Background
Cognitive impairment is recognized as a core characteristic of schizophrenia. There has always been a debate about the nature, selectivity, and time of onset of these deficits in relationship to the onset of illness and treatment factors. To our knowledge, the present study represents the largest sample of mostly neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia that has been reported to date.
Methods
A group of 94 patients experiencing their first episode of schizophrenic illness and 305 normal comparison subjects were administered a comprehensive clinical and neuropsychological evaluation. Seventy-three patients were neuroleptic naive, 14 had received treatment for less than 1 week, and the remaining 7 had been medicated for less than 2 weeks.
Results
Patients performed significantly worse than the comparison subjects on every neuropsychological variable except those assessing savings scores (ie, forgetting over time). Twenty-five of 30 tests had an effect size (ES) greater than 0.75 when the 2 groups were compared. An ES analysis within the schizophrenia group revealed that the greatest relative impairments were on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised digit symbol (ES, −0.52) and comprehension (ES, −0.42) subscales.
Conclusions
Our findings are in concert with others demonstrating that significant cognitive impairment across multiple ability domains is a core characteristic of schizophrenia and is not caused by chronic illness, treatment, or institutionalization. The ES analysis emphasizes that patients with schizophrenia have a generalized deficit that is not easily explained by a single anatomical region or ability area.
IMPAIRED PERFORMANCE on standardized neuropsychological tests is recognized as a core characteristic of schizophrenia.1-5 There has always been debate about the nature, selectivity, and time of onset of these cognitive dysfunctions in relationship to the onset of illness. Further characterization of cognitive deficits could be of considerable value in defining the nature of schizophrenia, determining its neurobiological basis, and understanding the types of problems patients have in their daily lives.6
Variations in neuropsychological findings are likely to occur, at least in part, because heterogeneous patient groups are studied at different stages of their illness and because of differences in the assessment procedures used. One method used to address some of the confounding factors that could influence neuropsychological performance is to examine the neuropsychological profile of patients experiencing their first episode of psychosis.1,7,8 Bilder et al1 examined 40 patients with first-episode schizophrenia after 6 months of treatment and concluded that patients with schizophrenia suffer both developmental and deterioration effects on intellectual functions. Unfortunately, their analysis was restricted to the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R).9 Hoff et al10 examined 56 first-episode patients and found a diffuse pattern of cognitive impairment compared with controls that improved over time. Although these studies attempted to avoid the confounding factors of institutionalization and chronicity, an important problem stems from the fact that they examined patients' performance after they had been treated with neuroleptic and anticholinergic medication, both of which have been found to influence cognition.11-13
To avoid the confounding factor of medication effects on cognition, Saykin et al14 offered the first study to examine a sample (n=37) of patients with first-episode schizophrenia who were never exposed to neuroleptics. They reported generalized impairment, with more pronounced impairment in verbal learning and memory, speeded visual-motor processing, and attention vigilance, and relative strengths in verbal intelligence, language function, spatial organization, and fine motor function skills. Most recently, Censits et al15 reported the results of a study with 28 neuroleptic-naive patients with schizophrenia. Their findings supported earlier findings of generalized deficits of all functions except motor skills, with a greater level of impairment on verbal memory.
All of these studies to date examined performance on a large battery of tests but used summary scores of multiple tests to reflect a certain cognitive domain. Although important for data reduction, the use of a global score can mask the different strategies, processes, and errors an individual may demonstrate. In addition, tests grouped within the same domain (eg, memory) may measure different processes (eg, encoding or retrieval). The cognitive demands of tests that are measuring the same domain often vary depending on the stimuli used (eg, recall of a story vs a word list), the manner in which the stimuli are presented (single vs multiple trials), and the procedure for data collection (timed vs nontimed tests). An example of the disadvantage of using summary scores is using a global score of multiple verbal tests to describe verbal intelligence and language functions. Several studies added measures of word knowledge (eg, vocabulary), social cognition (eg, comprehension), concept formation (eg, similarities), and fluency (controlled oral word association). Examining a summary score of these tests is certainly useful in that it gives an indication of an individual's verbal ability, particularly in a relatively small sample. However, measuring performance on each individual test can increase our understanding of the particular cognitive deficit contributing to overall decline in verbal functioning. This could be particularly important in the study of schizophrenia.
The examination of individual test scores and subscores within each test is valuable in that it allows us to dissect each cognitive function into its component processes. Profiling the performance of patients with schizophrenia on individual test scores could allow us to demonstrate impaired and spared cognitive processes.
