[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Sample tracings of smooth pursuit eye movements of a schizophrenic patient (top) and a normal control (middle) following a 0.4-Hz sine wave (bottom, dashed line). Note that the normal control record is smooth except for a blink at about 12 seconds into the record. The record of the schizophrenic patient shows a very irregular pattern, suggestive of low gain pursuit with frequent saccadic eye movements.

Sample tracings of smooth pursuit eye movements of a schizophrenic patient (top) and a normal control (middle) following a 0.4-Hz sine wave (bottom, dashed line). Note that the normal control record is smooth except for a blink at about 12 seconds into the record. The record of the schizophrenic patient shows a very irregular pattern, suggestive of low gain pursuit with frequent saccadic eye movements.

Figure 2. 
Step-ramp pursuit of a normal control (left) and a schizophrenic patient (right). The target, illustrated by the dotted line, steps abruptly to the left and remains stationary for 200 milliseconds before beginning a 20° per second ramp trajectory to the right. The open-loop period is denoted by the black bars, which begin at 130 milliseconds after the target starts its ramp and continue for 100 milliseconds. In response, at about 150 milliseconds after the start of the ramp, the normal control begins a smooth eye movement that accelerates at a rate that is discernibly faster than that of the schizophrenic patient, whose initial eye movement barely accelerates.

Step-ramp pursuit of a normal control (left) and a schizophrenic patient (right). The target, illustrated by the dotted line, steps abruptly to the left and remains stationary for 200 milliseconds before beginning a 20° per second ramp trajectory to the right. The open-loop period is denoted by the black bars, which begin at 130 milliseconds after the target starts its ramp and continue for 100 milliseconds. In response, at about 150 milliseconds after the start of the ramp, the normal control begins a smooth eye movement that accelerates at a rate that is discernibly faster than that of the schizophrenic patient, whose initial eye movement barely accelerates.

Figure 3. 
The relation between target ramp velocity and initial acceleration. In contrast to the higher accelerations by normal control subjects, the schizophrenic patients show initial accelerations that are considerably lower in magnitude at all 3 target velocities.

The relation between target ramp velocity and initial acceleration. In contrast to the higher accelerations by normal control subjects, the schizophrenic patients show initial accelerations that are considerably lower in magnitude at all 3 target velocities.

Figure 4. 
Scatter diagram of the relationship within the schizophrenia group (n=15) between open-loop acceleration for the 10° per second target and velocity discrimination between 2 targets (11° per second vs 9° per second).

Scatter diagram of the relationship within the schizophrenia group (n=15) between open-loop acceleration for the 10° per second target and velocity discrimination between 2 targets (11° per second vs 9° per second).

Figure 5. 
Scatter diagram of the relationship within the schizophrenia group (n=15) between peak gain for the 0.4-Hz sine wave and velocity discrimination between 2 targets (11° per second vs 9° per second).

Scatter diagram of the relationship within the schizophrenia group (n=15) between peak gain for the 0.4-Hz sine wave and velocity discrimination between 2 targets (11° per second vs 9° per second).

