Background
Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence of abnormal frontal and temporal lobe function in schizophrenia. Frontal cortex abnormalities have been associated with negative symptoms and temporal lobe abnormalities with positive symptoms. The deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia were predicted to differ in prefrontal cortical activity, but not in medial temporal lobe activity.
Methods
Regional cerebral blood flow was studied using oxygen 15 positron emission tomography during 3 different memory retrieval conditions in 8 control subjects, 8 patients with the deficit syndrome, and 8 patients without the deficit syndrome. Behavioral and positron emission tomography data were analyzed using a mixed-effects model to test for population differences.
Results
In all memory conditions, frontal cortex activity was higher in patients without the deficit syndrome than in patients with the deficit syndrome. During the attempt to retrieve poorly encoded words, patients without the deficit syndrome recruited the left frontal cortex to a significantly greater degree than did patients with the deficit syndrome. The 2 schizophrenia subtypes did not differ in the activity or recruitment of the hippocampus during memory retrieval.
Conclusion
Frontal cortex function during memory retrieval is differentially impaired in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia, whereas hippocampal recruitment deficits are not significantly different between the 2 schizophrenia groups.
NEUROIMAGING studies have revealed dysfunctional neural networks in schizophrenia.1-3 Studies of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and glucose metabolism have found abnormalities in frontal cortex and temporal lobe structures at rest as well as during the performance of cognitive tasks. There is, however, no pattern that is diagnostic for schizophrenia. For example, frontal and temporal cortex activity at rest have been found to be lower by some investigators but not by others.1 Similarly, frontal cortex recruitment during task performance was found to be decreased in some studies4-14 but not in others.15-17
The clinical heterogeneity of schizophrenia might explain why it is not associated with a pathognomonic abnormality of brain function. For example, frontal cortex activity at rest correlates inversely with the degree of negative symptoms,18-24 and left medial temporal lobe activity at rest correlates positively with the severity of psychopathology25,26 or the degree of reality distortion.23 Similarly, decreased frontal cortex recruitment during the performance of some cognitive tasks occurs primarily in patients with negative symptoms.4,11
Negative symptoms such as anhedonia, avolition, and affective blunting may be primary features of schizophrenia or may be secondary to depression or drug treatment. In about 15% of patients with schizophrenia, classified as having deficit syndrome, negative symptoms are primary and enduring features.27 Carpenter and colleagues28 have postulated that patients with deficit syndrome show more prominent prefrontal cortex deficits than the rest of the schizophrenia spectrum, but that all patients with schizophrenia show medial temporal lobe abnormalities.
Using positron emission tomography (PET), we have recently provided the first evidence for an impaired recruitment of the hippocampus in schizophrenia.29 Patients with schizophrenia lacked the normal modulation of hippocampal activity associated with different modes of memory recall,30,31 but frontal cortex activation was not impaired. We report on an extension of our previous sample to a larger group of patients with deficit or nondeficit syndrome32 similar in sex, age, duration of illness, and medication status. We tested the hypothesis that deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia differ in prefrontal cortical function, but not in medial temporal lobe function. To achieve this goal, we constrained encoding strategies (shallow or deep) to dissociate frontal cortex activation (associated with low recall accuracy) from hippocampal activation (associated with high recall accuracy).
We studied 16 male subjects with schizophrenia and 8 male control subjects. Data from 13 patients with schizophrenia and the 8 control subjects were previously analyzed to test for differences between patients with schizophrenia and normal controls.29
The patients with schizophrenia were recruited from an outpatient mental health clinic in Boston, Mass, and diagnosed according to DSM-IV criteria33 by an experienced clinician (D.G.). Control subjects were recruited by advertisement. All subjects provided written informed consent. Subjects were excluded if English was a second language or if they had a history of neurological or medical illness, current substance abuse, or lifetime substance dependence. The study was approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the Massachusetts General Hospital and the Central Office Research Review Committee of the Commonwealth of Massachusetts Department of Mental Health.
