Background
The Jerusalem Infant Development Study is a prospective investigation comparing offspring of schizophrenic parents with offspring of parents who have no mental disorder or have nonschizophrenic mental disorders. During infancy and school age, a subgroup of offspring of schizophrenic parents showed global neurobehavioral deficits that were hypothesized to be indicators of vulnerability to schizophrenia. The purposes of the present investigation were to determine if neurobehavioral deficits were present in the offspring of schizophrenics at adolescence, to examine their stability over time, and to explore their relation to concurrent mental adjustment.
Methods
Sixty-five Israeli adolescents were assessed on a battery of neurologic and neuropsychological assessments. They were also administered psychiatric interviews from which best-estimate DSM-III-R diagnoses and scores of global adjustment were derived.
Results
Adolescents with poor neurobehavioral functioning were identified from composites of motor and cognitive-attentional variables. A disproportionate number of offspring of schizophrenic parents (42%; 10/24), and especially male offspring of schizophrenic parents (73%; 8/11), showed poor neurobehavioral functioning relative to offspring of nonschizophrenic parents (22%; 9/41). Adolescent offspring of schizophrenics with poor neurobehavioral functioning had been poorly functioning at earlier ages and had poor psychiatric adjustment at adolescence. All 4 offspring of schizophrenics receiving schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses by adolescence showed a pattern of poor neurobehavioral functioning across developmental periods.
Conclusions
Results are consistent with the hypothesis that individuals at genetic risk for schizophrenia may display lifelong neurobehavioral signs that are indicators of vulnerability to schizophrenia and that are associated with psychiatric adjustment generally and schizophrenic spectrum disorder specifically.
SCHIZOPHRENIC patients show neurobehavioral deficits in a variety of motor, visual-motor, attentional, and cognitive tasks.1-6 While some neurobehavioral signs may simply accompany symptoms of schizophrenic illness, family studies suggest that some may also be indicators of genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. First-degree nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients are more likely than individuals with no schizophrenic relatives to have abnormalities in smooth-pursuit eye movements,7-9 grip-induced muscle tension,10 perceptual motor speed,11,12 sustained attention,13,14 and mental flexibility.15-17 Offspring of schizophrenics show deficits on the Continuous Performance Test,18-21 the Span of Apprehension Test,22 eye-tracking tasks,23,24 the visual backward masking procedure,25 and fine motor coordination tasks.26-29 Anomalous patterns of neurobehavioral development have been observed in offspring of schizophrenics as early as the first days of life.30-39 Debate continues about which specific neurobehavioral signs show the greatest sensitivity and specificity to schizophrenia and whether specific or general deficits are better indicators of vulnerability to schizophrenia.35-40
Although studies of first-degree relatives of schizophrenics suggest that neurobehavioral signs are genetic vulnerability indicators, this view would be strengthened by evidence that neurobehavioral deficits are stable over time and are associated with psychopathology, particularly with disorders in the schizophrenia spectrum. The longitudinal high-risk studies of offspring of schizophrenics are uniquely well suited to examine these issues.
To date, high-risk studies have reported stability of neurobehavioral signs from infancy to middle childhood29 and from childhood through adolescence.27,41 They have also shown correlations between childhood measures of neurobehavioral functioning, particularly attentional functioning, and later indicators of general adjustment19,41-48 and schizophrenia spectrum disorders.49-51 The anomalous neurodevelopmental pattern of infancy termed pandysmaturation has also been related to adult schizophrenia spectrum disorder.52,53
The present article reports on the development of adolescents at risk for schizophrenia from the Jerusalem Infant Development Study (JIDS). Within this longitudinal sample, more global neurobehavioral signs were previously identified in infant and school-aged offspring of schizophrenic parents than in children whose parents had no mental disorder or nonschizophrenic disorders. Assuming that neurobehavioral signs are indicators of genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia, in this article we will examine hypotheses that (1) during adolescence neurobehavioral signs will be more prevalent in adolescent offspring of schizophrenics than in other young people, (2) adolescent offspring of schizophrenics with neurobehavioral signs will have shown signs at earlier developmental stages, and (3) neurobehavioral signs will be associated with psychiatric adjustment at adolescence, as well as with adolescent-onset schizophrenia spectrum disorder.
Original study sample: infancy and school age
The original JIDS sample was recruited from 1973 through 1977 by identifying pregnant women from Jerusalem's maternal and child care centers and mental health clinics.32 Research Diagnostic Criteria diagnoses54 were made for all biological mothers and fathers based on a Hebrew-language version of the Current and Past Psychopathology Scales55 and/or psychiatric and social work records for individuals who had received treatment. Based on these diagnoses, families were assigned to 3 groups: schizophrenic parent, parent with nonschizophrenic mental disorder, and parent with no mental illness.
A follow-up of the JIDS children was conducted when the children were a mean age of 10.3 years. Siblings of the original children who had not been part of the infant cohort were added to the school-age sample if they were between the ages of 8 and 13 years.
