Background
Verbal learning and memory deficits are among the most severe cognitive deficits observed in schizophrenia. We have demonstrated that such deficits do not extend to working memory for tones in a substantial number of patients even when verbal working memory is impaired. In this study we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to study the neural basis of this dissociation of auditory verbal and nonverbal working memory in individuals with schizophrenia.
Methods
While undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging, 12 schizophrenic patients and 12 matched control subjects performed auditory Word Serial Position Task and Tone Serial Position Task.
Results
Both tasks produced activation in frontal cortex and temporal and parietal lobes of the cerebrum in both groups. While robust activation was observed in the left inferior frontal gyrus (areas 6, 44, and 45) in the control group during the Word Serial Position Task, activation in the patient group was much reduced in these areas and failed to show the same task-specific activation as in controls. Reduced activation in patients was not confined to the inferior frontal gyrus, but also extended to a medial area during the Tone Serial Position Task and to premotor and anterior temporal lobe areas during both tasks.
Conclusions
These findings support the hypothesis that abnormalities in cortical hemodynamic response in the inferior frontal gyrus underlie the verbal working memory deficit in schizophrenia. The relationship of verbal working memory deficits to other cognitive functions suggests that abnormal functioning in the speech-related areas may reflect a critical substrate of a broad range of cognitive dysfunctions associated with schizophrenia.
ALTHOUGH schizophrenia disrupts a broad range of perceptual, sensorimotor, and cognitive functions, tasks assessing verbal learning and memory appear to be particularly disturbed.1-6 In our companion article,7 we report that auditory verbal working memory is selectively impaired in schizophrenic patients with basic perceptual and attentional competence while working memory for nonverbal auditory stimuli (ie, tones) remains intact. This suggests that the neural systems for processing auditory verbal and nonverbal material are segregated at some level and that the former can be selectively impaired in schizophrenia.
Functional imaging studies of verbal working memory tasks consistently report activation in the inferior and middle frontal cortex, and in posterior temporoparietal areas in control subjects,8-12 results consistent with reports that verbal deficits are associated with left inferior frontal and posterior temporal lesions.13 In contrast, imaging work suggests that tonal memory involves cortical and subcortical areas partially lateralized to the right hemisphere,14 consistent with clinical studies demonstrating right temporal and frontal lobe involvement in pitch discrimination and memory.15-17
Although there are no published imaging studies of auditory verbal working memory per se in schizophrenia, a number of studies have reported frontal and temporal lobe abnormalities during word recognition,18 word generation,19-22 and auditory selective attention.23 In addition, several imaging studies have reported decreased frontal lobe activation in schizophrenic patients during performance of neuropsychological tasks sensitive to frontal lobe lesions.23-26
The present study sought to examine cortical hemodynamic activity in schizophrenia during a verbal working memory task as well as during a nonverbal working memory task that patients performed normally.
All subjects provided informed consent before the study. The patient group consisted of 14 medicated outpatients (8 men) who met Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R27 criteria for schizophrenia. Symptom scores (mean±SD) on the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale were as follows: positive, 16.1±6.0; negative, 17.2±4.5; general, 32.1±9.0; and total, 65.0±18.4.
Fourteen subjects (8 men) without history of psychiatric disorder served as controls. None was taking medications that affect the central nervous system. All patients and control subjects were right-hand dominant, as assessed by writing and performing at least 5 of 7 daily activities with their right hand. Subjects with histories of neurologic disorder or trauma, or substance abuse within the last 6 months, were excluded from the study. As assessed by Student t test (P<.05), the control and schizophrenia groups did not differ in age (controls, 36.8±11.8 years; patients, 39.6±8.6 years), years of education (controls, 13.9±1.1 years; patients, 14.5±1.9 years), or parent education (controls, 13.5±2.3 years; patients, 13.0±3.2 years). All subjects scored at least 90% correct on a screening test of auditory pitch discrimination, described in our companion article,7 assuring that all subjects could discriminate the tonal stimuli used in the Tone Serial Position Task (TSPT).