Eight of the 16 patients with schizophrenia were classified as having deficit syndrome schizophrenia and 8 were classified as having nondeficit syndrome schizophrenia using the Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome.32 The patients were assessed by a clinician trained in the administration of the schedule: the total score for negative symptoms ranged from 10 to 15 (minimum, 0; maximum, 24), and the Global Severity Rating scores were 2 (n = 1) or 3 (n = 7).
All subjects were right-handed34 and the 3 groups were similar in age (controls, 40.0 ± 6.3 years; patients with nondeficit syndrome, 42.6 ± 5.7 years; and patients with deficit syndrome, 40.0 ± 5.0 years). No control subject had a history of a psychiatric disorder as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R.35 The level of education (controls, 14.9 ± 1.1 years; patients with nondeficit syndrome, 12.8 ± 2.6 years; and patients with deficit syndrome, 13.2 ± 1.4 years) was significantly different only between the control and deficit syndrome groups (P = .02). The subjects' parental level of education was not significantly different between groups (controls, 12.6 ± 2.7 years; patients with nondeficit syndrome, 11.0 ± 2.6 years; and patients with deficit syndrome, 11.9 ± 0.6 years; controls vs nondeficit syndrome: P = .24; controls vs deficit syndrome: P = .50; and deficit vs nondeficit syndrome: P = .33). Both schizophrenia samples had a similar duration of illness (patients with nondeficit syndrome, 18.1 ± 6.3 years; patients with deficit syndrome, 17.5 ± 7.5 years; P = .86).
At the time of the PET experiment, schizophrenia symptoms were assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale36 and Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms.37 The patients with nondeficit syndrome scored higher on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale positive scale (patients with nondeficit syndrome, 17.0 ± 6.3; patients with deficit syndrome, 11.4 ± 2.6; P = .03). The patients with deficit syndrome scored higher on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale negative scale (patients with nondeficit syndrome, 17.6 ± 3.3; patients with deficit syndrome, 22.6 ± 6.1; P = .06) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (total score: patients with nondeficit syndrome, 32.4 ± 8.1; patients with deficit syndrome, 50.0 ± 15.8; P = .01).
All patients with schizophrenia were treated with typical neuroleptic drugs; the 2 schizophrenia groups did not differ significantly in medication dosage (chlorpromazine-equivalent dosages: patients with nondeficit syndrome, 350 ± 216 mg/d; patients with deficit syndrome, 450 ± 195 mg/d; P = .57). Two patients with nondeficit syndrome and 5 patients with deficit syndrome were treated with benztropine mesylate (1-2 mg/d).
Subjects performed 3 different tasks.29,30 During the baseline condition, subjects were instructed to complete 3-letter word stems (presented on a computer screen) into the first word that came to mind. Two other conditions (low recall and high recall), followed an off-line study session. During the study session, subjects were presented with a randomized list of 100 target words, consisting of 20 words presented once and 20 words presented 4 times. The subjects were instructed to count T junctions (ie, perpendicular lines that cross) in each letter of the target words presented once (perceptual encoding strategy) and to count meanings of the target words presented 4 times (semantic encoding strategy). We gave instructions before each study session to count either T junctions or the number of meanings of the word presented on the screen. All subjects successfully completed an off-line practice trial to ensure that they were able to follow the instructions. The accuracy of counting the T junctions during the study session was not significantly different between the 3 groups (F2,20 = 1.6; P = .22). During scanning, the subjects were asked to complete 3-letter word stems of words presented either once (low-recall condition) or 4 times (high-recall condition). The experiment consisted of 2 runs of each condition. The 2 baseline conditions bracketed the 2 pairs of low-recall/high-recall sessions, which were counterbalanced for order across subjects.
The PET facilities and procedures were identical to those previously described.29,38 Positron emission tomography data were acquired with a General Electric Scanditronix PC4096 15-slice whole-body tomographic scan (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wis). The slice geometry consists of contiguous slices with a center-to-center distance of 6.5 mm (axial field, 97.5 mm) and axial resolution of 6.0 mm full-width half maximum. The axial field of view of the PET camera in a single-bed position precluded total brain coverage. We determined head positioning to ensure maximal coverage of prefrontal areas and complete coverage of the hippocampus. Positron emission tomography images were reconstructed with a conventional convolution-backprojection algorithm, corrected for photon absorption, scatter, and dead-time effects. Subjects underwent six 1-minute scans and inhaled oxygen 15 carbon dioxide beginning 30 seconds after the initiation of the task. Subjects performed tasks while viewing a computer screen and responded verbally. Each scan was followed by a 10-minute washout period.