At the school-age follow-up, more than 80% of biological parents were interviewed using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version.56 Hospital and clinic records were assembled for all parents receiving mental health treatment. If parents were deceased or unavailable, spouses provided mental health history updates. Based on all available information, revised lifetime parent diagnoses were made using DSM-III-R57 criteria and, where necessary, changes made in group assignment. All but 7 parents were available for research psychiatric interviewing at the infant and/or school-age assessments, and those 7 (3 schizophrenics, 1 mentally ill spouse of a schizophrenic, 2 nonschizophrenics with mental disorders) had sufficiently complete psychiatric or social worker records to make group assignment.
Beginning in 1992, a follow-up of the JIDS sample was conducted when the offspring were between the ages of 14 and 21 years (mean, 17.56; SD, 1.75). Sixty-five adolescents participated in the follow-up: 29 females, 36 males; 24 from the schizophrenic group, 25 from the other mental illness group, and 16 from the no mental illness group (Table 1). The parent diagnosis groups did not differ significantly in terms of mean age of adolescents (17.8 years for schizophrenia, 17.5 years for other mental illness, and 17.3 years for no mental illness) or proportion of males (46% [11/24] for schizophrenia, 56% [14/25] for other mental illness, and 69% [11/16] for no mental illness). Seven families (8 offspring) chose not to participate in the adolescent follow-up and 1 youth was excluded from participation because of a head injury that impacted neuropsychological functioning.
During the adolescent follow-up, parents were rediagnosed only if additional treatment records were available. These records required some revised diagnoses and eliminated some previously "questionable" diagnoses, but did not necessitate changes in group assignment.
Written informed consent was obtained from youth participants older than 18 years and from parents of those younger than 18 years.
Offspring Neurobehavioral Assessments
Adolescents were administered a neurobehavioral battery during two 212-hour sessions at the Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel. Examiners were a psychiatrist (for neurologic examination) and master's degree–level psychologists (for all other instruments) trained to reliability and unaware of parents' diagnoses. The battery included assessments used in the National Institute of Mental Health Israeli High-Risk Study28,58 and the school-age JIDS, with additional tests found in other studies to be sensitive to schizophrenic diathesis.38,59,60 The battery included neurologic tasks scored on 4-point clinical scales (indicating no, mild, moderate, or severe impairment)61-63 and averaged across multiple presentations of items as well as standard administered neuropsychological tests and procedures.58-60,64-81 For analyses in this article, 20 summary variables were selected from these instruments (Table 2).
Offspring Psychiatric Assessment
During home visits, each adolescent and 1 parent were administered 2 diagnostic interviews: the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Epidemiologic Version (K-SADS-E)82 and the Semi-Structured Kiddie Interview for Personality Syndromes (K-SKIPS),83,84 which, modeled after the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Disorders,85 was designed to assess personality syndromes falling within the schizophrenia spectrum. Parental information was used primarily to corroborate or add to the information provided by the adolescent. Where available, additional information from school records and mental health providers was obtained. All available information was used to make best-estimate DSM-III-R diagnoses57 and Children's Global Assessment Scale ratings.86 The interviewers included a senior psychiatrist and 2 other experienced clinicians who had received specialized training on administration of the K-SADS-E and K-SKIPS. Twenty randomly selected cases were blindly reviewed and independently diagnosed by 2 psychiatrists. Interrater agreement was high, although this random sample did not contain cases within the schizophrenia spectrum. All 7 cases diagnosed within the schizophrenia spectrum were independently reviewed by one of the senior investigators (J.M.), who agreed that all belonged within the spectrum. Disagreement on the specific schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis occurred for 1 case and was resolved by further discussion among all authors.
In the infancy and school-age phases of this project, global measures of neurobehavioral functioning derived from 2-dimensional data structures were more effective than specific items or tests at discriminating a subgroup of poorly functioning offspring of schizophrenic parents from other children.29 To produce a 2-dimensional data structure, the 20 adolescent neurobehavioral variables were subjected to a 2-factor principal components analysis with varimax rotation. Three of the variables (abnormal arm movements, tendon reflexes, and eye movements) had loadings less than 0.35. The principal components analysis was recomputed without these 3 variables. The rotated components in this analysis explained 20.3% and 18.9% of the total variance. Variables with loadings of at least 0.35 on the first component were primarily cognitive-attentional; variables with loadings of at least 0.35 on the second component were primarily motoric (Table 2). The 17 variables were standardized, and cognitive-attentional and motor component scores were computed by averaging items with unit weights. Bender scores were entered into both the motor and cognitive-attentional averages. All variables were scaled before averaging so that positive scores indicated more problematic behavior.
To divide the sample into subjects with good and poor neurobehavioral functioning, the contours of an Epanechnikov kernel87-89 were superimposed on a plot of subjects' 2 principal components scores (Figure 1). This nonparametric procedure identifies the most concentrated region of data points in an empirical bivariate distribution. Because we wanted an empirical basis for identifying a region of normality, we used only the subjects with mentally healthy parents as a basis for computing the kernel and set program defaults so that approximately 68% of subjects fell within the kernel. Subjects from all 3 groups whose data points fell within the kernel, as well as any whose data points fell outside the boundary of the kernel but in the direction of better than average performance, were considered to be functioning well. All others outside the boundaries of the kernel were considered functioning poorly.
