using the Block Design and Vocabulary short form as recommended by Sattler.21 Seventeen boys with ADHD (mean age, 129 months; age range, 88-168 months; mean IQ, 96; IQ range, 80-115) were diagnosed as having ADHD by pediatricians and met DSM-III-R criteria22 for ADHD according to maternal and/or teacher reports obtained using the Australian Twin Behaviour Rating Scale (ATBRS).23,24 Comorbidity with disruptive behavior and learning disorders was also assessed by the ATBRS. Twelve boys with ADHD (71%) met criteria for oppositional defiant disorder, 6 (35%) met criteria for conduct disorder, and 15 (88%) had a history of learning difficulties. The 15 subjects with ADHD who were being treated with stimulant medication remained medication free for at least 24 hours before their brain electrical activity recording. Seventeen healthy control subjects (mean age, 132 months; age range, 104-159 months; mean IQ, 111; IQ range, 88-152) were rated free of ADHD, disruptive behaviors, and learning difficulties according to maternal and teacher reports obtained using the ATBRS.
All subjects first performed a low-demand visual vigilance task, followed by the CPT-X (reference task) and the CPT-AX. In the reference task, subjects were required to press a microswitch on the unpredictable appearance of an X, while in the CPT-AX they were required to respond on the unpredictable appearance of an X that had been preceded by an A. In all tasks, the letters remained on the screen for 2 seconds and were followed by a blank screen for 1.5 seconds. The ratio of targets to nontargets was 1:4 and the task duration was 280 seconds. Reaction time was recorded to an accuracy of 1 millisecond. For all tasks, a correct response to a target was defined as one that occurred no less than 100 milliseconds and no more than 1.5 seconds after the appearance of the target (X or X preceded by an A). Any responses outside the "correct" time intervals were defined as errors of commission, or false alarms, while failure to respond in the correct interval was defined as an error of omission.
The cognitive tasks were presented on a computer monitor. Each letter subtended a horizontal and vertical angle of approximately 1.0° when viewed by subjects from a fixed distance of 1.3 m. The stimulus used to evoke the SSVEP was a spatially diffuse 13-Hz sinusoidal flicker subtending a horizontal angle of 160° and a vertical angle of 90°, which was superimposed on the visual fields. This flicker was present throughout the task and special goggles enabled subjects to view the cognitive task and the sinusoidal flicker simultaneously.25
Brain electrical activity was recorded from 64 scalp sites that included all international 10-20 positions, with additional sites located midway between 10-20 locations. The specific locations of the recording sites have been previously described.25 The average potential of both earlobes served as a reference and a nose electrode served as a ground. Brain electrical activity was amplified and bandpass filtered (3 dB down at 0.1 Hz and 30 Hz) before digitization to 12-bit accuracy at a rate of 200 Hz.
The major features of the signal processing have been described.25,26 Briefly, the SSVEP was determined from the 13-Hz Fourier coefficients evaluated over 10 stimulus cycles at the stimulus frequency of 13 Hz, thus yielding a temporal resolution of 0.77 seconds. The 10-cycle evaluation period was shifted 1 stimulus cycle and the coefficients were recalculated for this overlapping period. This process was continued until the entire 280 seconds of activity was analyzed. An identical procedure was applied to data recorded from all 64 recording sites.
To assess the changes in the SSVEP associated with different components of the cognitive tasks, the following procedure was used. Ten-second epochs of SSVEP real and imaginary components centered on the appearance of a target were averaged, for all correct responses to the targets in both the reference task and CPT-AX. These complex time series were then averaged across the subject pool. The complex SSVEP time series was then expressed in terms of SSVEP amplitude and the phase difference between the visual sinusoidal stimulus and the SSVEP. Variations in the phase were expressed in terms of latency variations.