Velocity Discrimination and Eye Tracking Measurements
Velocity Discrimination and Eye Tracking Measurements
1.
Chen  YPalafox  GPNakayama  KLevy  DLMatthysse  SHolzman  PS Motion perception in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56149- 154Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Holzman  PSProctor  LRHughes  DW Eye tracking patterns in schizophrenia.  Science. 1973;181179- 181Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Holzman  PSProctor  LRLevy  DLYasillo  NJMeltzer  HYHurt  SW Eye tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients and their relatives.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31143- 151Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Cegalis  JASweeney  JA Eye movements in schizophrenia: a quantitative analysis.  Biol Psychiatry. 1979;1413- 26Google Scholar
5.
Iacono  WGLykken  DT Eye tracking and psychopathology.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;361361- 1369Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Iacono  WGLykken  DT Two-year retest stability of eye tracking performance and comparison of electrooculographic and infrared recording techniques: evidence of EEG in the electrooculogram.  Psychophysiology. 1981;1849- 55Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Shagass  CAmadeo  MOverton  D Eye-tracking performance in psychiatric patients.  Biol Psychiatry. 1974;9245- 260Google Scholar
8.
Levin  SLuebke  AZee  DHain  TRobinson  DRHolzman  PS Smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenics: quantitative measurements with the search-coil technique.  J Psychiatr Res. 1988;22195- 206Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Clementz  BAMcDowell  JE Smooth pursuit in schizophrenia: abnormalities of open- and closed-loop responses.  Psychophysiology. 1994;3179- 86Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSMatthysse  SMendell  NR Eye tracking and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.  Schizophr Bull. 1993;19461- 536Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Leigh  RJZee  DS The Neurology of Eye Movements. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa F.A. Davis Co1991;
12.
Lisberger  SEvinger  CJohanson  GFuchs  A Relationship between eye acceleration and retinal image velocity during foveal smooth pursuit in man and monkey.  J Neurophysiol. 1981;46229- 249Google Scholar
13.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSProctor  LR Vestibular responses in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35972- 981Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Latham  CHolzman  PSManschrek  TTole  J Optokinetic nystagmus and pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38997- 1003Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Lipton  RBLevin  SHolzman  PS Horizontal and vertical pursuit movements, the oculocephalic reflex, and the functional psychoses.  Psychiatry Res. 1980;3193- 203Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Levin  SJones  AStark  LMerrin  ELHolzman  PS Saccadic eye movements of schizophrenic patients measured by reflected light technique.  Biol Psychiatry. 1982;171277- 1287Google Scholar
17.
Iacono  WGTuason  VBJohnson  RA Dissociation of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye tracking in remitted schizophrenics: an ocular reaction time task that schizophrenics perform well.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38991- 996Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Newsome  WTWurtz  RH Probing visual cortical function with discrete chemical lesions.  Trend Neurosci. 1988;11394- 400Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Baloh  RWYee  RDHonrubia  V Optokinetic nystagmus and parietal lobe lesions.  Ann Neurol. 1980;7269- 276Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Spitzer  RWilliams  JGibbon  MFirst  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Patient Version.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1990;
21.
Levitt  D The transformed up-down methods.  J Acoust Soc Am. 1972;10465- 479Google Scholar
22.
Shagass  CRoemer  RAAmadeo  M Eye tracking performance and engagement of attention.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976;33121- 125Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Rashbass  C The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements.  J Physiol. 1961;159326- 338Google Scholar
24.
Benitez  JT Eye tracking and optokinetic tests: diagnostic significance in peripheral and central vestibular disorders.  Laryngoscope. 1970;80834- 848Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Clementz  BA Saccades to moving targets in schizophrenia: evidence for normal posterior cortex functioning.  Psychophysiology. 1996;33650- 654Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Pola  JWyatt  HJ Target position and velocity: the stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements.  Vision Res. 1980;20523- 534Google ScholarCrossref
27.
McPeek  RMVerstraten  PCavanagh  P Target position, not target velocity, drives sustained smooth pursuit.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;385932Google Scholar
28.
Zeki  SM Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey.  J Physiol (Lond). 1974;236549- 573Google Scholar
29.
Van Essen  DCMaunsell  JHR Hierarchical organization and the functional streams in the visual cortex.  Trend Neurosci. 1983;6370- 375Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Maunsell  JHRNewsome  WT Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 1987;10363- 401Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Newsome  WTPare  EB A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT).  J Neurosci. 1988;82201- 2211Google Scholar
32.
Wurtz  RHYamasaki  CJDuffy  CJRoy  JP Functional specialization for motion processing in primate cerebral cortex.  Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. Cold Spring Harbor, NJ Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press1990;717- 727Google Scholar
33.
Levy  DLLajonchere  CDorogusker  BZither  RTartaglini  ALieberman  JASzymanski  SLee  SMendell  NR Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia, I: quantitative characterization in first-episode patients.  Schizophr Res. In press.Google Scholar
34.
Stuve  TAFriedman  LJesberger  JAGilmore  GCStrauss  MEMeltzer  HY The relationship between smooth pursuit performance, motion perception and sustained visual attention in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls.  Psychol Med. 1997;27143- 152Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Bruce  CJGoldberg  MEBushnell  MCStanton  GB Primate frontal eye fields, II: physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements.  J Neurophysiol. 1985;54714- 734Google Scholar
36.
MacAvoy  MGGottlieb  JPBruce  CJ Smooth-pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field.  Cereb Cortex. 1991;195- 102Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Heinen  SJ Single neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth pursuit eye movements.  Exp Brain Res. 1995;104357- 361Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Morrow  MJSharpe  JA Cerebral hemispheric localization of smooth pursuit asymmetry.  Neurology. 1990;40274- 292Google Scholar
39.
Goldman-Rakic  PS Cellular basis of working memory.  Neuron. 1995;14477- 485Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Park  SHolzman  PS Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49975- 982Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Posner  MIPetersen  SEFox  PTRaichle  ME Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain.  Science. 1988;2401627- 1631Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Sereno  ABHolzman  PS Spatial selective attention in schizophrenic, affective disorder, and normal subjects.  Schizophr Res. 1996;2033- 50Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Maunsell  JHRVan Essen  DC Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey, I: selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation.  J Neurophysiol. 1983;491127- 1147Google Scholar
44.
Maioli  MGSquatrito  SGalletti  CBattaglini  PPSanserino  ER Cortico-cortical connections from the visual region of the superior temporal sulcus to frontal eye field in the macaque.  Brain Res. 1983;265294- 299Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Lynch  JC Frontal eye field lesions in monkeys disrupt pursuit.  Exp Brain Res. 1987;68437- 441Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Sharpe  JAMorrow  MJ Cerebral hemispheric smooth pursuit disorders.  Acta Neurol Belg. 1991;9181- 96Google Scholar
47.
Levin  S Frontal lobe dysfunctions in schizophrenia: eye movement impairments.  J Psychiatr Res. 1984;1827- 55Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Nawrot  MRizzo  M Motion perception deficits from midline cerebellar lesions in humans.  Vis Res. 1995;35723- 731Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Thaker  GKRoss  DEBuchanan  RWMoran  MJLahti  AKim  CMedoff  D Does pursuit abnormality in schizophrenia represent a deficit in the predictive mechanism?  Psychiatry Res. 1996;59221- 237Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Newsome  WTWurtz  RHDursteler  MRMikami  A Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the Macaque monkey.  J Neurosci. 1985;5825- 840Google Scholar
Original Article
February 1999