The relation of parent diagnosis to offspring neurobehavioral functioning was examined using contingency tables and logistic regressions predicting to the dichotomous neurobehavioral outcome from dummy variables for parental diagnosis, sex, and age. The stability of good and poor neurobehavioral outcomes over time was examined using contingency tables and κ statistics as a measure of association appropriate for dichotomous variables. The relation of neurobehavioral functioning to offspring psychopathology was examined using an analysis of variance with Children's Global Assessment Scale scores as the dependent variable and neurobehavioral functioning (2 levels) and parental diagnosis (3 levels) as independent variables.
All statistical tests are 2-tailed. For testing primary hypotheses, α levels were set at P=.05.
Diagnostic group differences
Forty-two percent (n=10) of the offspring of schizophrenic parents showed poor neurobehavioral functioning compared with 22% (n=9; 5 from the other mental illness and 4 from the no mental illness group) of the offspring of nonschizophrenic parents.
The logistic regression model predicting poor neurobehavioral functioning was significant (χ23=8.05, P<.05). Male children were nearly 4 times more likely to be poorly functioning (odds ratio, 3.99; 95% confidence interval, 1.12-14.20) than females. Trends suggested that offspring of schizophrenics were more than 3 times as likely to be poorly functioning as offspring of parents with other mental illness (odds ratio, 3.64; 95% confidence interval, 0.93-14.24; P=.06) and no mental illness (odds ratio, 3.15; 95% confidence interval, 0.70-14.15; P=.13). Since the odds ratios for the 2 comparison groups were similar, they were combined to provide greater statistical power and regressions recomputed. In this analysis, offspring of schizophrenic parents were more than 3 times as likely to be poorly functioning as offspring of nonschizophrenic parents (odds ratio, 3.43; 95% confidence interval, 1.03-11.42; P<.05).
Because inclusion of siblings in the sample violates the statistical assumption of independent sampling, we explored whether a few sibships were unduly contributing to the finding of poor functioning in offspring of schizophrenics. This was not the case. In the 9 schizophrenic sibships, there were only 2 in which both siblings were poorly functioning and 2 in which both were well functioning.
Sex differences moderated the relation of parent diagnosis to poor neurobehavioral functioning. Seventy-three percent of the male offspring of schizophrenics (8 of 11) were poorly functioning, compared with 24% of the male offspring of nonschizophrenic parents (6 of 25; χ21=7.63, P=.006), 15% of the female offspring of schizophrenics (2 of 13; χ21=8.06, P=.005), and 19% of the female offspring of nonschizophrenic parents (3 of 16; χ21=7.87, P=.005).
Because in the JIDS school-age follow-up, pregnancy and birth complications (PBCs) were related to poor motoric functioning in the offspring of schizophrenic parents, we explored the role of PBCs for the 36 adolescent subjects with Research Obstetric Scale90 data. Within the group of male offspring of schizophrenics, there was a strong trend for PBCs to be related to poor motoric functioning (component 2) (r=0.64, P<.09, 2-tailed; n=8). The 3 male offspring of schizophrenics with more than 4 PBCs (numbers 45, 58, 61 in Figure 1) all showed poor motoric functioning. Exploratory analyses suggested no relation between PBCs and poor motoric behavior in female offspring of schizophrenics or offspring without a schizophrenic parent. Pregnancy and birth complications were also unrelated to poor cognitive-attentional functioning.
Stability of poor functioning from earlier ages to adolescence
Offspring of schizophrenic parents showed greater stability in neurobehavioral functioning over age (Cohen κ=0.92) than offspring of nonschizophrenic parents (κ=0.48). More offspring of schizophrenics (42%; n=10) were poorly functioning at both ages than offspring of mentally healthy parents (12.5%, n=2; χ21=3.89, P<.05) or than offspring of parents with other mental disorders (12%, n=3; χ21=5.53, P<.05) (Table 3). In contrast, stable good functioning across 2 age periods occurred for a high proportion of the offspring of schizophrenics (54%; n=13) and the offspring of nonschizophrenics (71%; n=29).
Similar analyses comparing neurobehavioral functioning at infancy and adolescence suggest considerably less stability. The offspring of schizophrenic parents showed only very modest stability from infancy (Cohen κ=0.32) and the offspring of nonschizophrenic parents showed no stability (Cohen κ=0.08).
Although many children did show shifts in functioning across age periods, fully half of the 40 children tracked from birth showed consistent levels of functioning across infancy, school age, and adolescence. Twelve offspring of nonschizophrenics (48%) showed consistently good functioning at all 3 developmental periods, and none showed consistently poor functioning. Four offspring of schizophrenics (27%) showed consistently good functioning, and 6 (40%) showed consistently poor functioning.