Artifact detection and compensation
A specific advantage of the SSVEP is its relative noise and artifact insensitivity.14 This is of special importance in a patient population with compromised impulse control and a limited ability to comply with the constraints of brain electrical activity recording. The electroencephalogram and SSVEP time series were subjected to several tests to ascertain whether the artifact levels had exceeded a predetermined threshold level. These tests have been described previously.26
Statistical analyses and topographic mapping
Topographic maps of the difference between the mean values of amplitude and latency for correct responses in the reference task (CPT-X), and the time series for correct responses centered on the appearance of the target X in the A-X sequence of the CPT-AX, were produced using a spherical spline interpolation procedure.27
Statistical parametric mapping based on a multivariate permutation test using the Hotelling T2 statistic was used to illustrate the topography of the statistical strength of the effect. The use of multivariate permutation tests to evaluate differences in event-related potential topography was first suggested by Blair and Karniski.28,29 With the availability of inexpensive and powerful computers, these have become increasingly popular in the field of functional brain imaging as they are distribution free, require no assumptions about the underlying correlation structure of the data, and produce exact P values for any number of subjects and observations (time points and electrodes).30
In this study, a multivariate permutation test based on the Hotelling T2 statistic was used to estimate the probability of falsely rejecting the null hypothesis (type I error) associated with task differences in the SSVEP amplitude and phase. Specifically, the multivariate permutation test was used to compare the SSVEP observed during the reference task with that of the CPT-AX. These tests were conducted at 3 points during the CPT-AX: at the appearance of the A, the disappearance of the A, and the appearance of the X. Thus, for each population, 192 (64 recording sites by 3 time points) null hypotheses were tested. It should be noted that the multivariate permutation test explicitly takes into account the correlation between SSVEP values at different recording sites and points in time and thus yields exact P values corrected for multiple comparisons.30,31
A Mann-Whitney U test was performed to compare IQ scores for the 2 groups. As the mean IQ of the ADHD group was significantly lower than that of the control group (U17,17=72.5; P<.05), we selected 2 subgroups of matched mean IQ that included all subjects with estimated IQs in the "average" range of 85 to 115 (control subgroup: mean IQ=102, SD=9.0, n=12; ADHD subgroup: mean IQ=99, SD=8.6, n=15; U12,15=72.5; P>.3) to determine whether the effects apparent in the entire groups persisted in the IQ-matched subgroups. The following procedure was adopted. The SSVEP amplitude and latency differences between the CPT-AX and the mean of the reference task were calculated for the entire control group (n=17) and the entire ADHD group (n=17). This procedure was repeated for the IQ-matched control subgroup (n=12) and the IQ-matched ADHD subgroup (n=15). This yielded 2 sets of SSVEP waveforms, 1 for the total control and ADHD populations, and 1 for the IQ-matched subgroups. The control group waveforms were subtracted from the ADHD group waveforms for both the entire groups and the IQ-matched subgroups. This yielded 2 sets of group difference waveforms, 1 for the entire group and 1 for the IQ-matched subgroups. The correlation coefficient between the total group difference waveforms and the IQ-matched difference waveforms was evaluated at each of the 64 recording sites.
A high correlation coefficient would mean that the differences apparent in the entire group are also apparent in the IQ-matched subgroups and the SSVEP differences between populations are unlikely to be a consequence of the differences in mean IQ.
Eight 1-way analyses of variance were performed to compare the task performance of the 2 groups (Table 1). The control group demonstrated faster reaction times than the ADHD group, although this difference was only significant for the reference task. More correct trials were averaged for the control group than for the ADHD group and the control group made fewer errors of omission than the ADHD group, but these differences were not significant. The ADHD group made significantly more errors of commission in both tasks.
Brain electrical activity
The SSVEP amplitude and latency demonstrated variations that were synchronized with the various components of the CPT-AX.
For controls, the SSVEP amplitude in the interval between the appearance and disappearance of the A was generally below the mean value for the reference task at a central prefrontal site (Fz). This suggests increased regional activity in this interval relative to the reference task (Figure 1). By contrast, subjects with ADHD demonstrated an increase in prefrontal SSVEP amplitude, suggesting reduced activation during this interval.
In control subjects, the appearances of the A and X, and more prominently the disappearance of the A, were associated with a reduction in SSVEP latency relative to the reference task at the same prefrontal site (Figure 2). By contrast, the ADHD group demonstrated an increase in SSVEP latency at this prefrontal site on the disappearance of the A.
The group difference SSVEP amplitude waveforms for the entire populations (solid line, Figure 3) and the IQ-matched subgroups (dashed line, Figure 3) are similar, with a corresponding correlation coefficient of 0.87. This similarity extends to the SSVEP latency waveforms (Figure 4), with a correlation coefficient of 0.83.
