Background
Increasing plasma glucose levels improves memory in patients with Alzheimer disease (AD). Increasing plasma glucose levels also increases endogenous insulin levels, raising the question of whether memory improvement is due to changes in insulin, independent of hyperglycemia. We address this question by examining memory and counterregulatory hormone response during hyperglycemia when endogenous insulin was suppressed by concomitant infusion of the somatostatin analogue octreotide (Sandostatin).
Methods
Twenty-three patients with AD and 14 similarly aged healthy adults participated in 4 metabolic conditions on separate days: (1) hyperinsulinemia (538 pmol/L) with fasting glucose (5.6 mmol/L [100 mg/dL]), achieved by insulin and variable dextrose infusion; (2) hyperglycemia (12.5 mmol/L [225 mg/dL]) with fasting insulin (57 pmol/L), achieved by dextrose and somatostatin (octreotide) infusion (150 mg/h); (3) placebo with isotonic sodium chloride solution (saline) infusion (fasting insulin and glucose); and (4) an active control condition in which somatostatin alone was infused (150 mg/h). Declarative memory (story recall) and selective attention (Stroop interference test) were measured during steady metabolic states.
Results
Patients with AD showed improved memory during hyperinsulinemia relative to placebo (P = .05) and relative to hyperglycemia (P<.005). Memory did not improve during hyperglycemia when insulin was suppressed. Somatostatin analogue infusion alone also improved memory for patients with AD (P<.05). Hyperinsulinemia increased cortisol levels in subjects with AD, whereas somatostatin alone lowered cortisol concentrations.
Conclusions
These results confirm that elevated insulin without hyperglycemia enhances memory in adults with AD, and indicate that insulin is essential for hyperglycemic memory facilitation. These results also suggest a potential therapeutic role for somatostatin in AD.
ALZHEIMER disease (AD) is accompanied by disrupted glucose metabolism and insulin resistance that may contribute to its characteristic memory impairment.1-3 Increasing glucose availability by raising plasma glucose levels improves memory for new information in patients with AD.2,4-6 Such memory facilitation is associated with a glucose-stimulated elevation in endogenous insulin levels, raising the question of whether improvement is due to changes in insulin, independent of hyperglycemia. In previous work, we have demonstrated that raising insulin levels while keeping plasma glucose at fasting baseline levels was sufficient to improve memory for patients with AD.2 However, the question remains whether raising plasma glucose levels alone without an accompanying elevation in insulin is also sufficient to enhance memory. This question is of considerable theoretical import. Although numerous studies have demonstrated that raising plasma glucose to an optimal level improves memory for healthy adults and patients with AD,7 insulin effects on cognitive abilities such as memory have only been recently demonstrated.2 Indeed, the question of whether and how the brain is affected by insulin is controversial. A potential role for insulin in the brain is supported by documentation of dense insulin receptor distributions in several brain regions, including the CA1, CA3, and dentate gyrus regions of the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, hypothalamus, and olfactory bulb.8,9 Interestingly, these regions are also affected earliest and most severely in patients with AD.
The finding that hyperglycemic memory facilitation is abolished when insulin is suppressed would suggest that previous observations of enhanced memory with glucose administration are not due to glucose per se, but instead due to changes in endogenous insulin levels in response to elevated glucose. This finding would also support previous studies suggesting that defects in insulin action may contribute to the pathophysiology of AD, and to its characteristic memory impairment.2,10 One method of suppressing endogenous insulin during hyperglycemia is by administering the neuropeptide somatostatin, which inhibits insulin release from pancreatic beta cells.11 Somatostatin levels and receptor distributions are consistently noted to be reduced in AD,12-14 although the significance of this observation has not been established.