Relation of neurobehavioral functioning to global psychiatric adjustment
Analysis of variance computed on the Children's Global Assessment Scale scores using parent diagnosis (3 levels) and neurobehavioral functioning at adolescence (2 levels) as the independent variables showed a significant effect for neurobehavioral functioning (F1,56=8.61, P<.001) and no effects for parent diagnosis (F2,56=2.25) or the interaction between parent diagnosis and neurobehavioral functioning (F2,56=2.25). Post hoc Fisher least significant difference tests indicated that the offspring of schizophrenic parents with poor neurobehavioral functioning had poorer mean Children's Global Assessment Scale scores (n=8; mean, 49.13; SD, 13.67) than the offspring of schizophrenic parents with good neurobehavioral scores (n=13; mean, 71.08; SD, 12.06; P<.001), the offspring of parents with other mental illness who had poor (n=5; mean, 64.80; SD, 14.84; P<.05) or good (n=20; mean, 67.80; SD, 13.85; P<.001) neurobehavioral scores, and the offspring of parents with no mental illness who had good neurobehavioral scores (n=12; mean, 72.42; SD, 14.47; P<.001). None of the other groups differed from one another.
Relation of neurobehavioral functioning to schizophrenic spectrum illness
Although the JIDS offspring were early in the period of risk for schizophrenic breakdown at the time of follow-up, 4 youth with schizophrenic parents received diagnoses in the schizophrenia spectrum: 1 schizophrenia, 1 schizotypal personality disorder, and 2 paranoid personality disorder. All 4 showed a stable pattern of poor neurobehavioral functioning at school age and adolescence. Three also showed poor infant functioning as reported by Marcus et al32; the fourth was not in the sample during infancy. In an independent analysis of the infant behavior of these children, Fish et al53 identified 2 of these infants as having probable pandysmaturation (developmental problems and growth retardation) and another as having a developmental pattern that was consistent with pandysmaturation but inconclusive because of missing physical growth data. An additional 3 male offspring without schizophrenic parents also received paranoid personality disorder diagnoses, but did not show consistently poor neurobehavioral functioning.
Three types of evidence from the adolescent follow-up of JIDS converge to suggest that neurobehavioral signs may be markers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: (1) global neurobehavioral signs were more prevalent in offspring of schizophrenic parents than in other young people; (2) neurobehavioral signs were stable over development for a subgroup of offspring of schizophrenic parents, but not other young people; (3) for offspring of schizophrenic parents, neurobehavioral signs were associated with psychopathology and adolescent schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
When the JIDS sample was assessed during the first year of life, more offspring of schizophrenics showed problems in motor and sensorimotor behavior than offspring of parents with no mental disorder or nonschizophrenic mental disorder. During school age (7-13 years) when the JIDS children and their similarly aged siblings were assessed, again a subgroup of offspring of schizophrenics showed poor motor and cognitive functioning. The present article, reporting on the adolescent follow-up of the original JIDS sample and their siblings, again identifies a subgroup of children of schizophrenics with poor neurobehavioral functioning. Overall, 42% (n=10) of the offspring of schizophrenics were poorly functioning at adolescence compared with only 22% (n=9) of the offspring of parents with other mental disorders or no mental disorder. During the school-age and infancy assessments, the proportion of poorly functioning offspring of schizophrenics had been 44% and 68%, respectively, compared with 20% and 23% for the children without schizophrenic parents. After controlling for age and sex in logistic regression analyses, offspring of schizophrenics were more than 3 times as likely as other young people to have neurobehavioral signs.
Although we had not originally hypothesized sex differences, adolescents with neurobehavioral signs, particularly those with motoric signs, were predominantly male offspring of schizophrenics. The incidence of poor neurobehavioral functioning was 73% in the males with a schizophrenic parent compared with less than 25% in the female offspring of schizophrenics and male and female offspring of nonschizophrenics. Although the incidence of schizophrenia is similar between males and females, sex differences abound in the study of schizophrenia.91-93 Castle and Murray94 have even hypothesized that male schizophrenics typically suffer from a different type of schizophrenia than females, with schizophrenia in males characterized by more neurobehavioral and neuroanatomical signs and more closely associated with PBCs than genetic vulnerability. Data from the present study are consistent with this hypothesis in that at-risk males show poorer neurodevelopmental course prior to illness onset and their poor functioning, at least in the motoric domain, may be associated with a history of PBCs in the small number of subjects for whom PBC data were available. However, data from the present study also suggest that the role of genetics cannot be ignored in considering the course of neurodevelopmental problems in schizophrenia for males, since neurobehavioral signs were only increased in males with PBCs and a schizophrenic parent. We believe the most parsimonious explanation of the data is that males at genetic risk are especially vulnerable to the effects of prenatal hazards.
The global neurobehavioral measures used in this study showed stability across ages. This stability was strong between school age and adolescence, although only modest between infancy and adolescence. As reported in other high-risk studies,41,95 the stability was greater for the offspring of schizophrenics than for the children with nonschizophrenic parents and most notably was anchored by a poorly functioning subgroup of offspring of schizophrenics. Fully 42% of the offspring of schizophrenics had been poorly functioning at both school age and adolescence, compared with only 12% of the offspring of nonschizophrenics. Six of the 40 children in the sample observed from birth through adolescence have shown consistently poor neurobehavioral functioning over time. All 6 of these children were the offspring of schizophrenics.