The SSVEP amplitude and latency waveforms were similar at all of the 64 recording sites (mean correlation coefficient for amplitude waveforms, 0.86; mean correlation coefficient for latency waveforms, 0.88), suggesting that the effects observed for the entire group are not a consequence of the differences in mean IQ between control and ADHD groups.
Topographic distributions
In the control group (Figure 5), the points "A on," "A off," and "X on" were associated with reductions in SSVEP latency relative to the reference task mean, although these effects were only statistically significant at right prefrontal sites at the appearance (P<.05) and disappearance (P<.005) of the A. By contrast, in the ADHD group (Figure 6), there were increases in SSVEP latency at central and prefrontal sites although these changes did not reach statistical significance (P>.05).
For the first time, differences in SSVEP latency were observed between controls and subjects with ADHD at 3 critical points in time of the CPT-AX. Control subjects demonstrated an SSVEP latency reduction at right prefrontal sites coinciding with the appearances of the A and X and the disappearance of the A. We have previously suggested that such a latency reduction may reflect increased efficiency of coupling between prefrontal neural networks. Such an interpretation is consistent with our previous findings that faster responses in the CPT-AX were associated with larger prefrontal SSVEP latency reductions.17 Compared with the prominent frontal SSVEP latency reductions observed in controls, subjects with ADHD showed only a slight right frontal latency reduction at the appearance of the A and latency increases at other frontal and temporal sites throughout the A-X interval.
A possible relationship between dopaminergic processes and SSVEP latency reductions in the A-X interval is suggested by the similarity of the SSVEP latency reduction topography and the primate neocortical dopaminergic distribution.32 The possibility of such a relationship is strengthened by our finding of reduced or nonexistent frontal SSVEP latency reduction in subjects with ADHD, and long-standing evidence pointing to dopaminergic deficits underlying the symptoms of ADHD.33,34
These findings are unlikely to be a consequence of ADHD group deficits in performance as the SSVEP amplitude and latency for both the reference task and the CPT-AX were derived from only the correct trials, and the number of correct responses included in the analyses was similar for both groups. Furthermore, we do not believe that these findings are a consequence of the group difference in mean IQ as the differences were also apparent in mean IQ-matched subgroups.
Our ADHD sample manifested considerable comorbidity with other disruptive behavior disorders and learning difficulties, but this is typical of ADHD populations.35-37 Relatively little is known about the effects of comorbidity on the type and severity of cognitive deficits found in children with ADHD. Some recent studies37-39 have reported that deficits in performance on neuropsychological tasks by children with ADHD cannot be accounted for by the presence of psychiatric comorbidity or learning difficulties. While the present study did not allow us to determine comorbidity effects on the electrophysiological and behavioral findings, we are currently undertaking a larger study that aims to examine this.
The failure of the ADHD group to demonstrate transient prefrontal SSVEP latency reductions at critical points in the CPT-AX suggests prefrontal deficits in ADHD. This is consistent with a range of neuropsychological and functional neuroimaging reports pointing to prefrontal deficits in ADHD. Although many neuropsychological tests point to prefrontal abnormalities, specific deficits in prefrontal processes mediating sustained attention also have been inferred from covert attention shift studies using the Posner paradigm.40,41
Our results are also consistent with those of a variety of functional neuroimaging studies9,42 that have identified deficits in prefrontal activation in ADHD. In particular, we find some of the most prominent SSVEP differences between the control group and the ADHD group to be situated at right prefrontal recording sites, suggesting a specific role for the right prefrontal cortex in the CPT-AX. Such prefrontal lateralization is consistent with PET studies indicating preferential right prefrontal cortical activation in prolonged vigilance.43 A preferential role for the right prefrontal cortex in planning and inhibitory motor control also has been recently reported by Rubia et al.44 In a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of a "stop-task," where motor responses to a "go" stimulus had to be inhibited in 50% of the trials, pronounced activation was observed at the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the right anterior cingulate in the population of 9 normal adults.
In summary, we suggest that steady-state probe topography may offer a useful neuroimaging modality to complement PET and functional magnetic resonance imaging in the investigations of the neurobiological substrate of ADHD. It should, however, be stressed that steady-state probe topography cannot match the high spatial resolution and 3-dimensional information available with PET and functional magnetic resonance imaging. This is especially important when considering the role of subcortical structures, such as the basal ganglia, in ADHD.