The present study measured memory and attention in patients with AD and healthy adults in 4 conditions: (1) when plasma insulin was raised to levels that previously facilitated performance while glucose was maintained at a fasting level; (2) when plasma glucose was raised to levels that previously facilitated memory while keeping insulin at a fasting level by concomitant administration of somatostatin; (3) when fasting levels of insulin and glucose were maintained with isotonic sodium chloride solution (saline) infusion; and (4) an active control condition, in which somatostatin was administered alone while glucose and insulin remained at fasting levels. The inclusion of this condition was necessary to determine whether the failure to observe memory facilitation during hyperglycemia was due to an inhibitory effect of somatostatin, rather than the absence of insulin. In addition, in light of the well-documented somatostatinergic deficiency in AD, it is possible that somatostatin might exhibit independent effects on memory. Given previous findings that insulin-induced memory improvement was accompanied by changes in neuroendocrine counterregulation, plasma cortisol, corticotropin, epinephrine, and norepinephrine levels were measured during each condition.2 Finally, we included a comparison group of similarly aged healthy adults to determine whether their memory and counterregulatory responses differed from patients with AD, an observation that might have pathogenetic implications.
This study was approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the University of Washington, Seattle. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects and from the legal representatives of the patients with AD. Subjects were 23 patients with AD and14 healthy adults recruited from the Clinical Core of the University of Washington Alzheimer's Disease Research Center. All subjects received $100 for their participation in the study. Subjects with AD and healthy subjects did not differ with respect to age, education, body mass index, or fasting plasma glucose level (Table 1). All subjects were in good physical health with stable weights for at least 2 months. Subjects were screened by medical history, physical examination, and by evaluation of serum electrolytes, glucose, renal, thyroid and hepatic function, and complete blood cell count. Subjects were free from past or present psychiatric disorders, alcoholism, head trauma, uncontrolled hypertension, neurologic disorders other than AD, diabetes mellitus, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, congestive heart failure, or cardiac arrhythmias. No subjects were taking medication with cognition-enhancing or other known central nervous system effects, or that affected glucose regulation. The AD subjects met National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria for probable AD as determined by board-certified neurologists and psychiatrists in the Clinical Core,15 were community-dwelling, and at mild-to-moderate stages of the disease. Dementia Rating Scale16 scores and other clinical data are presented in Table 1.
All healthy subjects and 19 of 23 AD subjects participated in 3 metabolic conditions on separate days at least 1 week apart but not more that 6 weeks apart: (1) Hyperinsulinemia with basal (fasting) plasma glucose. As described by Craft et al,2 subjects were maintained at plasma insulin levels of about 538 pmol/L using an insulin infusion dose of 1.0 mU · kg−1 · min−1. Dextrose (20%) was infused as needed to keep plasma glucose at levels of about 5.6 mmol/L (100 mg/dL). (2) Hyperglycemia with basal (fasting) plasma insulin. Dextrose was infused to reach and maintain glucose plasma levels of 12.5 mmol/L (225 mg/dL) while a somatostatin analogue (octreotide; Sandostatin, Sandoz Pharmaceuticals, Hanover, NJ) was infused at a rate of 150 mg/h to suppress the endogenous insulin response. (3) Placebo. Both plasma glucose and insulin were maintained at fasting levels (insulin levels of about 57 pmol/L and glucose levels of about 5.6 mmol/L [100 mg/dL]) with saline infusion. A subset of subjects also participated in a fourth, active control condition: (4) Somatostatin with basal glucose and insulin. Octreotide was infused at a rate of 150 mg/h while both plasma glucose and insulin were maintained at fasting baseline levels. Six healthy subjects and 12 AD subjects participated in the fourth condition; 8 of the AD subjects also participated in the first 3 conditions, whereas 4 AD subjects only participated in the placebo and somatostatin conditions owing to time constraints. Order of all conditions was counterbalanced, and subjects and testers were blinded to condition. Plasma glucose and insulin levels were similar between AD and healthy groups in all conditions (Table 2 and Table 3). Insulin values in the hyperglycemic and somatostatin-alone condition did not differ statistically from the placebo condition. Glucose values did not differ between placebo, hyperinsulinemic, and somatostatin-alone conditions.