An association was found between the neurobehavioral measures and psychopathology. Other high-risk studies have found linkages between neurobehavioral signs (especially attentional deviance) and concurrent global psychopathology19,41 and later schizophrenia spectrum disorders.49,50,52,96 The present study determined that the adolescent offspring of schizophrenics showing neurobehavioral signs were at risk for poorer global psychiatric adjustment as well as specific schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
At the time JIDS began, virtually no empirical attention had been given to biobehavioral measures indicating enhanced vulnerability to schizophrenia. Fish96 had described a pattern of anomalous early development she termed "pandysmaturation," which she proposed was "a ‘marker' in infancy for the inherited neurointegrative defect in schizophrenia." We interpreted the JIDS infancy findings as suggesting "some kind of genetically determined vulnerability to schizophrenia."32 Today, hosts of studies on first-degree relatives of schizophrenics have searched for behavioral, neurochemical, neuroanatomical, and genetic vulnerability indicators of schizophrenia,97,98 and a number of theoretical conceptions of vulnerability indicators have evolved from these (see further Kremen et al99). The JIDS adolescent follow-up data, in showing associations between neurobehavioral signs and genetic risk for schizophrenia, stability in neurobehavioral signs over time, and correlation between these signs and psychopathology, continue to support the hypothesis that global neurodevelopmental deficits may be premorbid indicators of genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. Because the measures we have used at adolescence, school age, and infancy are global, we do not claim that they are specific to schizophrenia. In fact, they occur, although with lower incidence and little stability, in offspring of parents with no mental illness and offspring of parents with other, nonschizophrenic mental disorders.
Researchers studying the brain basis of schizophrenia are suggesting that the types of neuroanatomical anomalies associated with schizophrenia do not result from degenerative processes, but from developmental processes that may begin early in life.100,101 The JIDS longitudinal data, combined with the pioneering work of Fish,96 the recent studies of Walker et al,102,103 and an accumulation of other infancy data from the high-risk field,33 add further support for the view that schizophrenia is a neurodevelopmental disorder with origins very early in life.
The primary limitation of the present study, and most high-risk studies, is its sample size. Results need to be interpreted with caution and in relation to findings from other studies. A second limitation is that, because of the long-term longitudinal design, measurement techniques are not always contemporary by the time of follow-up. Even in the most recent follow-up, limitations of budget and technology available in Israel did not allow for state-of-the-art measures such as neuroimaging or brain potentials. The greatest limitation of the present report is that none of the subjects in the sample have yet passed through the period of risk for schizophrenia and conclusions about actual schizophrenic illness must be treated cautiously.
Reprints: Sydney L. Hans, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, MC3077, The University of Chicago, 5841 S Maryland Ave, Chicago, IL 60637.
Accepted for publication March 30, 1999.
Collection of adolescent data and preparation of this article were supported by grant R01 MH45208 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, Md. Collection of infancy data was supported by grants from the US-Israel Binational Science Foundation (grant 598), Jerusalem, Israel; the Chief Scientists' Office of the Israel Ministry of Health, Jerusalem; the Olivetti Foundation, Boston, Mass; the Center for the Study of Human Sciences of the Hebrew University, Jerusalem; the Department of Psychiatry, the University of Chicago, Chicago, Ill; Forest Hospital Foundation, Des Plaines, Ill, and Harry Jacobs, Cleveland, Ohio. Collection and analysis of school-age follow-up data were supported by the W. T. Grant Foundation, New York, NY; the Scottish Rite Schizophrenia Research Program, Lexington, Mass; the Sturman Center of Human Development, Hebrew University; and by a gift from Sarah Cowan, Cleveland.
Data at the adolescent follow-up were collected by Miriam Barasch, MSW, Nomi Ban, MA, Batya Aloni, Slava Feinstein, MD, Sharon Arnon, MA, Nurit Kaveh, MA, Nili Mor, MA, and Gil Amihai, MA; the database was managed by Linda Henson. Statistical consultation was provided by Leland Wilkinson, PhD, Department of Statistics, Northwestern University, Evanston, Ill.
1.Cox
SMLudwig
AM Neurological soft signs and psychopathology: incidence in diagnostic groups.
Can J Psychiatry. 1979;24668- 673
Google Scholar 2.Manschrek
TCAmes
D Neurologic features and psychopathology.
Biol Psychiatry. 1984;19703- 719
Google Scholar 3.Heinreichs
DWBuchanan
RW The significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1988;14511- 18
Google Scholar 4.Nasrallah
HAWeinberger
DR The Handbook of Schizophrenia: The Neurology of Schizophrenia. Vol 1 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1986;
5.Steinhauer
SRGruzelier
JHZubin
J Handbook of Schizophrenia: Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing. Vol 5 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;
6.Seidman
LJCassens
GPKremen
WSPepple
JR Neuropsychology of schizophrenia. White
RFed.
Clinical Syndromes in Adult Neuropsychology: The Practitioner's Handbook. New York, NY Elsevier Science Inc1992;381- 449
Google Scholar 7.Clementz
BASweeney
JAHirt
MHaas
G Pursuit gain and saccadic intrusions in first-degree relatives of probands with schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1992;99327- 335
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Holzman
PSProctor
LRLevy
DLYasillo
NJMeltzer
HYHurst
SW Eye-tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients and their relatives.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31143- 151
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Keefe
RSSilverman
JMMohs
RCSiever
LJHarvey
PDFriedman
LRoitman
SELDuPre
RLSmith
CJSchmeidler
JDavis
KL Eye tracking, attention, and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54169- 176
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Rosen
AJLockhart
JJGants
ES Maintenance of grip-induced muscle tension: a behavioral marker of schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100583- 593
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Keefe
RSESilverman
JMLees Roitman
SEHarvey
PDDuncan
MAAlray
DSiever
LJDavis
KLMohs
RC Performance of nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients on cognitive tests.