Subjects were trained on the auditory Word Serial Position Task (WSPT) and TSPT before scanning as described in our companion article.7 In the WSPT, subjects were presented with lists of 4 words followed by a 9-second retention interval. One of the words was then repeated and subjects indicated its position (first, second, third, or fourth) in the list by briefly extending the corresponding number of fingers on their left hand. The TSPT was procedurally identical to the WSPT but consisted of lists of 3 tones instead of 4 words. Tones within each list differed from the next closest tone by a frequency ratio of 0.85 (eg, 500 Hz, 425 Hz, 361 Hz). Fewer tones than words were used in lists to improve the overall level of performance of the task.
Scanning took place in a 1.5-T Signa magnetic resonance imaging system (General Electric Corp, Milwaukee, Wis) equipped with echo planar imaging hardware (Advanced NMR Inc, Wilmington, Mass). Anatomical localizer scans were acquired in the sagittal plane, using conventional T1-weighted spin-echo sequence: echo time, 11 milliseconds; repetition time, 667 milliseconds; field of view, 24 cm2; slice gap, 0 mm; acquisition matrix, 256×128 with 1 excitation; and slice thickness, 5 mm. Ten T1-weighted oblique axial slices were acquired with the following imaging sequence: echo time, 13 milliseconds; repetition time, 500 milliseconds; field of view, 40 cm2; acquisition matrix, 256×192 with 2 excitations; and slice thickness, 9 mm. Slice orientation paralleled the plane transecting the anterior and posterior commissures, and the interslice distance was varied by subject to maintain the same location of slices in all subjects.
Functional images were acquired with an echo planar imaging gradient echo sequence: flip angle, 60°; echo time, 45 milliseconds; repetition time, 1500 milliseconds; field of view, 40×20 cm; acquisition matrix, 128×64 with 1 excitation; and voxel volume, 9×3.125×3.125 mm. Eighty images per slice were acquired during each scan.
Image processing was performed with statistical programs written in MATLAB 4.0 (Math Works, Inc, Natick, Mass). All images were screened for motion and other artifacts and contaminated images were eliminated. Subjects were eliminated if movement of the center of mass of activation was greater than 1 pixel across the session, or greater than 0.5 pixel within a scan. Two controls and 2 patients were eliminated because of failure to meet the above criteria, yielding the final sample of 24.
Subjects underwent 2 scans for each task. Computer-generated stimuli were presented via shielded speakers, through air conductance tubes, to headphones, which provided external noise reduction. The volume was adjusted to the comfort of each subject. A WSPT scan consisted of 4 trials each separated from the next by a 14-second baseline interval. The same procedure was used with the TSPT, except that 5 trials were conducted during each TSPT scan separated by 12-second intertrial intervals. The 6 images acquired during the 9-second retention interval preceding the target and the 6 images acquired during the final 9 seconds of the baseline period were compared in the data analysis.
Definition of regions of interest
After coregistration of the anatomical images and echo planar imaging functional maps from individual subjects, the images were transformed into the standard 3-dimensional coordinate system of Talairach and Tournoux.28,29 Slices were interpolated into the 10 most superior slices of Talairach space, centered at z=+69, +60, +51, +42, +33, +23, +14, +5, −5, and −16. The slices covered the entire cerebrum except for a portion of the anterior and inferior aspect of the temporal pole. Thirty regions of interest (ROIs) in each hemisphere were defined within the Talairach space. The ROIs were constructed from rectangular volumes (8×8×9 mm) and corresponded to Brodmann areas (Figure 1).
A 2-step analysis was carried out to calculate task-related activation in each individual subject; the values obtained were then used as the dependent measures in repeated-measures analysis of variance. First, t-values were calculated for each pixel during each task by comparing the 6 images acquired during the retention intervals with the last 6 images acquired during the baseline periods.28 A criterion for activation was set at a t-value greater than 1 on both scans of a task, with a minimum cluster size of 4 contiguous pixels meeting this same t-value threshold. A pixel was assigned the lesser t-value of the 2 scans for each task. The t-value criteria were selected because lower thresholds included activation in ventricles and white matter, while higher values eliminated all but a few pixels in some subjects.