Subjects rested with intravenous lines in place for a 30-minute habituation period. Dextrose, insulin, somatostatin, or saline were then infused until target levels were reached (approximately 90 minutes), after which a 30-minute stabilization period occurred. Following this stabilization period, a 30-minute cognitive protocol was administered, and 3 blood samples were obtained through an indwelling catheter at 0, 15, and 30 minutes during the protocol. Plasma glucose was measured at 5- to 10-minute intervals throughout each session with a glucose oxidase method (Beckman Glucose Analyzer, Fullerton, Calif). Plasma insulin, cortisol, corticotropin, epinephrine, and norepinephrine were measured with radioenzymatic assays or radioimmunoassays.2 Values for the 3 samples obtained during each cognitive protocol were averaged to ensure a reliable measure.
Four comparable versions of the cognitive protocol were assigned in counterbalanced order to the 4 metabolic conditions. Each protocol included a declarative memory and a selective attention measure. The latter measure was included to determine whether metabolic effects were limited to memory. Story recall was selected as the primary memory measure because it is a sensitive indicator of memory loss in early AD,17 and has been widely used in previous studies of hyperglycemic memory facilitation.2,4,5 Subjects heard a brief narrative containing 44 informational bits, and were asked to recall as much as possible both immediately and after a 10-minute delay. Subjects received credit for each informational bit recalled verbatim or for accurate paraphrases. Immediate and delayed scores were summed.
The Stroop Color-Word Interference selective attention task had 3 conditions.18 In the first 2 conditions, subjects were asked to read color words, and to name colors. In the interference condition, subjects were required to name the ink color of color names printed in discordant colors (eg, the word "red" printed in blue). Subjects were thus required to selectively attend to the color of the word and inhibit the prepotent reading response. Completion time and number of errors were recorded.
For AD and healthy groups, cognitive scores were log transformed and subjected to repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with condition (placebo, hyperinsulinemia, hyperglycemia) as the within-subjects factor. Effects with P<.05 were considered statistically significant, and all significance tests were 2 tailed. Following a significant F for condition, planned contrasts were conducted comparing performance in the hyperinsulinemic condition and in the hyperglycemic condition to performance in the placebo condition. Repeated-measures ANOVA was also used to compare cognitive scores in the placebo and somatostatin conditions for subjects who participated in these conditions. Repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted with hormone values, with Bonferroni post hoc tests used to determine the direction of significant effects.
Memory performance was greater in the hyperinsulinemic condition than in the placebo or the hyperglycemic condition for the AD group, whereas no difference in memory performance was observed between the placebo and hyperglycemic conditions (Figure 1). The repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant overall effect of condition (F2,17 = 5.79, P = .02). Planned contrasts documented better performance during hyperinsulinemia relative to placebo (F1,18 = 4.70, P = .05) and relative to hyperglycemia (F1,18 = 10.26, P<.005). No differences were observed between hyperglycemic and placebo conditions (F1,18 = 0.74, P = .42). Hyperinsulinemic enhancement was selective for story recall; no effect of condition was observed for the Stroop task (F2,15 = 1.69, P = .22).
Interestingly, subjects with AD showed a significant degree of memory improvement in story recall in the somatostatin condition relative to the placebo condition (F1,11 = 4.97, P = .05) (Figure 2). Effect sizes calculated according to Cohen19 for comparisons between placebo and hyperinsulinemic conditions, and placebo and somatostatin conditions, were moderate to large (effect size = 0.27 and 0.45, respectively). No change was observed in performance on the memory or selective attention test for healthy subjects in any condition (Table 4), which may reflect a different dose-response curve for healthy adults than for AD patients.