Neuropsychological studies have repeatedly attempted to localize brain lesions in schizophrenia based on performance on standardized tests. However, the data generally fail to support any model of localization and instead support generalized impairment.16 To our knowledge, the present study represents the largest sample of mostly neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia that has been reported to date (n=94). We examined the neuropsychological performance of patients with schizophrenia to evaluate hypotheses of localized or differential cognitive impairments. In addition, the present study reports performance on individual cognitive tasks rather than summary scores and compares the performance of patients with schizophrenia with that of a normal comparison group. Because of the tendency of patients with schizophrenia to perform below normal on nearly all tests, we examined the relative strengths and weaknesses within patients' performances. We hypothesized that patients with schizophrenia would demonstrate cognitive dysfunction in all cognitive domains. In addition, we anticipated that effect size (ES) analyses of specific tests would indicate relative impairments in tasks requiring more complex information processing, reflecting dysfunction at higher processes.
Subjects, materials, and methods
Patients were recruited from consecutive admissions to the general psychiatric ward and to the Mental Health Clinical Research Center, University of Iowa Hospital and Clinics, Iowa City, starting in 1982. After written informed consent was obtained, patients underwent comprehensive screening and assessment. Patients with history of head trauma were excluded. Substance abuse was not used as an exclusion criterion; however, the rate of substance abuse was relatively low. Ninety-four patients (53 men and 41 women) were experiencing their first episode of illness and were neuroleptic naive at the time of admission; 73 of them were still unmedicated at the time of the neuropsychological assessment. The remaining 21 had been given medication prior to testing for relatively brief periods (median, 7 days; 14 were medicated for ≤8 days). When the 73 never-treated patients were analyzed separately, the results were identical to those for the larger group. Consequently, this report presents the results for the entire sample. Demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1.
All patients were evaluated with the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History.18 All available sources of information (hospital records and interviews with patients, informants, nurses, and social workers) were used in completing the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History. All patients were diagnosed using either DSM-III-R or DSM-IV diagnostic criteria. Patients were evaluated weekly using the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms19 and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms.20 Patients' global psychotic symptoms (ie, delusions and hallucinations) and global negative symptoms (ie, affective flattening, alogia, avolition, and anhedonia) were in the mild to moderate range (mean [SD], 2.5 [1.5] and 2.4 [1.0], respectively). Their disorganized symptoms (ie, bizarre behavior and formal thought disorder) were in the questionable to mild range (mean [SD], 1.6 [1.0]).
Normal comparison subjects were recruited from the community through newspaper advertisements. These individuals were screened for psychiatric illnesses using a structured interview based on the Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History.18 Exclusion criteria included diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorders or affective illness, diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder in first-degree relatives, medical illness that might affect cognition, history of neurological disorders, and past or present drug use or dependence. A group of 305 individuals (162 men and 143 women) under the age of 40 years was included in the analysis. Older individuals were excluded from this analysis to ensure age similarity among the patients. Demographic characteristics are summarized in Table 1.
Cognitive assessment occurred as soon as possible after admission. Patients were administered a comprehensive battery of tests that took approximately 4 hours to complete. Since the battery was updated periodically to integrate new research findings and to maximize coverage of all cognitive domains, sample size varied among tests. The battery included the Finger Tapping Test (average number of finger taps in 10 seconds),21 Trail Making Test (time to completion),22 Continuous Performance Test (number of hits),23 Benton Visual Retention Test (number of correct responses),24 Logical Memory Test, Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (immediate recall, delayed recall, and percentage saving scores),25 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition),26 Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (copy, immediate recall, delayed recall, and percentage saving scores),27,28 Wisconsin Card Sorting test (WCST) (number of categories and perseverative errors),29 Word Fluency Test (number of words generated in 3 minutes in response to the letters C, F, L),30 and Stroop Test (number correct on card 3 in 45 seconds and the calculated interference score).31 In addition, the following subscales from the WAIS-R were administered: digit span, digit symbol, vocabulary, information, comprehension, similarities, block design, object assembly, picture arrangement, and picture completion. Tests were administered by psychometrists who were trained in standardized assessment and had experience working with patients with schizophrenia. They all had at least an undergraduate degree in psychology and were supervised by a psychologist with a PhD degree.