Psychiatry Res. 1994;531- 12
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Pogue-Geile
MFGarrett
AHBrunke
JJHall
JK Neuropsychological impairments are increased in siblings of schizophrenic patients [abstract].
Schizophr Res. 1991;4390
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Steinhauer
SRZubin
JCondray
RShaw
DBPeters
JLVan Kammen
DP Electrophysiological and behavioral signs of attentional disturbance in schizophrenics and their siblings. Tamminga
CASchulz
SCeds.
Schizophrenia Research Advances in Neuropsychiatry and Psychopharmacology. Vol 1 New York, NY Raven Press1991;169- 178
Google Scholar 15.Faraone
SVSeidman
LJKremen
WSPepple
JRLyons
MJTsuang
MT Neuropsychological functioning among the nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: a diagnostic efficiency analysis.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104286- 304
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Pogue-Geile
MF Siblings of schizophrenic probands: presence of neuropsychological impairments [abstract].
Schizophr Res. 1990;362
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Franke
PMaier
WHain
CKlingler
T Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: an indicator of vulnerability to schizophrenia?
Schizophr Res. 1992;6243- 249
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Nuechterlein
KH Signal detection in vigilance tasks and behavioral attributes among offspring of schizophrenic mothers and among hyperactive children.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1983;924- 28
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Cornblatt
BErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Early attentional predictors of adolescent behavioral disturbances in children at risk for schizophrenia. Watt
NAnthony
EJWynn
LRolfe
Jeds.
Children at Risk for Schizophrenia A Longitudinal Perspective New York, NY Cambridge University Press1984;198- 211
Google Scholar 20.Rutschmann
JCornblatt
BErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Sustained attention in children at risk for schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;34571- 575
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Rutschmann
JCornblatt
BErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Sustained attention in children at risk for schizophrenia: findings with two visual continuous performance tests in a new sample.
J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1986;14365- 385
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Asarnow
RFSteffy
RAMacCrimmon
DJCleghorn
JM An attentional assessment of foster children at risk for schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1977;86267- 274
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Schreiber
HStolz-Born
GBorn
JRothmeier
JRothenberger
AJurgens
RBecker
WKornhuber
HH Visually-guided saccadic eye movements in adolescents at genetic risk for schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 1997;2597- 109
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Rosenberg
DRSweeney
JASquires-Wheeler
EKeshavan
MSCornblatt
BAErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Eye-tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York High-Risk Project: diagnostic specificity and the role of attention.
Psychiatry Res. 1997;66121- 130
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Green
MFNuechterlein
KHBreitmeyer
B Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients: evidence for a vulnerability indicator.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54465- 472
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Erlenmeyer-Kimling
LCornblatt
BFriedman
DMarcuse
YRutschmann
JSimmens
S Neurological, electrophysiological and attentional deviations in children at risk for schizophrenia. Nasrallah
HAHenn
FAeds.
Schizophrenia as a Brain Disease. New York, NY Oxford University Press Inc1982;61- 98
Google Scholar 27.Marcus
JHans
SLLewow
EWilkinson
LBurack
CM Neurological findings in high-risk children: childhood assessment and 5-year followup.
Schizophr Bull. 1985;1185- 100
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Marcus
JHans
SLMednick
SSchulsinger
FMichelsen
N Neurological dysfunctioning in offspring of schizophrenics in Israel and Denmark: a replication analysis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42753- 761
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Marcus
JHans
SLAuerbach
JGAuerbach
AG Children at risk for schizophrenia: the Jerusalem Infant Development Study, II: neurobehavioral deficits at school age.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50797- 809
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Fish
BAlpert
M Abnormal states of consciousness and muscle tone in infants born to schizophrenic mothers.
Am J Psychiatry. 1962;119439- 445
Google Scholar 31.Fish
BDixon
WJ Vestibular hyporeactivity in infants at risk for schizophrenia: its association with critical developmental disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35963- 971
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Marcus
JAuerbach
JWilkinson
LBurack
CM Infants at risk for schizophrenia: the Jerusalem Infant Development Study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38703- 713
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Hans
SLMarcus
J Neurobehavioral development of infants at risk for schizophrenia: a review. Walker
EFed.
Schizophrenia A Life-Course Developmental Perspective. New York, NY Academic Press Inc1991;33- 57
Google Scholar 34.Schubert
EWBlennow
GMcNeil
TF Wakefulness and arousal in neonates born to women with schizophrenia: diminished arousal and its association with neurological deviations.
Schizophr Res. 1996;2249- 59
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Blanchard
JJNeale
JM The neuropsychological signature of schizophrenia: generalized or differential deficit?