Neuroendocrine data are presented in Table 2 and Table 3. The AD group had higher cortisol levels than the healthy group in each of the 3 conditions (F1,16 = 18.78, P<.001; F1,16 = 9.51, P<.008; and F1,16 = 5.22, P = .04, for the placebo, hyperinsulinemic, and hyperglycemic conditions, respectively). The AD group had higher cortisol levels in the hyperinsulinemic condition relative to the placebo condition (F1,16 = 8.42, P = .02). In contrast, no differences were observed between AD and healthy groups for corticotropin. Healthy subjects had lower corticotropin values in the hyperglycemic condition relative to the placebo condition (F1,6 = 18.82, P<.005). Both AD and healthy subjects had significantly lower cortisol levels during somatostatin infusion relative to saline infusion, resulting in a significant effect for condition (F2,26 = 9.49, P<.001). A similar lowering of corticotropin levels was observed for all subjects during somatostatin infusion (F1,9 = 28.84, P<.001).
No differences were observed between AD and healthy groups for epinephrine or norepinephrine in any of the 3 conditions. For AD and healthy groups, epinephrine levels were reduced in the hyperglycemic condition relative to the placebo condition (F1,16 = 6.24, P = .03 and F1,13 = 34.69, P<.001 for the AD and healthy groups, respectively), whereas no differences were observed between the placebo and hyperinsulinemic conditions. The healthy group had higher norepinephrine values in the hyperinsulinemic condition than in the placebo condition (F1,13 = 4.86, P<.05). No changes were observed in norepinephrine levels from placebo to somatostatin conditions for AD or healthy groups. Epinephrine levels decreased during somatostatin infusion relative to placebo infusion for both groups (F1,14 = 15.98, P<.001).
Declarative memory was selectively improved during hyperinsulinemia for adults with AD. In contrast, memory was unchanged in the hyperglycemic condition, in which plasma glucose was raised to levels that were associated with memory enhancement in 4 prior studies.2,4-6 Unlike these previous investigations, however, in the present study the endogenous insulin response to hyperglycemia was suppressed. The failure to find memory facilitation in the hyperglycemic condition cannot be attributed to inhibitory effects of somatostatin, the neuropeptide used to suppress insulin in this condition. On the contrary, somatostatin infusion during euglycemia resulted in significant memory facilitation for patients with AD. Taken together, these results suggest that insulin and insulin-associated mechanisms are essential for hyperglycemic memory enhancement in patients with AD. Moreover, previously reported glucose-induced memory improvement for patients with AD is likely secondary to elevations in endogenous insulin in response to hyperglycemia.
The present results suggest that insulin affects the medial temporal declarative memory system, although the precise mechanisms through which such effects occur must remain speculative. Dense insulin receptor distributions have been documented in the dentate gyrus, CA1, and CA3 fields of the hippocampus.8,9,20 These regions are known to play a role in declarative memory and are among the regions affected earliest and most severely by the neuropathologic changes of AD.21 Raising plasma insulin levels results in increased insulin binding in hippocampus, demonstrating that changes in peripheral insulin levels can affect the brain.20 In turn, increasing brain insulin levels results in increased glucose utilization in the entorhinal cortex.22 Recent observations that the insulin-sensitive glucose transporter GLUT4 is present in the hippocampus provide a mechanism for direct insulin effects on brain glucose metabolism,23 and argue against the traditional notion that the brain is not an insulin-sensitive organ. Insulin-promoted increases in glucose utilization also result in glycolytic production of acetyl-CoA and corresponding increases in acetylcholine,24 a neurotransmitter closely linked to memory function and severely reduced in AD.