Dependence of Impaired Eye Tracking on Deficient Velocity Discrimination in Schizophrenia

Author Affiliations

From the Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Mass (Drs Chen, Nakayama, Palafox, and Holzman), and the Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School and McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass (Drs Chen, Levy, Matthysse, and Holzman).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(2):155-161. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.2.155
Abstract

Background  Abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements have been found in many schizophrenic patients and in about 40% of their first-degree biological relatives. A velocity discrimination deficit has also been demonstrated in schizophrenic patients. In this study, we address the relation between deficient velocity discrimination and impaired smooth pursuit eye movements, inasmuch as the brain regions responsible for processing velocity signals are implicated in generating and maintaining smooth pursuit.

Methods  Horizontal eye movements of 15 schizophrenic patients and 8 normal controls were recorded in response to sine wave (predictable) and step-ramp (nonpredictable) targets. Smooth pursuit eye movements were assessed during both the initiation and maintenance periods. Correlations were computed between measures of smooth pursuit (qualitative rating, peak gain, saccade frequency, and initial acceleration) and contrast sensitivity for velocity discrimination.

Results  Contrast sensitivity for fine velocity discrimination was significantly correlated both with initial acceleration of smooth pursuit and with peak gain, but was not significantly correlated with saccade frequency and qualitative ratings of pursuit integrity. No significant correlations were found within the normal control group.

Conclusion  Deficient processing of velocity information seems to be one component that contributes to a dysfunction in the initiation and maintenance of smooth pursuit in schizophrenia.

PREVIOUSLY,1 we reported that about 40% of schizophrenic patients had significantly raised thresholds for fine velocity discrimination compared with normal controls. Here we address the relation between raised thresholds in motion perception and abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM), or eye tracking dysfunction (ETD), which has been stable in a significant proportion of schizophrenic patients and their first-degree biological relatives.2-6 This study was undertaken to explore the processes that underlie ETD.

Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia comprises irregularities in tracking that reflect at least low steady-state gain, low open-loop acceleration (OLA) (eye acceleration after the first 120 milliseconds following target movement), and increased frequency of saccades during eye tracking.7-9 A comprehensive review may be found in Levy et al.10

Smooth pursuit eye movements are complex oculomotor activities, and consist of processes that initiate pursuit and maintain pursuit.11,12 Both processes depend on the presence of motion signals from a stimulus, intact pathways in the brain for processing the motion signals, and an intact motor apparatus for executing eye movements. When any of these component processes is compromised, SPEM becomes abnormal. Motor control over eye movements seems to be normal in schizophrenic patients. They are able to generate slow and fast eye movements (such as nystagmus) in response to vestibular stimulation.13 Full-field optokinetic responses,14 as well as the oculocephalic reflex,15 are intact; and both latency and accuracy of voluntary saccades are essentially normal.16,17 Only SPEM, generated when pursuing a moving target, are impaired in schizophrenic patients.

Our earlier study of motion perception1 was undertaken to investigate the component processes assumed to be implicated in ETD. We proposed that impairedmotion processing is a major factor in these abnormalities. An underlying motion processing deficit seemed plausible because SPEM is impaired when the motion-sensitive areas of the brain—the middle temporal (MT) area and the medial superior temporal (MST) area—suffer lesions, either experimentally produced in monkeys18 or naturally occurring in humans.19 We demonstrated that schizophrenic patients had impaired motion perception, as measured by the contrast required to detect small differences in velocity, but that they did not show impairments in nonmotion tasks. These features of motion perception were stable over time in both normal controls and schizophrenic patients, and thus represent trait characteristics.

In this article, we examine whether and how performance on smooth pursuit tasks and on velocity discrimination tasks are related in schizophrenic patients. We hypothesized that relative insensitivity in velocity discrimination is associated with both low OLA and closed-loop gain in SPEM.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

Fifteen patients with chronic schizophrenia and 8 normal control subjects who had participated in the motion perception tasks1 took part in the current study. Diagnostic procedures are described in Chen et al.1 All patients metDSM-III-R criteria for schizophrenia (n=5) or schizoaffective disorder (n=10), based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R20 administered by trained interviewers and reviewed, together with all hospital records, by a team of experienced clinicians, all of whom were blind to the experimental data. Subjects had no concurrent substance abuse or dependence for at least 6 months and no diagnosed central nervous system abnormalities. Eleven patients were receiving antipsychotic pharmacologic treatment. All participants gave written informed consent and were paid a modest honorarium. Normal controls were also clinically screened with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R; none met criteria for any DSM-III-R Axis I psychotic condition.