We analyzed the effects of group and condition on recall accuracy with a 2-way mixed factor analysis of variance (subject as random effect) with a grouping factor and with condition as a within-subjects variable. Where indicated by significant effects, we performed post hoc 2-tailed t tests.
Realignment of images and transformation into the standard stereotactic space of Talairach were performed as described previously.39 Images were smoothed with a 2-dimensional gaussian filter with a width of 15-mm full-width at half maximum. Statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) 96 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, England). Mixed models in SPM 96 require that data be collapsed so that each condition is represented as a single file. That was accomplished with the proportional scaling option in the random-effects kit. The data were then modeled with explanatory variables for group and condition. Main effects and interactions were assessed using t statistics subsequently transformed into z scores. Considering that a mixed-effects model is appropriate to study population differences and that we had strong localizing hypotheses, we used a threshold for parametric maps of uncorrected P<.001 (ie, z>3.09). For completeness, and to obviate bias, all activations corresponding to z >3.09 are shown. However, this threshold is only appropriate for those territories about which unidirectional a priori hypotheses were posed. Therefore, other loci are shown in italics to reflect their post hoc status. Since our Talairach transformation algorithm is compatible with SPM 95, we used SPM 95 for the creation of the glass brain projections.
The findings of our original study29 (ie, a lack of hippocampal recruitment but preserved recruitment of prefrontal areas during different modes of memory retrieval in patients with schizophrenia compared with control subjects) were extended to the larger patient sample of this study. Herein, we focus on the comparison between patients with schizophrenia with and without the deficit syndrome. We refer to the control sample only as a reference to illustrate the differences between the 2 schizophrenia subtypes and the normal pattern of memory retrieval.
The 2 schizophrenia groups did not differ in recall accuracy scores. Recall accuracy was significantly greater during high recall than during low recall in all 3 groups (controls, 0.76 [ie, 76% correctly recalled target words] and 0.28, P<.001; patients with nondeficit syndrome, 0.64 and 0.35, P = .007; patients with deficit syndrome, 0.62 and 0.34, P = .001) (main effect of condition, F1,20 = 255.5, P<.001). The increment in recall accuracy was significantly different between the 3 groups (diagnosis × condition interaction, F2,20 = 8.45; P = .006). This was caused by different recall accuracy for the control group compared with both schizophrenia groups during low recall (patients with nondeficit syndrome vs controls, P = .01; patients with deficit syndrome vs controls, P = .04) and during high recall (controls vs patients with nondeficit syndrome, P = .09; controls vs patients with deficit syndrome, P = .02).
First, mean rCBF values were analyzed for each condition (baseline, low recall, and high recall) separately and for all 3 conditions combined, and contrasted between groups to compare rCBF values during memory retrieval (main effect of group). Second, mean rCBF values contrasted between conditions were analyzed across all 3 groups to compare rCBF values during 2 different modes of memory retrieval (main effect of condition). Third, changes in rCBF values during 2 modes of memory retrieval—low-success retrieval (contrast: low recall − baseline) and high-success retrieval (contrast: high recall − low recall)—were contrasted to compare the recruitment of brain areas during memory retrieval between the groups (group × retrieval condition interaction).
Consistent with our a priori hypothesis, frontal cortex rCBF values (means averaged across all 3 conditions) differed between the 2 schizophrenia subtypes (Table 1) (Figure 1). Comparable differences were found in the parietal and temporal cortex. When compared with controls, frontal cortex rCBF was markedly reduced in patients with deficit syndrome and much less so in patients with nondeficit syndrome (Figure 1). The most significant difference between patients with deficit and nondeficit syndrome was found in the right prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 44/9), where the rCBF values for the patients with nondeficit syndrome were mainly in the normal range, whereas all patients with deficit syndrome showed lower rCBF values (Figure 2). The same pattern of widespread cortical differences between patients with deficit and nondeficit syndromes was found when each condition (baseline, low recall, and high recall) was tested separately for group effects. Compared with the patients with nondeficit syndrome, the patients with deficit syndrome did not show higher levels of rCBF in any cortical or subcortical region.