Accepted for publication August 20, 1998.
This work was supported in part by an Australian Research Council Infrastructure Grant, a Swinburne University of Technology Research Support Grant, Melbourne, Australia, and by grant 0541000 from the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), Canberra, Australia. Ms Farrow was supported by an Australian Research Council Postgraduate Award.
Part of this work was presented in abstract form at the Annual Meeting of the Australasian Society for Psychiatric Research, Melbourne, Australia, November 30, 1995.
We would like to acknowledge the essential contribution of Peter Cadusch, PhD, in the development of the multivariate permutation test software and the assistance of Katie Wood, BBSc, in assessing control and patient populations and of the NHMRC Twin Registry in recruiting subjects.
Reprints: Richard B. Silberstein, PhD, Brain Sciences Institute, Swinburne University of Technology, PO Box 218, Hawthorn, Victoria, Australia, 3122 (e-mail: rbs@mind.scan.swin.edu.au).
1.Brown
CSCooke
SC Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: clinical features and treatment options.
CNS Drugs. 1994;195- 106
Google Scholar 2.Van der Meere
JSergeant
J Acquisition of attention skill in pervasively hyperactive children.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1988;29301- 310
Google Scholar 3.Benson
DF The role of frontal dysfunction in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
J Child Neurol. 1991;6(suppl)S9- S12
Google Scholar 4.Barkley
RAGrodzinsky
GDuPaul
GJ Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: a review and research report.
J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1992;20163- 188
Google Scholar 5.Carter
CSKrener
PChaderjian
MNorthcutt
CWolfe
V Abnormal processing of irrelevant information in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Psychiatry Res. 1995;5659- 70
Google Scholar 6.Lou
HCSkov
HHenriksen
L Intellectual impairment with regional cerebral dysfunction after neonatal cerebral blood flow.
Acta Paediatr Scand. 1989;360(suppl)72- 82
Google Scholar 7.Amen
DGPaldi
FThisted
RA Brain SPECT imaging [letter].
J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1993;321080- 1081
Google Scholar 8.Amen
DGCarmichael
BD High-resolution brain SPECT imaging in ADHD.
Ann Clin Psychiatry. 1997;981- 86
Google Scholar 9.Zametkin
AJNordahl
TEGross
MKing
ACSemple
WERumsey
JHamburger
SCohen
RM Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of childhood onset.
N Engl J Med. 1990;3231361- 1366
Google Scholar 10.Zametkin
AJLiebenauer
LLFitzgerald
GAKing
ACMinkunas
DVHerscovitch
PYamada
EMCohen
RM Brain metabolism in teenagers with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50333- 340
Google Scholar 11.Sokoloff
L Cerebral circulation, energy metabolism, and protein synthesis: general characteristics and principles of measurement. Phelps
MEMazziotta
JCSchelbert
HReds
Positron Emission Tomography and Autoradiography Principles and Applications for the Heart and Brain. New York, NY Raven Press1986;1- 71
Google Scholar 12.Petersen
SEFox
PTPosner
MIMintun
MRaichle
ME Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing.
Nature. 1988;331585- 589
Google Scholar 13.Satterfield
JHSchell
AMNicholas
T Preferential neural processing of attended stimuli in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal boys.
Psychophysiology. 1994;311- 10
Google Scholar 14.Regan
D Human Brain Electrophysiology: Evoked Potentials and Evoked Magnetic Fields in Science and Medicine. New York, NY Elsevier Publishers1989;
15.Silberstein
RB Steady-state visually evoked potentials, brain resonances, and cognitive processes. Nunez
PLed
Neocortical Dynamics and Human EEG Rhythms. New York, NY Oxford University Press1995;272- 303
Google Scholar 16.Pfurtscheller
GKlimesch
W Topographic display and interpretation of event-related desynchronization during a visual-verbal task.
Brain Topogr. 1990;385- 93
Google Scholar 17.Silberstein
RBCadusch
PJNield
GPipingas
ASimpson
DG Steady-state visually evoked potential topography dynamics and cognition.
XIth International Conference on Event-Related Potentials of the Brain. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science1996;379- 386
Excerpta Medica International Congress Series Google Scholar 18.Corkum
PVSiegel
LS Is the continuous performance task a valuable research tool for use with children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder?
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;341217- 1239
Google Scholar 19.Oldfield
RC The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory.