In the present study, the somatostatin analogue octreotide facilitated memory at basal glucose levels for patients with AD. Somatostatin is a neuropeptide with 5 known receptor types (somatostatin receptor types [SSRT] 1-5) in human brain.25 In particular, SSRT2 receptors, the primary target of the somatostatin analogue octreotide used in the present study,26 have been observed in the pyramidal layer of CA1, the granule cell layer of the dentate gyrus, the subiculum, and entorhinal cortex, as well as in frontal lobe.25 Recent studies have demonstrated that SSRT2 receptors are selectively reduced in frontal cortex and hippocampus of patients with AD.12,14 Administration of octreotide improves memory and increases hippocampal cholinergic activity in rodents, and enhances long-term potentiation.27-30 Such findings represent a potential basis for the memory enhancement observed in patients with AD following octreotide administration. However, evidence suggests that octreotide does not readily cross the blood-brain barrier.31 Thus, further work is needed to determine whether the memory enhancement observed following intravenous somatostatin administration in the present study reflects indirect effects of somatostatin, or compromised blood-brain barrier integrity in AD. Further study is also needed to identify the mechanisms through which hyperglycemia abolished the facilitatory effects of somatostatin. Similarly, the relationship between the memory-facilitating mechanisms induced by somatostatin and insulin is unclear. As intended, somatostatin administration suppressed plasma insulin, and thus memory facilitation in this condition must have occurred by an insulin-independent mechanism.
Two previous reports32,33 have investigated the therapeutic efficacy of somatostatin analogues in AD, both of which reported negative findings. The use of different somatostatin analogues, small sample sizes, lower doses, and diversified administration protocols may account for the different pattern of results. Based on the negative findings of their study, Mouradian et al33 concluded that "stimulation of the somatostatin system has no value in the symptomatic treatment of Alzheimer dementia."33 The present results contradict that conclusion, and suggest that further investigation of octreotide and other SSRT2-selective somatostatin analogues as potential therapeutic agents in AD is warranted.
In the present study, insulin and somatostatin administration were associated with changes in cortisol levels. The AD group had increased cortisol levels during hyperinsulinemia, the condition in which memory improvement was observed. This observation is somewhat puzzling, because several investigators have demonstrated that increased cortisol is associated with memory decline.34,35 In contrast, hyperinsulinemia was not associated with increased corticotropin levels in the AD or healthy group. The finding that patients with AD have higher cortisol levels than healthy adults despite similar corticotropin levels has been interpreted as indicating greater responsitivity to corticotropin in AD.36 Consistent with previous investigations, somatostatin administration was associated with reduced cortisol, corticotropin, and epinephrine levels for both healthy and AD groups.37-39 The cause and significance of such effects are not clear at this time.
In summary, defects in insulin-mediated energy metabolism have been linked to the pathophysiology and memory impairment of AD. The present results provide additional support for this relationship by demonstrating that insulin is essential for memory enhancement previously thought to result primarily from increasing glucose availability. The present study also demonstrates a potential therapeutic role for somatostatin in AD. Determining the precise mechanisms through which insulin and somatostatin facilitate memory is of critical importance, as this knowledge may identify pathogenetic factors in AD, as well provide new insight into insulin's role in normal memory function.
Accepted for publication August 17, 1999.
This study was supported by grant RO1-AG-10880 from the National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, Md, by grant IIRG 95-1151 from the Alzheimer's Association, Chicago, Ill, and by the Department of Veterans Affairs, Washington, DC.
We would like to thank Lyndel Cubberley, RN, Elizabeth Hunter, Laura Gibson, Maria Gonzales, Colby Wait, Elizabeth Colasurdo, Shannon Boldt, and Robert Beckham for their excellent technical assistance.
Reprints: Suzanne Craft, PhD, Geriatric Research, Education, and Clinical Center 182B, Veterans Affairs Puget Sound Health Care System, 1660 S Columbian Way, Seattle, WA 98108 (e-mail: scraft@u.washington.edu).
1.Blass
JPSheu
KFPiacentini
SSorbi
S Inherent abnormalities in oxidative metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: interaction with vascular abnormalities.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997;826382- 385
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Craft
SNewcomer
JKanne
SDagogo-Jack
SCryer
PESheline
YLuby
JDagogo-Jack
AAlderson
A Memory improvement following induced hyperinsulinemia in dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Neurobiol Aging. 1996;17123- 130
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Meneilly
GHill
A Alterations in glucose metabolism in patients with Alzheimer's disease.