Procedures
Previous Motion Perception Experiments

A full description of the procedures used to obtain thresholds for velocity discrimination is contained in Chen et al.1 In brief, we obtained contrast sensitivities for velocity discrimination (which of 2 targets moves faster), contrast detection, and orientation discrimination. We used the standard psychophysical "staircase" method, which increases the task difficulty by 1 step after 3 consecutive correct responses, and decreases the task difficulty by 1 step after 1 incorrect response. This procedure identifies for each individual the threshold of a psychometric function at which all subjects, patients and controls alike, perform at 79.4% accuracy.21 Thus, the dependent variable in all of these threshold measurements was the amount of target contrast necessary to achieve an accurate perceptual judgment of 79.4% correct.

SPEM Tasks

All subjects were asked to follow a small circle of light that subtended a visual angle of 1.25°, presented on a computer screen 56 cm in front of the subject. There were 2 eye movement tasks. In the first, the target moved horizontally and sinusoidally at a temporal frequency of 0.4 Hz. The amplitude of the excursion was 28° peak to peak; maximum velocity was 35.2° per second. The color of the circle changed unpredictably and the subject was asked to count silently the number of times the color changed. This manipulation enhances attention to the task and minimizes anticipatory saccades.22

The second target presentation employed a subset of the step-ramp introduced by Rashbass.23 The target, located straight ahead, remained stationary for a short period that varied quasirandomly from 1 to 3 seconds, and then jumped either right or left of the central fixation point (the "step"). After a pause (200 milliseconds), the target began to move slowly in a horizontal direction opposite to that of the jump (the "ramp"), crossing the midline 200 milliseconds later. The ramp velocities were 5°, 10°, and 20° per second. Both the directions and velocities were unpredictable from trial to trial. There were 4 trials at each of the 3 velocities and, for each direction of movement, for a total of 24 trials. Because the normal initial smooth pursuit response, which occurs within about the first 100 milliseconds, is to the perceived target movement alone and does not yet involve any corrective feedback from target position or from an efference copy of an eye movement, it is termed "open-loop." Sustained smooth pursuit then follows. This phase of eye tracking, during which corrections are based on the feedback of retinal slip, is usually measured by closed-loop gain.

Eye Movement Apparatus and Recording

The apparatus for recording eye movements was a fully computerized limbus tracker (Eye and Brain Technologies Inc, Thessaloníki, Greece). It consisted of photodiodes, sensors that receive infrared reflections from the eyes, an amplifier, and a digitizer. The sensors were placed on spectacle frames that the subject wore during the eye movement recordings. The sampling rate for eye position was 1000 Hz. Eye position was calibrated to ±12°. Eye position signals were recorded by a computer that also controlled the presentation of the moving targets. Each subject's head was immobilized by use of a custom-fitted bite bar made of dental compound.

Principal Measures

The data were analyzed both qualitatively and quantitatively using custom-designed software. For data obtained from sinusoidal tracking, a 5-point qualitative rating system,7 modified from the scale of Benitez,24 was used to characterize the integrity of the eye tracking. A score of 1 indicated very good eye tracking, and a score of 5 indicated very impaired eye tracking. The qualitative ratings mainly take account of the frequency of intrusive and corrective saccades as well as the peak gain.10 Two judges, highly experienced in rating SPEM, independently rated each sinusoidal recording; interrater agreement was more than 95%.

Data from the pursuit records were also analyzed quantitatively. For the sinusoidal targets, we computed peak gain and frequency of saccades. After excluding saccades (defined as eye movements faster than 60° per second, or 1.7 times the maximum velocity of the target), blinks, and square wave jerks,10 we calculated peak gain, defined as the ratio of eye velocity to target velocity, both averaged across a range of ±200 milliseconds around the maximum velocity of the target for every cycle of target movement. The number of saccadic events during 30 seconds of tracking was another quantitative index of eye tracking performance. For the step-ramp target we computed OLA, defined as the mean acceleration of initial pursuit, beginning 130 milliseconds after the onset of the ramp component of each of the 24 step-ramp trials and lasting 100 milliseconds.12 Eye acceleration was obtained by computing the second derivative of eye position signals after filtering by a second-order Butterworth low-pass filter (cutoff frequency, 50 Hz); trials containing saccades during this open-loop period were excluded.

In summary, 4 eye movement measures were used to evaluate smooth pursuit: a qualitative rating, peak gain, saccade frequency (all for the sinusoidal target), and OLA (for the step-ramp target).

Data Analysis

Using Pearson product-moment correlations separately within the schizophrenia group and the 8 normal controls, we examined the association between velocity discrimination and each of the 4 measures of smooth pursuit. We also analyzed the differences between the patients and normal controls with respect to OLA at the 3 ramp velocities by a repeated-measures analysis of variance. To determine whether our version of the step-ramp procedure was equivalent to the standard Rashbass presentation (no delay between step and ramp),23 we tested 20 additional subjects (approximately equal numbers of patients and controls) on both paradigms; the correlation between OLA on both procedures was 0.69 (P<.005), with no outliers. The 2 paradigms thus give similar OLA data. The effects of medication status and the differences between schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients on each of the measures were assessed by t tests, with significance fixed at an α level of .05, 2 tailed. There were no significant differences between these 2 patient groups on any of the measures used in this study. The 11 patients receiving antipsychotic medications did not differ from the 4 patients receiving no antipsychotic medications with respect to open-loop and peak gain, saccade frequency, qualitative score, and motion sensitivity.