Main effect of retrieval condition
In all 3 groups combined, low-success retrieval was associated with the recruitment of the right (areas 11 and 47) and left (area 9) prefrontal cortex as well as the right thalamus and right parietal cortex (area 40) (Table 1). However, during high-success retrieval, the normal pattern of medial temporal lobe recruitment was not found (Table 1).
Group × retrieval condition interactions
For the contrast of low recall minus baseline, patients with nondeficit syndrome exhibited significantly greater recruitment of left prefrontal area 47 compared with patients with deficit syndrome (Table 2) (Figure 3). There were no areas of significantly greater recruitment in the patients with deficit syndrome.
For the contrast of high recall minus low recall, there were no significant differences in the frontal or medial temporal lobes between the 2 schizophrenia groups (Table 2). Compared with the control group, both schizophrenia samples failed to recruit the hippocampus (z = 3.29 for patients with nondeficit syndrome; z = 2.99 for patients with deficit syndrome) (Figure 4).
Our study provides evidence that the frontal cortex is differentially impaired in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia; frontal cortex activity during memory retrieval and left frontal cortex recruitment during retrieval attempt were significantly greater in patients with nondeficit syndrome. However, the medial temporal lobe is similarly impaired in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia; both schizophrenia groups did not differ in medial temporal lobe activity and failed to exhibit the normal pattern of hippocampal recruitment during memory retrieval.
Recent functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus are associated with distinct components of memory retrieval.40-42 The right frontal cortex is consistently activated during intentional declarative or episodic retrieval of words, faces, scenes, or objects.30,40,43-46 The degree of right frontal activation during intentional retrieval may reflect the degree of strategic monitoring of memory retrieval.40,47-49 Hippocampal recruitment is associated with encoding and subsequent successful retrieval of memory.30,31,41,50-52 The pattern of right prefrontal cortex and hippocampal activation in our control group29 is consistent with these previous studies of memory retrieval.
Our results add new information to the existing literature regarding frontal and temporal lobe function in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia has long been associated with frontal lobe pathologic features.53 More recently, the theory of hypofrontality in schizophrenia4 has been advanced, based mainly on findings of structural and functional neuroimaging studies. However, the concept of hypofrontality has not remained unchallenged.15,54 We found hypofrontality when comparing mean rCBF values across memory retrieval conditions; it was most pronounced in the patients with deficit syndrome. However, rCBF changes in prefrontal areas during memory retrieval were more variable. Furthermore, although the 2 schizophrenia groups differed in frontal cortex activity during memory retrieval, they did not differ in recall accuracy scores. This raises 2 questions: How does prefrontal cortex activity relate to task performance?55 and Does a lower rCBF starting point provide greater capacity for rCBF increases?15,56 To our knowledge, these issues have not yet been studied in schizophrenia.
Previous studies have reported differential impairment of cognitive function,57-59 brain structure,60 and brain function61 in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. Two previous studies of cerebral glucose metabolism at rest have reported differential involvement of frontal and temporal areas in deficit vs nondeficit schizophrenia. Tamminga et al61 reported cortical hypometabolism in patients with deficit syndrome only and hippocampal hypometabolism in patients with deficit syndrome and those with nondeficit syndrome. Gur et al62 reported no differences in temporal lobe metabolism between deficit and nondeficit groups, but found increased left temporal metabolism in patients with negative symptoms as well as those with Schneiderian delusions and hallucinations. Differences in imaging methods (rest vs activation, metabolism vs blood flow, region-of-interest analysis vs SPM) make it difficult to compare our results with those in these 2 previous studies. However, the prominent involvement of the medial temporal lobe in both deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia and the decrease of frontal activity primarily in patients with negative symptoms is a consistent theme in all 3 studies.