Next we calculated the active pixel density in all ROI volumes in each brain for each task. First the number of active pixels in each ROI was divided by the total number of pixels contained in that ROI to adjust for differences in the sizes of the ROIs. Individual differences in global activation were removed by dividing the above ratio by the ratio of the total number of active pixels in the brain to the total number of pixels in the brain. For each ROI, an active pixel density of 1 is equivalent to the mean density of active pixels for the whole brain. To identify regions that showed clear task-related activation, an ROI was retained for further analysis if its mean pixel density was greater than 2 (twice the density of the whole brain) in at least 1 of the groups during 1 of the tasks.
The statistical significance of regional activation differences between groups was evaluated by subjecting pixel density scores for retained ROIs to 2×2×2 repeated-measures analysis of variance with group (patients vs controls) as a between-subject factor and task (WSPT vs TSPT) and hemisphere (left vs right) as within-subject factors. A minimum significance value of .05 was used throughout.
Tspt and wspt training performance
The pattern of behavioral deficits on the WSPT and sparing of TSPT performance reported in the larger sample1 was confirmed in the present sample. On the prescan WSPT test, the patient group performed at a mean of 85% correct, significantly worse than the 94% mean accuracy among the controls (F1,22=5.718; P=.03). Performance in the scanner was excellent for both groups on the limited number of WSPT trials, on which the patient group and control group performed at 91% and 95% mean accuracy, respectively (F1,22=0.88; P=.36). In training before scanning, the patient group averaged 68% correct on the TSPT compared with 71% for the controls (F1,22=0.217; P=.64). During scanning, the groups averaged 69% and 75%, respectively (F1,22=0.33; P=.59). Equivalent levels of performance by patients and controls during scanning rule out a failure by the patient group to attend to the stimuli or other extraneous factors as the basis of activation differences between groups.
Prescan group differences on the WSPT task were mirrored by cortical hemodynamic differences during WSPT scans. A cluster of ROIs in the left inferior frontal gyrus was preferentially activated by the WSPT task in the controls but not in patients (Figure 2). Analyses of variance confirmed these observations (Table 1). The activation centroids are presented in Table 2. The WSPT produced significantly greater activation than the TSPT in area 44 and inferior lateral area 6. Interactions between group and task in these 2 areas, as well as in area 45, indicated that increased activation was specific to the controls. The WSPT also produced significant effects of hemisphere, reflecting left lateralization of activation in area 44 (P<.004) and lateral inferior area 6 (P=.03). Similarly, in area 45, a 3-way interaction emerged between group, task, and hemisphere. Follow-up analyses of variance indicated that only the control group produced significantly greater activation during WSPT than during the TSPT in area 44 (F1,11=14.087; P<.004), area 45 (F1,11=7.642; P=.02), and inferior lateral area 6 (F1,11=11.227; P<.007) (Figure 2).
The TSPT task produced significantly greater activation than the WSPT in area 10 and medial area 6 in the frontal lobes, although the greater activation during the TSPT in medial area 6 appeared only in controls (Table 1). Separate analyses of the groups confirmed that controls produced greater activation during the TSPT than during the WSPT (F1,11=8.917; P<.01) in medial area 6, while the patients showed no such increase (F1,11=0.297; P=.59) (Figure 3).
Finally, a main effect of group appeared in lateral area 6, which was activated by both the WSPT and TSPT tasks in the control group to a greater degree than in the patient group.
The correlation between WSPT performance outside the scanner was compared with activation in ROIs that showed task×hemisphere interactions reflecting greater activation during the WSPT. Activation in left area 44 correlated with WSPT performance (Pearson r=0.45; P<.02), as did inferior lateral area 6 (Pearson r=0.49; P<.02) in the overall sample. Correlations in patients and controls separately were of equivalent magnitude. The correlation between left medial area 6 activation during the TSPT scan and TSPT behavioral performance was not significant (Pearson r=0.15; P=.49).