Raw test scores were transformed into standard z scores using the means and SDs of age-stratified normal comparison individuals to control for age-related change in performance of cognitive measures. When applicable, tests were reverse-scored so that higher scores always reflected better performance. The schizophrenia group and the control group differed significantly in education. We covaried education to provide a conservative estimate of illness-related impairments. A series of analyses of covariance controlling for education were conducted on the neuropsychological test scores to evaluate the profiles. Group (patient vs control) was the between-group factor. A highly conservative Bonferroni correction was used to adjust the level of significance of all analyses (critical P=.002). All tests were 2-tailed. Since the F values in the analysis of covariance are heavily influenced by the sample size, which varied in this study, ES was computed using normal controls' SDs to measure the magnitude of the difference in performance between patients and controls on individual tests.
A follow-up analysis sequentially compared patients' scores for a particular test with the mean of their scores on the remaining tests in the profile. This procedure allowed the determination of selectively more pronounced deficits. Bonferroni correction was used to adjust the level of significance of the contrast analysis. The ESs were computed to measure the magnitude of differences.
Demographics and neuropsychological comparison
As expected, there was a significant difference between patients and controls in education (F1,395=76.13, P<.001). There were no significant differences between the 2 groups in age or parental education.
We compared the performance of the 21 patients who were briefly medicated prior to testing with that of those who had received no medication. There were no significant differences in their performance on any of the tests. Therefore, they were treated as one group in further analyses.
The means of the z scores (adjusted for education) of the neuropsychological battery for normal controls and patient groups on individual test scores are summarized in Table 2. After Bonferroni adjustment (critical P=.002), patients performed significantly worse than controls on all tests except the saving scores of both the Logical Memory Test and the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (the ratio of delayed recall to immediate recall) and the Stroop Test interference score (Table 2).
The ESs of these differences were computed using the normal controls' SDs. Table 3 lists these ESs. The differences between patients and controls were greatest on the WCST perseverative errors score (ES, 1.86), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test recognition score (ES, 1.70), WAIS-R digit symbol score (ES, 1.69), WAIS-R comprehension score (ES, 1.68), WAIS-R picture arrangement score (ES, 1.59), Continuous Performance Test number of hits score (ES, 1.56), and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test immediate recall score (ES, 1.53). The differences between patients and controls were least pronounced on the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test savings score (ES, 0.09), Stroop Test interference score (ES, 0.29), and Logical Memory Test savings score (ES, 0.47).
Comparing the patients' performance on each test with the mean of the remaining tests revealed significantly greater impairment in the WAIS-R digit symbol (ES, −0.52), WAIS-R comprehension (ES, −0.42), and WAIS-R picture arrangement (ES, −0.29) scores.
Relatively good performance was noted on the Stroop Test interference score (ES, 0.95), Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test savings score (ES, 0.70), WAIS-R digit span score (ES, 0.53), Finger Tapping Test for the left hand (ES, 0.50), Finger Tapping Test for the right hand (ES, 0.38), Logical Memory Test savings score (ES, 0.38), and WCST number of categories score (ES, 0.30). These results are summarized in Table 3.
To examine the extent to which patients' performance on the neuropsychological tests was affected by their current symptoms, the relationship between performance on the neuropsychological tests and symptom severity was investigated. Only negative symptoms were associated with impaired cognitive test performance, although none of the correlations exceeded 0.28 (Table 4).
As expected, our large sample of neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia was found to display substantial impairments in most aspects of cognition. These findings are consistent with previous neuropsychological studies of patients with first-episode schizophrenia as well as previous findings in studies of chronic schizophrenia.7,32-35 The presence of these deficits in a sample of patients very early in the course of illness, most of whom have never been medicated, reflects patterns of dysfunction that are primary and pathophysiologically related to schizophrenia without the confounding factors of long-term treatment, illness, or hospitalization.
On memory tests, patients were as impaired on tests of immediate recall as they were on tests of delayed recall. The current findings converge with those previously reported by Paulsen et al,36 who suggested that the memory difficulties shown in patients with schizophrenia are primarily caused by deficits in encoding and retrieval rather than storage.
We also found evidence of impairment in speeded cognitive tasks with and without a motor component, such as the WAIS-R digit symbol test and the Stroop Test part C. Patients' relatively better performance on finger tapping, a speeded motor task, compared with other timed tests, suggests that poor performance on speeded tasks reflects bradyphrenia rather than bradykinesia. Consistent with this finding is the observation that patients with schizophrenia were impaired on the Stroop Test measure reflecting cognitive speed. Specifically, patients' performance on the Stroop Test part C score was very different from that of the control group (ES, 1.3). In contrast, patients' Stroop Test interference score was not significantly different from that of controls and was significantly higher than their mean score on the remaining tests (ES, 0.95). This indicates that patients' poor performance on the Stroop Test is primarily caused by slowness in processing information.