Several authors have proposed that temporal lobe dysfunction is associated with delusions and hallucinations in schizophrenia23,63-67 and in cases of organic psychosis.68 Furthermore, abnormal frontal-temporal connections are proposed to give rise to schizophrenia.69-73 Our study of memory retrieval provides evidence for differential dysfunction of the frontal-temporal neural network in patients with schizophrenia with and without the deficit syndrome.
We have to consider treatment with neuroleptic medication and small sample size as 2 limitations of our study. All patients with schizophrenia were stable outpatients, treated with typical neuroleptics; some were treated with anticholinergic drugs. This could have affected memory performance and rCBF. We decided to study stable patients given neuroleptic medication since drug discontinuation could have worsened hippocampal function and memory performance.74 Furthermore, there is no evidence that long-term exposure to typical neuroleptics changes blood flow patterns in schizophrenia in the temporal lobe or in the prefrontal cortex during cognitive activation.75,76 The findings from the direct comparison of patients with deficit syndrome and those with nondeficit syndrome are not confounded by age or duration of illness and are unlikely to be confounded by antipsychotic medication (various typical neuroleptics, similar chlorpromazine-equivalent doses). Although we studied small samples, we used a mixed-effects model in our analysis of the behavioral and PET data. This allows us to make inferences not only about the study samples, but also about the populations from which the samples were drawn.77-79
In summary, we found evidence for frontal and temporal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia. Deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia differ in the degree of frontal lobe dysfunction during memory retrieval but are similarly impaired in hippocampal recruitment.
Accepted for publication August 20, 1999.
This study was supported by a Dupont-Warren Fellowship from Harvard Medical School, Boston, Mass (Dr Heckers); by Staglin Music Festival Investigator Award (Dr Heckers) and a Young Investigator Award (Dr Rauch) from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, Great Neck, NY; and by grants K20MH01215 (Dr Rauch) and R01MH57915 (Dr Schacter), from the National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, Md.
We thank Dmitry Berdichevsky, MS, Zakhar Levin, MS, Avis Loring, RT, Sandra Barrow, BS, Steve Weise, BS, Ed Amico, MEd, and Dana Ruther, MA, for technical assistance.
Corresponding author: Stephan Heckers, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital-East, CNY-9132, Bldg 149, 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129 (e-mail: heckers@psych.mgh.harvard.edu).
1.Gur
RE Functional brain imaging studies in schizophrenia. Watson
SJed.
Psychopharmacology The Fourth Generation of Progress [book on CD-ROM] Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott-Raven Publishers1998;
Google Scholar 2.McClure
RJKeshavan
MSPettegrew
JW Chemical and physiologic brain imaging in schizophrenia.
Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1998;2193- 122
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Ingvar
DHFranzen
G Abnormalities of cerebral blood flow distribution in patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1974;50425- 462
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Weinberger
DRBerman
KFZec
RF Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, I: regional cerebral blood flow evidence.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43114- 124
Google ScholarCrossref 6.Berman
KFZec
RFWeinberger
DR Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, II: role of neuroleptic treatment, attention, and mental effort.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43126- 135
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Berman
KFIllowsky
BPWeinberger
DR Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, IV: further evidence for regional and behavioral specificity.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45616- 622
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Weinberger
DRBerman
KFIllowsky
BP Physiological dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, III: a new cohort and evidence for a monoaminergic mechanism.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45609- 615
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Rubin
PHolm
SFriberg
LVidebech
PAndersen
HSBendsen
BBStromso
NLarsen
JKLassen
NAHemmingsen
R Altered modulation of prefrontal and subcortical brain activity in newly diagnosed schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a regional cerebral blood flow study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48987- 995
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Berman
KFTorrey
EFDaniel
DGWeinberger
DR Regional cerebral blood flow in monozygotic twins discordant and concordant for schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49927- 934
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Andreasen
NCRezai
KAlliger
RSwayze
VW IIFlaum
MKirchner
PCohen
GO'Leary
DS Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia: assessment with xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of London.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49943- 958
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Catafau
AMParellada
ELomena
FJBernardo
MPavia
JRos
DSetoain
JGonzalez-Monclus
E Prefrontal and temporal blood flow in schizophrenia: resting and activation technetium-99m-HMPAO SPECT patterns in young neuroleptic-naive patients with acute disease.