Both the WSPT and TSPT produced substantial above-threshold activation throughout the superior temporal gyrus. Unlike the frontal lobe, temporal cortical areas did not show significant group×task interactions (Table 1). The WSPT produced significantly greater pixel density than the TSPT throughout the superior temporal gyrus in anterior and posterior area 22 as well as in the primary auditory areas (41/42) (Figure 2). The control group had somewhat higher pixel densities than patients throughout the superior temporal gyrus during both tasks, but group differences reached significance only in anterior area 22. Hemispheric effects of task were confined to area 41/42, reflecting a relative right hemisphere lateralization of activation in this area during the TSPT and a greater pixel density in the left hemisphere during the WSPT. No other effects in the temporal lobe ROIs reached significance.
Parieto-occipital regions
Group differences failed to appear in the parietal regions, although task-specific activation was present in area 7. A significant interaction of task and hemisphere reflected greater pixel density in the right hemisphere during the TSPT relative to the WSPT, while there was comparable pixel density in the left hemisphere during both tasks (Table 1). As expected, no ROI in the occipital lobe approached the pixel density threshold of 2 for inclusion in the analyses.
The present study indicates that areas in left inferior frontal cortex that are active during verbal working memory tasks in normal subjects9,12,30,31 are dysfunctional in schizophrenic patients who have specific performance deficits in verbal but not nonverbal working memory.1 The WSPT-specific hypoactivity in the left inferior frontal cortex of the schizophrenic group suggests that these language-related regions may be critically related to their verbal working memory deficit. This assertion is supported by the significant correlation between 2 of these ROIs (left areas 44 and inferior lateral 6) and performance on the WSPT assessed before scanning. It is not possible in this study to determine whether the marked failure of activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus during the WSPT scans was caused by abnormalities in the inferior frontal gyrus, failure by patients to engage these areas during the WSPT task because of strategic factors, or a result of failures at earlier stages of processing. Data from previous positron emission tomographic imaging studies of word generation tasks reported comparable activation in patient and control groups in the left inferior frontal gyrus when matched for rate of production, but abnormalities in superior temporal gyrus.19,20 Thus, under conditions of lexical retrieval, the frontal speech areas appeared normal, but in the present study, which emphasizes encoding and maintenance, these areas appear dysfunctional. It is important that we failed to observe activation in area 46, a site commonly reported active in other studies of working memory. This may reflect differences in stimuli used in the WSPT and other tasks, and, in addition, the heavy reliance on covert rehearsal during the WSPT, which would favor more inferior and specifically speech-related regions as compared with the more commonly used "N-back" task.12
Other cortical areas also showed signs of abnormal function, including areas activated by the behaviorally spared TSPT. The TSPT provided an opportunity to assess cortical hemodynamic abnormalities on a task that is not impaired in these schizophrenic patients.1 The 2 areas of TSPT-related activation were in the anterior frontal lobe (area 10) and in the supplementary motor and premotor area of the superior frontal gyrus (medial area 6). Connections between area 10 and the superior temporal gyrus have been observed in the rhesus monkey,32 but activation in area 10 has not been reported in previous functional imaging studies of auditory processing in humans.33 In medial area 6, the patients failed to produce increased activation during the TSPT scans, as seen in controls, but showed comparable activation during the WSPT scans. Performance of the TSPT task was not impaired either during scanning or on extensive prescan testing. Therefore, the decreased hemodynamic response in the patient group in medial area 6 during the TSPT cannot be attributed to a performance deficit, as appeared to be the case during the WSPT in the inferior frontal gyrus. Similar cases of hypoperfusion and spared behavior have been previously reported in schizophrenia.34
The schizophrenia group consistently produced less temporal lobe activation than the controls during both tasks, although a group difference reached statistical significance only in anterior area 22 bilaterally. These results suggest that, while abnormalities are present in temporal lobe regions in schizophrenia,35-39 they may not compromise auditory perception and memory during processing of relatively simple stimuli (ie, tones), as demonstrated by the sparing of the TSPT task. The verbal memory processes, which are widely considered essential for comprehension and reasoning,40,41 may rely more heavily on the frontal areas activated in the control subjects of the present study. Given that the WSPT deficit suggests that verbal encoding and maintenance are disrupted in the patients, the inferior frontal areas may also be critical for these processes. However, these patients were selected on the basis of their relatively good attention, and therefore the results must be generalized with caution. In addition, further understanding of the neural systems underlying the distinctions between verbal and auditory processing will be necessary to bear out the present interpretations.