Results
Velocity discrimination

The previous study of motion perception1 established that the contrast sensitivities for velocity discrimination were significantly lower in schizophrenic patients, as compared with normal controls, when the velocities to be compared differed by 20% (11° per second vs 9° per second) (Table 1). Contrast sensitivities for contrast detection and orientation discrimination and for judging 2 velocities that differed by 100% (15° per second vs 5° per second), however, were similar for both groups.

Smooth pursuit

Figure 1 shows representative responses of a normal control and a schizophrenic subject to the sine wave target. Compared with the normal control, the patient's sustained eye tracking appears to have lower gain and is consistently accompanied by saccades. Figure 2 shows representative responses of a normal control and a schizophrenic patient to the 20° per second step-ramp target. The normal control initiates a clearly accelerating pursuit eye movement about 150 milliseconds after the target begins to move in its ramp trajectory. In contrast, the patient's eye movement shows a very low initial acceleration.

Relation of velocity discrimination to initiation of smooth pursuit

We calculated the mean initial acceleration for all subjects during the open-loop period for the step-ramp targets. These values are plotted as a function of the 3 ramp velocities in Figure 3. In both the schizophrenic patients and the normal controls, initial acceleration progressively increased as ramp velocity increased. However, the initial acceleration of the schizophrenic patients was significantly lower than that of the normal controls for all 3 ramp velocities (F1,21=5.49,P=.03 at 5° per second; F1,21=4.78, P=.04 at 10° per second; F1,21=9.39,P=.006 at 20° per second). These data agree with previous reports of low initial acceleration in schizophrenic patients.8,9,25

The Pearson correlation coefficient between OLA for the ramp at 10° per second and velocity discrimination is 0.70 (P<.01), which indicates that about 50% of the variance in OLA can be accounted for by contrast sensitivity for velocity discrimination. The observed correlation suggests a strong relationship between velocity discrimination and OLA, but the confidence intervals (CIs) are necessarily wide because of the limited sample size (90% CI, 0.38-0.87). Therefore, a weaker but still a nonzero effect cannot be definitely ruled out. Both measurements (velocity discrimination and OLA) were obtained with the target moving at or around 10° per second, presumably within the optimal range for motion processing. Correlation coefficients with similar magnitudes were obtained between velocity discrimination and OLA for ramp targets of 5° per second (r=0.59, P<.05) and 20° per second (r=0.65, P<.01). Those schizophrenic patients with low contrast sensitivity for velocity discrimination show low initial acceleration, as seen in the scatter diagram of that relationship (Figure 4). For the 8 normal controls, none of the correlations between OLA and velocity discrimination were statistically significant.

We also tested motion discrimination around a base velocity of 20° per second.1 These faster-moving targets resulted in higher thresholds for both groups of participants. Schizophrenic patients, however, were again significantly less sensitive than the normal controls. The correlations between OLA and velocity discrimination between velocities of 18° per second and 22° per second followed the same pattern, with the same regression slopes as those around the 10° per second base.

Relation of velocity discrimination to maintenance of pursuit
Qualitative Rating

Five (33%) of the 15 patients had qualitative ratings of 4 or 5, indicating ETD. The correlation between the qualitative ratings of SPEM and the contrast sensitivities for discrimination of the 20% velocity differences was 0.25 (P=.37; 90% CI, −0.22 to 0.62). Thus, overall sustained eye tracking, as measured by global qualitative ratings, seems not to be significantly related to sensitivity in detecting small velocity changes.

Peak Gain

For the schizophrenia group and the normal control group, the mean (±SD) peak gain for the 0.4 Hz sine wave target was 0.75 (±0.10) and 0.80 (±0.04), respectively. The 5 patients with qualitative ratings of 4 or 5 had a mean peak gain score of 0.73 (±0.07), which was significantly lower than that of the normal controls (t=2.62, P<.05). The scatter diagram (Figure 5) of the relationship between mean peak gain and contrast sensitivities for velocity discrimination shows that patients with low contrast sensitivities for velocity discrimination tended to have low peak gain. The correlation between velocity discrimination and peak gain for the sine wave was 0.53 (P<.05; 90% CI, 0.12-0.79).

Saccade Frequency

The correlation between saccade frequency and velocity discrimination in the schizophrenic patients was −0.39 (P=.15; 90% CI, −0.71 to 0.06). Although not reaching a statistically significant level, the negativity of the correlation is consistent with the relation described above between peak gain and velocity discrimination. That is, patients with low contrast sensitivity for velocity discrimination tended to make more saccades to compensate for low gain pursuit.

Comment

We report that velocity discrimination, previously shown to be impaired in schizophrenic patients,1 is associated with lowered OLA and closed-loop gain. This association contributes to our understanding of the origin of ETD in schizophrenia.