J Nucl Med. 1994;35935- 941
Google Scholar 13.Parellada
ECatafau
AMBernardo
MLomena
FCatarineu
SGonzalez-Monclus
E The resting and activation issue of hypofrontality: a single photon emission computed tomography study in neuroleptic-naive and neuroleptic-free schizophrenic female patients.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44787- 790
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Stevens
AAGoldman-Rakic
PSGore
JCFulbright
RKWexler
BE Cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia during auditory word and tone working memory demonstrated by functional resonance imaging.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551097- 1103
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Frith
CDFriston
KJHerold
SSilbersweig
DFletcher
PCahill
CDolan
RJFrackowiak
RSLiddle
PF Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during the performance of a verbal fluency task.
Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167343- 349
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Fletcher
PCMcKenna
PJFrith
CDGrasby
PMFriston
KJDolan
RJ Brain activations in schizophrenia during a graded memory task studied with functional neuroimaging.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551001- 1008
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Manoach
DSPress
DZThangaraj
VSearl
MMGoff
DCHalpern
ESaper
CBWarach
S Schizophrenia subjects activate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task as measured by fMRI.
Biol Psychiatry. 1999;451128- 1137
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Volkow
NDWolf
APVan Gelder
PBrodie
JDOverall
JECancro
RGomez-Mont
F Phenomenological correlates of metabolic activity in 18 patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144151- 158
Google Scholar 19.Volkow
NDWolf
APBrodie
JDCancro
ROverall
JERhoades
HVan Gelder
P Brain interactions in chronic schizophrenics under resting and activation conditions.
Schizophr Res. 1988;147- 53
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Wiesel
FAWik
GSjogren
IBlomqvist
GGreitz
TStone-Elander
S Regional brain glucose metabolism in drug free schizophrenic patients and clinical correlates.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1987;76628- 641
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Wolkin
ASanfilipo
MWolf
APAngrist
BBrodie
JDRotrosen
J Negative symptoms and hypofrontality in chronic schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49959- 965
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Suzuki
MKurachi
MKawasaki
YKiba
KYamaguchi
N Left hypofrontality correlates with blunted affect in schizophrenia.
Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol. 1992;46653- 657
Google Scholar 23.Liddle
PFFriston
KJFrith
CDHirsch
SRJones
TFrackowiak
RS Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1992;160179- 186
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Ebmeier
KPBlackwood
DHMurray
CSouza
VWalker
MDougall
NMoffoot
APO'Carroll
REGoodwin
GM Single-photon emission computed tomography with 99mTc-exametazime in unmedicated schizophrenic patients.
Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33487- 495
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Friston
KJLiddle
PFFrith
CDHirsch
SRFrackowiak
RS The left medial temporal region and schizophrenia: a PET study.
Brain. 1992;115367- 382
Google ScholarCrossref 26.DeLisi
LEBuchsbaum
MSHolcomb
HHLangston
KCKing
ACKessler
RPickar
DCarpenter
WT
JrMorihisa
JMMargolin
RWeinberger
DR Increased temporal lobe glucose use in chronic schizophrenic patients.
Biol Psychiatry. 1989;25835- 851
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Carpenter
WT
JrHeinrichs
DWWagman
AM Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept.
Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145578- 583
Google Scholar 28.Carpenter
WT
JrBuchanan
RWKirkpatrick
BTamminga
CWood
F Strong inference, theory testing, and the neuroanatomy of schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50825- 831
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Heckers
SRauch
SLGoff
DSavage
CRSchacter
DLFischman
AJAlpert
NM Impaired recruitment of the hippocampus during conscious recollection in schizophrenia.
Nat Neurosci. 1998;1318- 323
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Schacter
DLAlpert
NMSavage
CRRauch
SLAlbert
MS Conscious recollection and the human hippocampal formation: evidence from positron emission tomography.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93321- 325
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Schacter
DLSavage
CRAlpert
NMRauch
SLAlbert
MS The role of hippocampus and frontal cortex in age-related memory changes: a PET study.