Neuropsychologia. 1971;973- 113
Google Scholar 20.Wechsler
D Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex The Psychological Corporation1990;
21.Sattler
JM Assessment of Children. 3rd ed, revised San Diego, Calif Jerome M Sattler Publisher Inc1992;
22.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
23.Levy
FHay
DA The Australian Twin Behaviour Rating Scale. Melbourne, Australia La Trobe University Twin Study1991;
24.Levy
FHay
DAMcLaughlin
MWood
CWaldman
I Twin-sibling differences in parental reports of ADHD, speech, reading and behavior problems.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;37569- 578
Google Scholar 25.Silberstein
RBSchier
MAPipingas
ACiorciari
JWood
SRSimpson
DG Steady-state visually evoked potential topography associated with a visual vigilance task.
Brain Topogr. 1990;3337- 347
Google Scholar 26.Silberstein
RBCiorciari
JPipingas
A Steady-state visually evoked potential topography during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995;9624- 35
Google Scholar 27.Nunez
PLSilberstein
RBCadusch
PJWijesinghe
RSWestdorp
AFSrivinasan
R A theoretical and experimental study of high resolution EEG based on surface Laplacians and cortical imaging.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;9040- 57
Google Scholar 28.Blair
RCKarniski
W An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potential.
Psychophysiology. 1993;30518- 524
Google Scholar 29.Blair
RCKarniski
W Distribution-free statistical analyses of surface and volumetric maps. Thatcher
RWHallet
TVZeffiro
TJohn
ERHeurta
Meds
Functional Neuroimaging. New York, NY Academic Press1994;19- 28
Google Scholar 30.Holmes
APBlair
RCWatson
JDFord
I Nonparametric analysis of statistic images from functional mapping experiments.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1996;167- 22
Google Scholar 31.Eddington
ES Randomization Tests. 2nd ed. New York, NY Marcel Dekker1987;
32.Fuster
JM The Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY Raven Press1989;
33.Robbins
TWSahakian
BJ Behavioral effects of psychomotor stimulant drugs: clinical and neuropsychobiological implications. Creese
Ied
Stimulants Clinical, Behavioral and Neurochemical Perspectives. New York, NY Raven Press1983;301- 338
Google Scholar 34.Levy
F The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1991;25277- 283
Google Scholar 35.Biederman
JNewcorn
JSprich
S Comorbidity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety and other disorders.
Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148564- 577
Google Scholar 36.Barkley
RA Workshop Manual: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adolescents. Melbourne The Australian Psychological Society Ltd1995;
37.Seidman
LJBenedict
KBBiederman
JBernstein
JHSeiverd
KMilberger
SNorman
DMick
EFaraone
SV Performance of children with ADHD on the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: a pilot neuropsychological study.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;361459- 1473
Google Scholar 38.Seidman
LJBiederman
JFaraone
SVMilberger
SNorman
DSeiverd
KBenedict
KGuite
JMick
EKiely
K Effects of family history and comorbidity on the neuropsychological performance of children with ADHD: preliminary findings.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1995;341015- 1024
Google Scholar 39.Wood
CMaruff
PLevy
FFarrow
MHay
D Covert orienting of visual spatial attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: does comorbidity make a difference?
Arch Clin Neuropsychol. In press.
Google Scholar 40.Posner
MI Structure and function of selective attention. Boll
TBryant
DKeds
Clinical Neuropsychology and Brain Function Research, Assessment and Practice. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1988;173- 202
Google Scholar 41.Swanson
JMShea
CMcBurnett
KPotkin
SGFiore
CCrinella
F Attention and hyperactivity. Enns
JTed
The Development of Attention Research and Theory. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier (North-Holland)1990;383- 403
Google Scholar 42.O'Tuama
LATreves
ST Brain single-photon emission computed tomography for behavior disorders in children.
Semin Nucl Med. 1993;23255- 264
Google Scholar 43.Pardo
WFox
PTRaichle
ME Localisation of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography.
Nature. 1991;34961- 64
Google Scholar 44.Rubia
KOvermeyer
STaylor
EBullmore
EBrammer
MWilliams
SSimmons
AAndrew
C Functional neuroanatomy of motor inhibition using fMRI.
Neuroimage. 1997;5(suppl)S111
Google Scholar