Motion discrimination deficit: a component of etd

Both OLA and closed-loop gain involve detection of motion signals. To initiate a normal pursuit movement in response to a visual stimulus that has begun to move, the viewer must first be aware that the target is moving. Open-loop acceleration, which appears to be a pure response to motion signals from the sensory system, is reduced when motion detection is impaired. During this period of open loop, no feedback is available from the effects of a previous eye movement response to the target, since none has yet been made. Cognitive factors such as anticipation of target movement may also influence pursuit initiation, a factor that merits separate study. Steady state or peak gain, which is involved in maintaining smooth pursuit, is also associated with impairments in velocity discrimination, and is affected by previous eye movements, such as the feedback when retinal slip occurs. It has been suggested that the position of a moving target drives sustained eye tracking.26,27 It is, therefore, possible that schizophrenic patients with impaired velocity discrimination partially compensate for their motion perception deficit by reliance on position cues to keep the fovea on a moving target, particularly when target velocity is relatively slow and when target movement is predictable. This interpretation may partially explain why the qualitative ratings of sustained tracking target are poorly related to velocity discrimination, whereas measures of OLA and closed-loop gain are significantly related to velocity discrimination. The qualitative rating reflects more than the velocity match between eye and target movement. It no doubt includes position match and some extraretinal events (such as predictive tracking), which appear as irregularities in the record and which are not taken into account in the acceleration and gain measurements. We note here that correlations between OLA and orientation discrimination (r=0.17, P=.56) and contrast sensitivity (r=0.40,P=.08) are not significant, indicating that the results of this study do not reflect impaired motivation and generalized deficit performance frequently found in schizophrenia.

Are motion-sensitive areas of the brain implicated in etd?

The processing of visual signals in the brain is carried out in many different areas of the visual system. These areas respond to different attributes of a visual stimulus, such as color, form, slant, and motion.28 It is now known that the MT and MST areas have a large population of cells devoted to visual motion processing, including detection of changes in velocity.29,30 Moreover, these same areas play a principal role in the adaptive control of eye movements, including SPEM.31 Wurtz et al,32 for example, reported that a punctate chemical lesion in monkey MT produced a deficit in motion perception tasks and in the onset of smooth pursuit. These deficits are quite similar to those seen in the present experiment in the initial eye tracking responses of schizophrenic patients with compromised motion discrimination. Studies also show that the lateral dorsal area of MST is involved in the regulation of the maintenance of pursuit.32 The cells in this region respond to both retinal and extraretinal signals, the latter perhaps representing a proprioceptive input that is relevant to pursuit maintenance.

Because MT and MST are involved in both motion perception and smooth pursuit, any irregularity in the neural responses of these 2 areas should affect performance on both types of tasks. This indeed was the outcome of the present experiment, which showed that schizophrenic patients whose velocity discrimination was poor had lowered open-loop and peak gains.33 Stuve et al34 had earlier reported that responses to the direction of motion in a coherent motion task and performance on a smooth pursuit task are significantly correlated in schizophrenic patients. Our present findings show that sensitivity to perception of velocity is implicated. Both studies suggest that impaired functioning of motion-sensitive areas such as MT and MST may be causally implicated in the ETD found in schizophrenic patients.

Involvement of other brain areas in etd

Smooth pursuit eye movements, including planning and executing them, are complex processes. They involve many brain areas, as has been shown in numerous physiological and brain-lesion studies. Those areas involved in the generation of smooth pursuit include both cortical regions—such as MT and MST and frontal eye fields (FEF)—and subcortical regions such as the basilar pons and cerebellum. It is conceivable, however, that after the motion signals are processed and relayed, other brain areas, such as those in the frontal lobes, play important roles in executing and maintaining smooth pursuit based both on current eye movements and on target movement. Information from eye velocity and eye position are also used adaptively to control on-line smooth pursuit, which may not solely be registered in MT and MST. Information about target movement may be conveyed not only from updated sensory signals but also from memory of, or extrapolation from, previously retained knowledge about the target motion; the latter would implicate neuronal activity in the frontal lobes. Indeed, Bruce et al35 found so-called "pursuit neurons" in FEF. Other reports36,37 showed clear activity of neurons in several areas of the frontal lobes while monkeys tracked a small target. Smooth pursuit to a target with predictable movement is usually spared when lesions occur in other brain areas but impairments were observed in a patient with damage to the FEF.38

With respect to schizophrenia, it has been shown that the behavioral impairment in at least 2 independent tasks is related to functional integrity in the frontal lobe. First, spatial working memory, which is associated with neural responses of area 46 in the frontal lobe,39 was found to be impaired in schizophrenic patients.40 Second, endogenous (sustained), but not exogenous (transient), attention engagement, in which prefrontal cortex may be involved,41 was compromised in schizophrenia.42 These considerations suggest that smooth pursuit impairment in schizophrenia may reflect contributions from nonsensory components, particularly during pursuit maintenance.

Many of the brain areas mentioned are, of course, interconnected with each other. Area MT is located within the occipitoparietal cortex of the rhesus monkey. Maunsell and Van Essen43 traced projections from the striate cortex to MT via the striate areas V2 and V3, and from there to the superior temporal sulcus and parietal cortex, which includes MST and the inferior parietal lobule. These areas contain neurons that project to the FEF,44 which also discharge during SPEM. Lesions to FEF and MT and MST all cause very profound impairment of SPEM.45 Similarly, after comprehensively reviewing the effects on SPEM of various diseases and specific lesions, Sharpe and Morrow46 conclude that MT, MST, inferior parietal lobule, and the FEF are critically implicated in specific abnormalities of SPEM, including lowered gain. The existence of the network linking these brain areas makes it difficult to be certain, when one brain area (such as MT) is damaged, whether the resulting SPEM impairment is caused by damage to this area or by the effects of such damage on other connected areas of the network, such as the MST or FEF.