Neuroreport. 1996;71165- 1169
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Kirkpatrick
BBuchanan
RWMcKenney
PDAlphs
LDCarpenter
WT
Jr The Schedule for the Deficit Syndrome: an instrument for research in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 1989;30119- 123
Google ScholarCrossref 33.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
35.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBGibbon
MFirst
MB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1991;
36.Kay
SRFiszbein
AOpler
LA The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1987;13261- 276
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): conceptual and theoretical foundations.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl. November1989;49- 58
Google Scholar 38.Rauch
SLSavage
CRAlpert
NMMiguel
ECBaer
LBreiter
HCFischman
AJManzo
PAMoretti
CJenike
MA A positron emission tomographic study of simple phobic symptom provocation.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;5220- 28
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Alpert
NMBerdichevsky
DWeise
STang
JRauch
SL Stereotactic transformation of PET scans by nonlinear least squares. Uemura
KJones
Teds.
Quantification of Brain Function Tracer Kinetics and Image Analysis in Brain PET Amsterdam, the Netherlands Excerpta Medica1993;459- 463
Google Scholar 41.Fletcher
PCFrith
CDRugg
MD The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory.
Trends Neurosci. 1997;20213- 218
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Ungerleider
LG Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory.
Science. 1995;270769- 775
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Kapur
SCraik
FIJones
CBrown
GMHoule
STulving
E Functional role of the prefrontal cortex in retrieval of memories: a PET study.
Neuroreport. 1995;61880- 1884
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Nyberg
LTulving
EHabib
RNilsson
LGKapur
SHoule
SCabeza
RMcIntosh
AR Functional brain maps of retrieval mode and recovery of episodic inormation.
Neuroreport. 1995;7249- 252
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Rugg
MDFletcher
PCFrith
CDFrackowiak
RSDolan
RJ Differential activation of the prefrontal cortex in successful and unsuccessful memory retrieval.
Brain. 1996;1192073- 2083
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Wagner
ADDesmond
JEGlover
GHGabrieli
JD Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory: functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes.
Brain. 1998;121
(pt 10)
1985- 2002
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Fletcher
PCShallice
TFrith
CDFrackowiak
RSDolan
RJ The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory, II: retrieval.
Brain. 1998;1211249- 1256
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Buckner
RLKoutstaal
WSchacter
DLDale
AMRotte
MRosen
BR Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval, II: selective averaging of event-related fMRI trials to test the retrieval success hypothesis.
Neuroimage. 1998;7163- 175
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Rugg
MDFletcher
PCAllan
KFrith
CDFrackowiak
RSJDolan
RJ Neural correlates of memory retrieval during recognition memory and cued recall.
Neuroimage. 1998;8262- 273
Google ScholarCrossref 50.Squire
LROjemann
JGMiezin
FMPetersen
SEVideen
TORaichle
ME Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a functional anatomical study of memory.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;891837- 1841
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Schacter
DLWagner
AD Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval.
Hippocampus. 1999;97- 24
Google ScholarCrossref 52.Nyberg
LMcIntosh
ARHoule
SNilsson
L-GTulving
E Activation of medial temporal structures during episodic memory retrieval.
Nature. 1996;380715- 717
Google ScholarCrossref 53.Alzheimer
A Beiträge zur Pathologischen Anatomie der Dementia praecox.
Monatsschrift Psychiatrie Neurologie. 1897;282- 120
Google ScholarCrossref 55.Braver
TSCohen
JDNystrom
LEJonides
JSmith
EENoll
DC A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory.
Neuroimage. 1997;549- 62
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Kastrup
ALi
T-QKrueger
GGlover
GHMoseley
ME Relationship between cerebral blood flow changes during visual stimulation and baseline flow levels investigated with functional MRI.