Am J Psychiatry. 1994;15140- 48
Google Scholar 36.Braff
DLHeaton
RKuck
JCullum
MMoranville
JGrant
IZisook
S The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48891- 898
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Flashman
LAFlaum
MGupta
SAndreasen
NC Soft signs and neuropsychological performance in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153526- 532
Google Scholar 38.Asarnow
RFGranholm
ESherman
T Span of apprehension in schizophrenia. Steinhauer
SRGruzelier
JHZubin
Jeds.
Handbook of Schizophrenia: Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing. Vol 5 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;335- 370
Google Scholar 39.Nuechterlein
K Vigilance in schizophrenia and related disorders. Steinhauer
SRGruzelier
JHZubin
Jeds.
Handbook of Schizophrenia: Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing. Vol 5 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;397- 433
Google Scholar 40.Saykin
AJGur
RCGur
REMozley
PDMozley
LHResnick
SMKester
BStafiniak
P Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48618- 624
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Winters
LCornblatt
BErlenmeyer-Kimling
L The prediction of psychiatric disorders in late adolescence. Walker
Eed.
Schizophrenia A Life-Course Developmental Perspective. New York, NY Academic Press Inc1991;123- 137
Google Scholar 42.Erlenmeyer-Kimling
LACornblatt
BA The New York High-Risk Project: a followup report.
Schizophr Bull. 1987;13451- 461
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Cornblatt
BMarcuse
Y Children at high risk for schizophrenia: predictions from childhood to adolescence. Erlenmeyer-Kimling
LMiller
Neds.
Life-span Research on the Prediction of Psychopathology. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates1986;81- 100
Google Scholar 44.Erlenmeyer-Kimling
LCornblatt
BGolden
R Early indicators of vulnerability to schizophrenia in children at high genetic risk. Guze
SBEarls
FJBarrett
JEeds.
Childhood Psychopathology and Development. New York, NY Raven Press1983;247- 261
Google Scholar 45.Cornblatt
BErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Global attentional deviance as a marker of risk for schizophrenia: specificity and predictive validity.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1985;94470- 486
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Cornblatt
BALenzenweger
MDworkin
RErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Positive and negative schizophrenic symptoms, attention, and information processing.
Schizophr Bull. 1985;11397- 407
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Dworkin
RHCornblatt
BAFriedmann
RKaplansky
LMLewis
JARinaldi
AShilliday
CErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Childhood precursors of affective vs social deficits in adolescents at risk for schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1993;19563- 577
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Erlenmeyer-Kimling
LCornblatt
BARock
DRoberts
SBell
MWest
A The New York High-Risk Project: anhedonia, attentional deviance, and psychopathology.
Schizophr Bull. 1993;19141- 153
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Marcus
JHans
SLNagler
SAuerbach
JGMirsky
AFAubrey
A A review of the NIMH Israeli Kibbutz-City Study and the Jerusalem Infant Development Study.
Schizophr Bull. 1987;13425- 437
Google ScholarCrossref 50.Hans
SLMarcus
J A process model for the development of schizophrenia.
Psychiatry. 1987;50361- 370
Google Scholar 51.Mirsky
AFIngraham
LJKugelmass
S Neuropsychological assessment of attention and its pathology in the Israeli cohort.
Schizophr Bull. 1995;21193- 204
Google ScholarCrossref 52.Fish
B Infant predictors of the longitudinal course of schizophrenic development.
Schizophr Bull. 1987;13395- 409
Google ScholarCrossref 53.Fish
BMarcus
JHans
SLAuerbach
JGPerdue
S Infants at risk for schizophrenia: sequelae of a genetic neurointegrative defect: a review and replication analysis of pandysmaturation in the Jerusalem Infant Development Study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49221- 235
Google ScholarCrossref 54.Spitzer
RLEndicott
JRobins
E Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. New York2nd ed. Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute1975;
55.Endicott
JSpitzer
RL Current and Past Psychopathology Scales (CAPPS): rationale, reliability, and validity.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1972;27678- 687
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Spitzer
RLEndicott
J Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version. New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1975;
57.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
58.Lifshitz
MKugelmass
SKarov
M Perceptual-motor and memory performance of high-risk children.
Schizophr Bull. 1985;1174- 84
Google ScholarCrossref 59.Manschreck
TCMaher
BARucklos
MWVereen
DRAder
DN Deficient motor synchrony in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1981;90321- 328
Google ScholarCrossref 60.Grant
DABerg
EA A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem.
J Exp Psychol. 1948;38404- 411
Google ScholarCrossref 61.Touwen
BCPrechtl
HFR The neurological examination of the child with minor nervous dysfunction.
Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. 8. London, England William Heinemann Medical Books Ltd1970;
Google Scholar 62.Luria
AR Higher Cortical Functions in Man. New York, NY Basic Books1966;
63.Luria
AR The Working Brain: An Introduction to Neuropsychology. New York, NY Basic Books1973;
64.Wechsler
DHartogs
R The clinical measurement of anxiety: an experimental approach.
Psychiatr Q. 1945;19618- 635
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Purdue Research Foundation, Examiner's Manual for the Purdue Pegboard. Chicago, Ill Science Research Associates1948;
66.Wechsler
D Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised. New York, NY The Psychological Corp1974;
67.Reitan
RM Investigation of the validity of Halstead's measures of biological intelligence.
Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1955;7328- 35
Google ScholarCrossref 68.Reitan
RMTarshes
EL Differential effects of lateralized brain lesions on the Trail Making Test.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1959;129257- 262
Google ScholarCrossref 70.Loong
J Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–IBM Version. San Luis Obispo, Calif Wang Neuropsychological Laboratories1991;
71.Rosvold
HEMirsky
AFSarason
IBransome
ED
JrBeck
LH A continuous performance test of brain damage.
J Consult Psychol. 1956;20343- 350
Google ScholarCrossref 72.Nuechterlein
KHParasuraman
RJiang
Q Visual sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time.
Science. 1983;220327- 329
Google ScholarCrossref 73.Nuechterlein
KHAsarnow
RF Continuous Performance Test (CPT) Program for IBM-Compatible Microcomputers, Version 4, for Degraded Stimulus CPT. Los Angeles, Calif Nuechterlein and Asarnow1990;
74.Estes
WKTaylor
HA Visual detection in relation to display size and redundancy of critical elements.
Percept Psychophysics. 1966;19- 16
Google ScholarCrossref 75.Asarnow
RFMacCrimmon
DF Span of apprehension deficits during the post-psychotic stages of schizophrenia: a replication and extension.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;381006- 1011
Google ScholarCrossref 76.Asarnow
RFNuechterlein
KH Span of Apprehension Program for IBM-Compatible Microcomputers, Version 4. Los Angeles, Calif Asarnow and Nuechterlein1991;
78.Ingraham
LJChard
FWood
MMirsky
AF A Hebrew language version of the Stroop Test.
Percept Mot Skills. 1988;67187- 192
Google ScholarCrossref 79.Birch
HGBelmont
L Auditory-visual integration in brain damaged and normal children.
Dev Med Child Neurol. 1965;7135- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 80.Bender
L A Visual Motor Test and Its Clinical Use. New York, NY American Orthopsychiatric Association1938;American Orthopsychiatric Association Research Monograph No. 3.
81.Pascall
GRSuttell
BJ The Bender-Gestalt Test: Quantification and Validity for Adults. New York, NY Grune & Stratton Inc1951;
82.Orvaschel
HPuig-Antich
J Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Epidemiologic Version. Ft Lauderdale, Fla Nova University, Center for Psychological Study1987;
83.Asarnow
JRTalovic
S Semi-Structured Kiddie Interview for Personality Syndromes (K-SKIPS). Los Angeles University of California, Los Angeles, Dept of Psychiatry1986;
85.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R, Personality Disorders. New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1986;
86.Shaffer
DGould
MSBrasic
JAmbrosini
PFisher
PBird
HAluwahlia
S A Children's Global Assessment Scale (CGAS).
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;401228- 1231
Google ScholarCrossref 87.Silverman
BW Density Estimation for Statistics and Data Analysis. London, England Chapman & Hall1986;
88.Scott
DW Multivariate Density Estimation: Theory, Practice, and Visualization. New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1992;
89.Wilkinson
L SYSTAT: The System for Statistics. Evanston, Ill SYSTAT Inc1990;
90.Zax
MSameroff
AJBabigian
HM Birth outcomes in the offspring of mentally disordered women.
Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1977;47218- 230
Google ScholarCrossref 91.Lewine
RRJ Gender and schizophrenia. Tsuang
MTSimpson
JCeds.
Handbook of Schizophrenia: Nosology, Epidemiology and Genetics of Schizophrenia. Vol 3 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1988;379- 397
Google Scholar 93.Goldstein
JMTsuang
MT Gender and schizophrenia: an introduction and synthesis of findings.
Schizophr Bull. 1990;16179- 183
Google ScholarCrossref 94.Castle
DJMurray
RM The neurodevelopmental basis of sex differences in schizophrenia.
Psychol Med. 1991;21565- 575
Google ScholarCrossref 95.Marcus
JHans
SByhouwer
BNorem
J Relationships among neurological functioning, intelligence quotients and physical anomalies.
Schizophr Bull. 1985;11101- 106
Google ScholarCrossref 96.Fish
B Neurobiologic antecedents of schizophrenia in children.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1977;341297- 1313
Google ScholarCrossref 97.Kremen
WSSeidman
LJPepple
JRLyons
MJTsuang
MTFaraone
SV Neuropsychological risk indicators for schizophrenia: a review of family studies.
Schizophr Bull. 1994;20103- 119
Google ScholarCrossref 98.Moldin
SOErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Measuring liability to schizophrenia: progress report 1994: editors' introduction.
Schizophr Bull. 1994;2025- 29
Google ScholarCrossref 99.Kremen
WSTsuang
MTFaraone
SVLyons
MJ Using vulnerability indicators to compare conceptual models of genetic heterogeneity in schizophrenia.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1992;180141- 152
Google ScholarCrossref 100.Weinberger
DR Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44660- 669
Google ScholarCrossref 103.Walker
EFGrimes
KEDavis
DM Childhood precursors of schizophrenia: facial expressions of emotion.
Am J Psychiatry. 1993;1501654- 1660
Google Scholar