Accepted for publication August 7, 1998.
This study was supported by a National Alliance for Schizophrenia and Depression Young Investigator Award (Dr Stevens) and a Research Scientist Development Award, National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, Md (Dr Wexler).
Reprints: Alexander A. Stevens, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, CR139, Oregon Health Sciences University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Rd, Portland, OR 97201-3098 (e-mail: stevensal@ohsu.edu).
1.Saykin
AJGur
RGur
REMozley
PDMozley
LHResnick
SMKester
DBStafiniak
P Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48618- 624
Google Scholar 2.Saykin
AJShtasel
DLGur
REKester
DBMozley
LHStafiniak
PGur
RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131
Google Scholar 3.Frame
CLOltmann
TF Serial recall by schizophrenic and affective patients during and after psychotic episodes.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1982;91311- 318
Google Scholar 4.Calev
AVenebles
PMonk
AF Evidence for distinct verbal memory pathologies in severely and mildly disturbed schizophrenics.
Schizophr Bull. 1983;9247- 264
Google Scholar 5.Fleming
KGoldberg
TGold
JMWeinberger
DR Verbal working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: use of a Brown-Peterson paradigm.
Psychiatry Res. 1995;56155- 161
Google Scholar 6.Randolph
CGold
JCarpenter
CJGoldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Release from proactive interference: determinants of performance neuropsychological correlates.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14785- 800
Google Scholar 7.Wexler
BEStevens
AABowers
AASernyak
MJGoldman-Rakic
PS Word and tone working memory deficits in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551093- 1096
Google Scholar 8.Awh
ESmith
EEJonides
J Human rehearsal processes and the frontal lobes: PET evidence. Ann N Y Acad Sci1995;76997- 117
9.Cohen
JDPerlstein
WBraver
TSNystrom
LENoll
DCJonides
JSmith
EE Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task.
Nature. 1997;386604- 607
Google Scholar 10.Cohen
JDForman
SDBraver
TSCasey
BJServan-Schrieber
DNoll
DC Activation in the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI.
Hum Brain Map. 1994;1293- 304
Google Scholar 11.Kammer
TBelleman
MGuckel
FBrix
GGass
ASchlemmer
HSpitzer
M Functional MR imaging of the prefrontal cortex: specific activation in a working memory task.
Magn Reson Imaging. 1998;15879- 889
Google Scholar 12.Smith
EEJonides
JKoeppe
RA Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET.
Cereb Cortex. 1996;611- 20
Google Scholar 13.Damasio
ARGeschwind
N The neural basis of language.
Annu Rev Neurosci. 1984;7127- 147
Google Scholar 14.Zatorre
RJEvans
AMeyer
E Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch.
J Neurosci. 1994;141908- 1919
Google Scholar 15.Robin
DATranel
DDamasio
H Auditory perception of temporal and spectral events in patients with focal left and right cerebral lesions.
Brain Lang. 1990;39539- 555
Google Scholar 16.Zatorre
RJSamson
S Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short-term memory.
Brain. 1991;1142403- 2417
Google Scholar 17.Zatorre
RJHalpern
AR Effect of unilateral temporal lobe excision on perception and imagery of song.
Neuropsychologia. 1993;31221- 232
Google Scholar 18.Wood
FBFlowers
D Hypofrontal vs. hypo-sylvian blood flow in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1990;16413- 424
Google Scholar 19.Fletcher
PCFrith
CGrasby
PMFriston
KJDolan
RJ Local and distributed effects of apomorphine on fronto-temporal function in acute unmedicated schizophrenia.