Neuroreport. 1999;101751- 1756
Google ScholarCrossref 57.Buchanan
RWStrauss
MEBreier
AKirkpatrick
BCarpenter
WT
Jr Attentional impairments in deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154363- 370
Google Scholar 58.Bustillo
JRThaker
GBuchanan
RWMoran
MKirkpatrick
BCarpenter
WT Visual information processing impairments in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154647- 654
Google Scholar 59.Buchanan
RWStrauss
MEKirkpatrick
BHolstein
CBreier
ACarpenter
WT
Jr Neuropsychological impairments in deficit vs nondeficit forms of schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51804- 811
Google ScholarCrossref 60.Buchanan
RWBreier
AKirkpatrick
BElkashef
AMunson
RCGellad
FCarpenter
WT
Jr Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1993;15059- 65
Google Scholar 61.Tamminga
CAThaker
GKBuchanan
RKirkpatrick
BAlphs
LDChase
TNCarpenter
WT Limbic system abnormalities identified in schizophrenia using positron emission tomography with fluorodeoxyglucose and neocortical alterations with deficit syndrome.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49522- 530
Google ScholarCrossref 62.Gur
REMozley
PDResnick
SMMozley
LHShtasel
DLGallacher
FArnold
SEKarp
JSAlavi
AReivich
MGur
RC Resting cerebral glucose metabolism in first-episode and previously treated patients with schizophrenia relates to clinical features.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52657- 667
Google ScholarCrossref 63.Roberts
DR Schizophrenia and the brain.
J Neuropsychiatry. 1963;571- 79
Google Scholar 64.Luchins
DJ A possible role of hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenic symptomatology.
Biol Psychiatry. 1990;2887- 91
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Venables
PH Hippocampal function and schizophrenia: experimental psychological evidence.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992;658111- 127
Google ScholarCrossref 66.Krieckhaus
EEDonahoe
JWMorgan
MA Paranoid schizophrenia may be caused by dopamine hyperactivity of CA1 hippocampus.
Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31560- 570
Google ScholarCrossref 67.Bogerts
B The temporolimbic system theory of positive schizophrenic symptoms.
Schizophr Bull. 1997;23423- 435
Google ScholarCrossref 68.Hirono
NMori
EIshii
KKitagaki
HSasaki
MIkejiri
YImamura
TShimomura
TIkeda
MYamashita
H Alteration of regional cerebral glucose utilization with delusions in Alzheimer's disease.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10433- 439
Google Scholar 69.Hemsley
DR A simple (or simplistic?) cognitive model for schizophrenia.
Behav Res Ther. 1993;31633- 645
Google ScholarCrossref 70.Ruppin
EReggia
JAHorn
D Pathogenesis of schizophrenic delusions and hallucinations: a neural model.
Schizophr Bull. 1996;22105- 123
Google ScholarCrossref 71.Friston
KJFrith
CD Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome?
Clin Neurosci. 1995;389- 97
Google Scholar 72.Liddle
PF Brain imaging. Hirsch
SRWeinberger
DReds.
Schizophrenia Oxford, England Blackwell Science Ltd1995;425- 439
Google Scholar 73.Chen
EY A neural network model of cortical information processing in schizophrenia, II: role of hippocampal-cortical interaction: a review and a model.
Can J Psychiatry. 1995;4021- 26
Google Scholar 74.Gilbertson
MWvan Kammen
DP Recent and remote memory dissociation: medication effects and hippocampal function in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42585- 595
Google ScholarCrossref 75.Goldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Effects of neuroleptic medications on the cognition of patients with schizophrenia: a review of recent studies.
J Clin Psychiatry. 1996;57
(suppl 9)
62- 65
Google Scholar 76.Miller
DDRezai
KAlliger
RAndreasen
NC The effect of antipsychotic medication on relative cerebral blood perfusion in schizophrenia: assessment with technetium-99m hexamethyl-propyleneamine oxime single photon emission computed tomography.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41550- 559
Google ScholarCrossref 77.Kutner
MHNachtsheim
CJWasserman
WNeter
J Applied Linear Statistical Models. 4th ed. Homewood, Ill Richard D Irwin1996;
79.Holmes
APFriston
KJ Generalisability, random effects, and population inference.
Neuroimage. 1998;7S754
Google Scholar