In the case of schizophrenia, several previous eye tracking studies attribute the dysfunction to prefrontal involvement.47 The neural activities of the frontal and prefrontal lobes in SPEM of schizophrenic patients should be considered in the context that these brain areas also receive inputs from, or provide feedback to, the posterior parietal lobe, where motion information is primarily processed (but also see Nawrot and Rizzo48 for a role of the cerebellum in visual motion processing). Two studies of schizophrenic patients25,49 reported that patients' initial saccades to a step-ramp target were similar to those of the normal controls. The authors concluded that the patients' generation of unimpaired saccades to a moving target indicated that motion processing was intact, because an earlier study of monkeys with lesions in the motion-sensitive MT area50 showed impaired saccades to moving targets. However, we must point out that generating saccades to moving or stationary visual targets may rely on position as well as motion information. Therefore a direct assessment of motion discrimination, as was done in our earlier study,1 is required to decide the issue of whether motion processing is impaired in schizophrenia. It is noteworthy, moreover, that Newsome et al50 reported that a decrease in initial eye velocity in smooth pursuit accompanied damage to area MT, and Clementz,25 in a study of saccades to moving targets in schizophrenia, also reported low OLA. In this article, we show that low OLA is significantly related to raised motion discrimination thresholds. We therefore suggest that whereas MT and MST are functionally involved in abnormal generation of smooth pursuit, the impairment in the maintenance of smooth pursuit in schizophrenia probably also implicates frontal and prefrontal areas.36,37 Our finding of a significant functional relation between impaired velocity discrimination and reduced OLA and closed-loop gain in schizophrenic patients represents only the beginning phase of unraveling these complex processes.

Accepted for publication November 13, 1998.

This work was supported in part by grants MH 31154, MH 31340, MH 49487, MH 44866, and MH 01021 from the United States Public Health Service, Washington, DC; a grant from the Roy A. Hunt Foundation, Pittsburgh, Pa; and by a research grant from the Scottish Rite Schizophrenia Fund, Lexington, Mass.

Portions of this work were presented at the Cornell University Symposium on Schizophrenia, Ithaca, NY, November 8, 1996; at the Biennial International Congress on Schizophrenia Research, Colorado Springs, Colo, April 15, 1997; and at the annual meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Ft Lauderdale, Fla, May 11, 1997.

We thank Anne Grossetete, MS, for help in data analysis and Laurie Teraspulsky, PhD, for help with recruiting subjects.

Reprints: Philip S. Holzman, PhD, Psychology Research Laboratory, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill St, Belmont, MA 02178.