Executive skills (eg, sequencing, organization, and flexibility) were among the areas that were highly impaired relative to controls. Similarly, we found evidence of more pronounced deficits in sustained attention and social cognition. These findings are consistent with our hypotheses and with several previous reports emphasizing frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia.
The WCST is frequently used in the study of schizophrenia. Our results point to a significant discrepancy between 2 scores of the WCST: the number of perseverative errors and the number of categories (paired t test: t83=4.44, P<.001). This finding may indicate that patients have greater difficulty with the cognitive flexibility than the concept formation requirements of the WCST. This explanation is consistent with their performance on the similarities subtest of the WAIS-R. Alternatively, this association may simply reflect that the WCST is less demanding as a conceptual task than as a test of cognitive flexibility. These findings suggest that careful analyses of specific variables on neuropsychological tests may be critical for proper interpretation of cognitive ability. Similarly, examining the patients' scores on tests with verbal components (ie, comprehension, vocabulary, similarities, information, and word fluency) also revealed discrepancies. Patients demonstrated more difficulties with a test that has a social cognition aspect (ie, comprehension) than with other verbal tasks (paired t test: t87=4.05, P<.001). Similarly, a paired t test revealed relatively worse performance on picture arrangement, a test that requires social cognition, compared with other WAIS performance subtests (ie, block design and picture completion) (t87=3.57, P<.001).
Symptoms were weakly correlated with some cognitive tasks. However, other tests were essentially unrelated to symptoms in first-episode schizophrenia.
Our findings of a generalized neurocognitive deficit should be interpreted in the context of the psychometric limitations of currently available neuropsychological measures. The inability to remove the possibly confounding factor of the differential discriminating power of the various tasks limits the interpretability of this and any other investigation of the neuropsychological functioning of patients with schizophrenia. Another limitation of this study could be that patients were tested while off medications during an acute episode. This might have made their performance worse than it would have been after they had received treatment. This hypothesis could only be tested if patients were tested after their condition had been stabilized after receiving treatment, which was outside the range of the current study. The inclusion of a subset of patients who were briefly treated prior to testing could be viewed as a potential limitation. However, we repeated the analyses on the never-medicated sample and the results were identical.
In general, these results emphasize a need for new models to understand schizophrenia. Old models are useful in explaining one cognitive deficit, but fall short of describing the complex and diverse phenomena of schizophrenia. Several research teams, including ours, have pursued this strategy and have proposed "candidate cognitive processes"37: eg, deficits in information processing, working memory, the inability to think in metarepresentations, and problems in representationally guided behavior.38-42 We have hypothesized that the cognitive disturbance in schizophrenia encompasses multiple functions, including learning, attention, speed of processing, and executive functions. To emphasize the diversity of the cognitive disturbance in schizophrenia and to call attention to the importance of disturbed cortical-cerebellar-thalamic-cortical circuits, we refer to the overarching deficit in schizophrenia as cognitive dysmetria.37 This concept emphasizes that schizophrenia reflects a disruption in fundamental circuitry in the brain, resulting in a generalized deficit in a basic cognitive process that leads to impairment in all cognitive systems and subsystems, including memory, attention, language, and executive functions. It highlights the importance of explaining the multiple symptoms of schizophrenia by identifying a basic mechanism and attempting to define its underlying neural circuits.
Corresponding author: Somaia Mohamed, MD, PhD, 2911 JPP, University of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, 200 Hawkins Dr, Iowa City, IA 52242-1057 (e-mail: smohamed@kety.psychiatry.uiowa.edu).
Accepted for publication March 30, 1999.
1.Bilder
RMLipschutz-Broch
LReiter
RGeizler
SHMayerhoff
DILieberman
JA Intellectual deficits in first-episode: evidence for progressive deterioration.
Schizophr Bull. 1992;18437- 448
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Heaton
RKBaade
LEJohnson
KL Neuropsycholgical test results associated with psychiatric disorders in adults.
Psychol Bull. 1978;85141- 162
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Kolb
BWhishaw
IQ Performance of schizophrenic patients on tests sensitive to left or right frontal, temporal, or parietal function in neurological.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1983;171435- 443
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Seidman
LJ Schizophrenia and brain dysfunction: an integration of recent neurodiagnostic findings.
Psychol Bull. 1983;94195- 238
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Goldberg
ESeidman
L Higher cortical functions in normals and in schizophrenia: a selective review. Steinhauer
SRGruzelier
JHZubin
Jeds.
Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology, and Information Processing: Handbook of Schizophrenia Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;5553- 597
Google Scholar 6.Green
MF What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153321- 330
Google Scholar 7.Hoff
ALRiordan
HO'Donnell
DWMorris
LDeLisi
LE Neuropsychological functioning of first-episode schizophreniform patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149898- 903
Google Scholar 8.Sweeney
JGretchen
LHShuhua
L Neuropsychological and eye movement abnormalities in first-episode and chronic schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1992;18283- 293
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Wechsler
D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. Cleveland, Ohio Psychological Corp1981;
10.Hoff
ARiordan
HO'Donnell
DStritzke
PNeale
CBoccio
AAnand
AKDeLisi
LE Anomalous lateral sulcus asymmetry and cognitive function in first-episode schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1992;18257- 270
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Frith
CD Schizophrenia, memory and anticholinergic drugs.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1984;98463- 472
Google Scholar 12.Medalia
AGold
JMerriam
A The effects of neuroleptics on neuropsychological test results of schizophrenics.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1988;3249- 319
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Bilder
RMTurkel
ELipschutz-Borch
LLieberman
JA Antipsychotic medication effects on neuropsychological functions.
Psychopharmacol Bull. 1992;28353- 366
Google Scholar 14.Saykin
AJShtasel
DLGur
GEKester
DBMozley
LHStafiniak
PGur
RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Censits
DMRagland
DJGur
RCGur
RE Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study.
Schizophr Res. 1997;24289- 298
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Heinrichs
RWZakzanis
KK Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrnia: a quantitative review of the evidence.
Neuropsychology. 1998;12426- 445
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Hollingshead
ABRedlich
FC Social Class and Mental Illness: A Community Sample. New York, NY John Wiley & Sons1958;
18.Andreasen
NCFlaum
MArndt
S The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History (CASH): an instrument for assessing diagnosis and psychopathology.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49615- 623
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City University of Iowa1983;
20.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City University of Iowa1984;
21.Reitan
RMDavidson
LA Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and Applications. New York, NY Hemisphere Publishing Corp1974;
22.Reitan
RMWoltson
D The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, Ariz Neuropsychology Press1985;
23.Valentino
DArruda
JGold
S Comparison of QEEG and response accuracy in good vs poorer performers during a vigilance task.
Int J Psychophysiol. 1993;15123- 133
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Benton
AL The Revised Visual Retention Test: Clinical and Experimental Applications. 4th ed. New York, NY Psychological Corp1974;
25.Wechsler
D Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1987;
26.Rey
A L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris, France Presses Universitaires de France1964;
27.Rey
A L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique.
Arch Psychol. 1941;28286- 340
Google Scholar 28.Osterrieth
PA Le test de copie d'une figure complexe.
Arch Psychol. 1994;30206- 356
Google Scholar 29.Heaton
RKChelune
GJTalley
JLKay
GGCurtiss
G Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla Psychological Assessment Resources1993;
30.Benton
ALHamsher
K Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City University of Iowa1976;
31.Golden
CJ Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Chicago, Ill Stoelting Co1978;
32.Braff
DLHeaton
RKuck
JCullum
MMoranville
JGrant
IZisook
S The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48891- 898
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Goldberg
TERagland
JDTorrey
EFGold
JMBigelow
LBWeinberger
DR Neuropsychological assessment of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;471066- 1072
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Taylor
MAAbrams
R Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1984;141196- 201
Google Scholar 35.Blanchard
JNeale
JM The neuropsychological signature of schizophrenia: generalized or differential deficit?
Am J Psychiatry. 1994;15140- 46
Google Scholar 36.Paulsen
JSHeaton
RKSadek
JRPerry
WDelis
DCBraff
DKuck
JZisook
SJeste
DV The nature of learning and memory impairments in schizophrenia.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1995;188- 99
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Andreasen
NCParadiso
SO'Leary
DS "Cognitive dysmetria" as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry?
Schizophr Bull. 1998;26203- 218
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Braff
DL Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1993;19233- 259
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Swerdlow
NRLipska
BKWeinberger
DRBraff
DLJaskiw
GEGeyer
MA Increased sensitivity to the sensorimotor gating-disruptive effects of apomorphine after lesions of medial prefrontal cortex or ventral hippocampus in adult rats.
Psychopharmacology. 1995;12227- 34
Google ScholarCrossref 40.Goldman-Rakic
PS Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6348- 357
Google Scholar 41.Frith
CDFriston
KLiddle
PFFrackowiak
RS Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET.
Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1991;244241- 246
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Frith
CD The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates1992;