J Neurosci. 1996;167055- 7062
Google Scholar 20.Frith
CDFriston
KHarold
SSilbersweig
DFletcher
PCahill
CDolan
RJFrackowiak
RSJLiddle
PF Regional brain activation in chronic schizophrenia patients during the performance of a verbal fluency task.
Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167343- 349
Google Scholar 21.Dolan
RJFletcher
PCFrith
CDFriston
KJFrackowiak
RSJGrasby
PM Dopaminergic modulation of impaired cognitive activation in the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia.
Nature. 1995;378180- 182
Google Scholar 22.Yurgelun-Todd
DAWasternaux
CMCohen
BMGruber
BSEnglish
CDRenshaw
PF Functional magnetic resonance imaging of schizophrenic patients and comparison subjects during word production. Am J
Psychiatry1996;153200- 205
23.O'Leary
DSAndreasen
NCHurtig
RRKesler
MLRogers
MArndt
SCizadlo
TWatkins
GLBoles-Ponto
LLKirchner
PTHichwa
RD Auditory attentional deficits in patients with schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53633- 641
Google Scholar 24.Andreasen
NCRezai
KAlliger
RSwayze
VWFlaum
MKirchner
PCohen
GO'Leary
DS Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49943- 958
Google Scholar 25.Berman
KFIllowsky
BPWeinberger
DR Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45616- 622
Google Scholar 26.Weinberger
DRBerman
KFIllowsky
BP Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45609- 615
Google Scholar 27.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
28.Constable
RTSkudlarski
PMencl
EPugh
RKFulbright
RKLacadie
CShaywitz
SEShaywitz
BA Quantifying and comparing region-of-interest activation patterns in functional brain MR imaging: methodology considerations.
Magn Reson Imaging. In press.
Google Scholar 29.Talairach
JTournoux
P Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-D Proportional System: An Approach to Imaging. New York, NY Georg Thieme Verlag1988;
30.Paulesu
EFrith
CDFrackowiak
RJS The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory.
Nature. 1993;362342- 345
Google Scholar 31.Cohen
RMSemple
WEGross
MKing
ACNordahl
TE Metabolic brain pattern of sustained auditory discrimination.
Exp Brain Res. 1992;92165- 172
Google Scholar 32.Petrides
MPandya
DN Association fiber pathway to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey.
J Comp Neurol. 1988;27352- 66
Google Scholar 33.Petrides
MAlivisatos
BMeyer
EEvans
AC Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90878- 882
Google Scholar 34.Wexler
BE Comments on: Guenther et al [
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;244:115-125]. Diminished cerebral metabolic response to motor stimulation in schizophrenics.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;24660
Google Scholar 35.McCarley
RWShenton
MEO'Donnell
BFFaux
SFKikinis
RNestor
PGJolesz
FA Auditory P300 abnormalities and left posterior superior temporal gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50190- 197
Google Scholar 36.Chua
SEMcKenna
P Schizophrenia—a brain disease? a critical review of structural and functional cerebral abnormality in the disorder.
Br J Psychiatry. 1995;166563- 582
Google Scholar 37.DeLisi
LEHoff
ASchwartz
JEShields
GWHalthore
SNSimhadri
GMHenn
FAAnand
AK Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29159- 175
Google Scholar 38.Falkai
PBogarts
BSchneider
TGreve
BPfeiffer
UPilz
KGonsiorzcyk
CMjtenyi
COvery
I Disturbed planum temporale asymmetry in schizophrenia: a quantitative post-mortem study.
Schizophr Res. 1995;14161- 176
Google Scholar 39.Kawasaki
YMaede
YUrata
KHigashima
MYamaguchi
NSuzuki
MTakashima
TIde
Y A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of patients with schizophrenia.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;242268- 272
Google Scholar 40.Just
MACarpenter
P A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory.
Psychol Rev. 1992;99122- 149
Google Scholar 41.Baddeley
AD Working Memory. New York, NY Oxford University Press1986;