References
1.
Chen  YPalafox  GPNakayama  KLevy  DLMatthysse  SHolzman  PS Motion perception in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56149- 154Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Holzman  PSProctor  LRHughes  DW Eye tracking patterns in schizophrenia.  Science. 1973;181179- 181Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Holzman  PSProctor  LRLevy  DLYasillo  NJMeltzer  HYHurt  SW Eye tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients and their relatives.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31143- 151Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Cegalis  JASweeney  JA Eye movements in schizophrenia: a quantitative analysis.  Biol Psychiatry. 1979;1413- 26Google Scholar
5.
Iacono  WGLykken  DT Eye tracking and psychopathology.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;361361- 1369Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Iacono  WGLykken  DT Two-year retest stability of eye tracking performance and comparison of electrooculographic and infrared recording techniques: evidence of EEG in the electrooculogram.  Psychophysiology. 1981;1849- 55Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Shagass  CAmadeo  MOverton  D Eye-tracking performance in psychiatric patients.  Biol Psychiatry. 1974;9245- 260Google Scholar
8.
Levin  SLuebke  AZee  DHain  TRobinson  DRHolzman  PS Smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenics: quantitative measurements with the search-coil technique.  J Psychiatr Res. 1988;22195- 206Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Clementz  BAMcDowell  JE Smooth pursuit in schizophrenia: abnormalities of open- and closed-loop responses.  Psychophysiology. 1994;3179- 86Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSMatthysse  SMendell  NR Eye tracking and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.  Schizophr Bull. 1993;19461- 536Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Leigh  RJZee  DS The Neurology of Eye Movements. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa F.A. Davis Co1991;
12.
Lisberger  SEvinger  CJohanson  GFuchs  A Relationship between eye acceleration and retinal image velocity during foveal smooth pursuit in man and monkey.  J Neurophysiol. 1981;46229- 249Google Scholar
13.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSProctor  LR Vestibular responses in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35972- 981Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Latham  CHolzman  PSManschrek  TTole  J Optokinetic nystagmus and pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38997- 1003Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Lipton  RBLevin  SHolzman  PS Horizontal and vertical pursuit movements, the oculocephalic reflex, and the functional psychoses.  Psychiatry Res. 1980;3193- 203Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Levin  SJones  AStark  LMerrin  ELHolzman  PS Saccadic eye movements of schizophrenic patients measured by reflected light technique.  Biol Psychiatry. 1982;171277- 1287Google Scholar
17.
Iacono  WGTuason  VBJohnson  RA Dissociation of smooth pursuit and saccadic eye tracking in remitted schizophrenics: an ocular reaction time task that schizophrenics perform well.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38991- 996Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Newsome  WTWurtz  RH Probing visual cortical function with discrete chemical lesions.  Trend Neurosci. 1988;11394- 400Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Baloh  RWYee  RDHonrubia  V Optokinetic nystagmus and parietal lobe lesions.  Ann Neurol. 1980;7269- 276Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Spitzer  RWilliams  JGibbon  MFirst  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Patient Version.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1990;
21.
Levitt  D The transformed up-down methods.  J Acoust Soc Am. 1972;10465- 479Google Scholar
22.
Shagass  CRoemer  RAAmadeo  M Eye tracking performance and engagement of attention.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976;33121- 125Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Rashbass  C The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements.  J Physiol. 1961;159326- 338Google Scholar
24.
Benitez  JT Eye tracking and optokinetic tests: diagnostic significance in peripheral and central vestibular disorders.  Laryngoscope. 1970;80834- 848Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Clementz  BA Saccades to moving targets in schizophrenia: evidence for normal posterior cortex functioning.  Psychophysiology. 1996;33650- 654Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Pola  JWyatt  HJ Target position and velocity: the stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements.  Vision Res. 1980;20523- 534Google ScholarCrossref
27.
McPeek  RMVerstraten  PCavanagh  P Target position, not target velocity, drives sustained smooth pursuit.  Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;385932Google Scholar
28.
Zeki  SM Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey.  J Physiol (Lond). 1974;236549- 573Google Scholar
29.
Van Essen  DCMaunsell  JHR Hierarchical organization and the functional streams in the visual cortex.  Trend Neurosci. 1983;6370- 375Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Maunsell  JHRNewsome  WT Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci. 1987;10363- 401Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Newsome  WTPare  EB A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT).  J Neurosci. 1988;82201- 2211Google Scholar
32.
Wurtz  RHYamasaki  CJDuffy  CJRoy  JP Functional specialization for motion processing in primate cerebral cortex.  Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology. Cold Spring Harbor, NJ Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press1990;717- 727Google Scholar
33.
Levy  DLLajonchere  CDorogusker  BZither  RTartaglini  ALieberman  JASzymanski  SLee  SMendell  NR Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia, I: quantitative characterization in first-episode patients.  Schizophr Res. In press.Google Scholar
34.
Stuve  TAFriedman  LJesberger  JAGilmore  GCStrauss  MEMeltzer  HY The relationship between smooth pursuit performance, motion perception and sustained visual attention in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls.  Psychol Med. 1997;27143- 152Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Bruce  CJGoldberg  MEBushnell  MCStanton  GB Primate frontal eye fields, II: physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements.  J Neurophysiol. 1985;54714- 734Google Scholar
36.
MacAvoy  MGGottlieb  JPBruce  CJ Smooth-pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field.  Cereb Cortex. 1991;195- 102Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Heinen  SJ Single neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth pursuit eye movements.  Exp Brain Res. 1995;104357- 361Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Morrow  MJSharpe  JA Cerebral hemispheric localization of smooth pursuit asymmetry.  Neurology. 1990;40274- 292Google Scholar
39.
Goldman-Rakic  PS Cellular basis of working memory.  Neuron. 1995;14477- 485Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Park  SHolzman  PS Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49975- 982Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Posner  MIPetersen  SEFox  PTRaichle  ME Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain.  Science. 1988;2401627- 1631Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Sereno  ABHolzman  PS Spatial selective attention in schizophrenic, affective disorder, and normal subjects.  Schizophr Res. 1996;2033- 50Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Maunsell  JHRVan Essen  DC Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey, I: selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation.  J Neurophysiol. 1983;491127- 1147Google Scholar
44.
Maioli  MGSquatrito  SGalletti  CBattaglini  PPSanserino  ER Cortico-cortical connections from the visual region of the superior temporal sulcus to frontal eye field in the macaque.  Brain Res. 1983;265294- 299Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Lynch  JC Frontal eye field lesions in monkeys disrupt pursuit.  Exp Brain Res. 1987;68437- 441Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Sharpe  JAMorrow  MJ Cerebral hemispheric smooth pursuit disorders.  Acta Neurol Belg. 1991;9181- 96Google Scholar
47.
Levin  S Frontal lobe dysfunctions in schizophrenia: eye movement impairments.  J Psychiatr Res. 1984;1827- 55Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Nawrot  MRizzo  M Motion perception deficits from midline cerebellar lesions in humans.  Vis Res. 1995;35723- 731Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Thaker  GKRoss  DEBuchanan  RWMoran  MJLahti  AKim  CMedoff  D Does pursuit abnormality in schizophrenia represent a deficit in the predictive mechanism?  Psychiatry Res. 1996;59221- 237Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Newsome  WTWurtz  RHDursteler  MRMikami  A Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the Macaque monkey.  J Neurosci. 1985;5825- 840Google Scholar
×