J Am Geriatr Soc. 1993;41710- 714
Google Scholar 4.Craft
SDagogo-Jack
SWiethop
BMurphy
CNevins
RFleischman
SRice
VNewcomer
JCryer
PE The effects of hyperglycemia on memory and hormone levels in mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type: a longitudinal study.
Behav Neurosci. 1993;107926- 940
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Craft
SZallen
GBaker
LD Glucose and memory in mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1992;14253- 267
Google ScholarCrossref 6.Manning
CRagozzino
MGold
PE Glucose enhancement of memory in patients with probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type.
Neurobiol Aging. 1993;14523- 528
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Korol
DLGold
PE Glucose, memory, and aging.
Am J Clin Nutr. 1998;67
(suppl)
764S- 771S
Google Scholar 8.Adamo
MRaizada
MKLeRoith
D Insulin and insulin-like growth factor receptors in the nervous system.
Mol Neurobiol. 1989;371- 100
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Unger
JMcNeill
THMoxley
RT
IIIWhite
MMoss
ALivingston
JN Distribution of insulin receptor-like immunoreactivity in the rat forebrain.
Neuroscience. 1989;31143- 157
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Craft
SPeskind
ESchwartz
MWSchellenberg
GDRaskind
MPorte
D
Jr Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma insulin levels in Alzheimer's disease: relationship to severity of dementia and apolipoprotein E genotype.
Neurology. 1998;50164- 168
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Cryer
PE Glucose homeostasis and hypoglycemia. Wilson
JDFoster
DWeds.
Textbook of Endocrinology. Philadelphia, Pa8th ed. WB Saunders Co1992;1223- 1253
Google Scholar 12.Beal
MFMazurek
MFTran
VTChattha
GBird
EDMartin
MB Reduced numbers of somatostatin receptors in the cerebral cortex in Alzheimer's disease.
Science. 1985;229289- 291
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Davies
PKatzman
RTerry
RD Reduced somatostatin-like immunoreactivity in cerebral cortex from cases of Alzheimer disease and Alzheimer senile dementia.
Nature. 1980;228279- 280
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Krantic
SRobitaille
YQuirion
R Deficits in the somatostatin SS1 receptor sub-type in frontal and temporal cortices in Alzheimer's disease.
Brain Res. 1992;573299- 304
Google ScholarCrossref 15.McKhann
GDrachman
DFolstein
M Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease.
Neurology. 1984;34939- 944
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Mattis
S Dementia Rating Scale. Tampa, Fla Psychological Assessment Resources1988;
17.Storandt
MHill
RD Very mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type, II: psychometric test performance.
Arch Neurol. 1989;46383- 386
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Perret
E The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior.
Neuropsychologia. 1974;12323- 330
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Cohen
JC Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1983;
20.Marfaing
PPenicaud
LBroer
YMraovitch
SCalando
YPicon
L Effects of hyperinsulinemia on local cerebral insulin binding and glucose utilization in normoglycemic awake rats.
Neurosci Lett. 1990;115279- 285
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Squire
LR Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis of findings with rats, monkeys, and humans.
Psychol Rev. 1992;99195- 231
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Hoyer
SHenneberg
NKnapp
SLannert
HMartin
E Brain glucose metabolism is controlled by amplification and desensitization of the neuronal insulin receptor.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996;777374- 379
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Leloup
CArluison
MKassis
NLepetit
NKCartier
NFerre
PPenicaud
L Discrete brain areas express the insulin-responsive glucose transporter GLUT4.
Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1996;3845- 53
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Messier
CDestrade
C Insulin attentuates scopolamine-induced memory deficits.
Psychobiology. 1994;2216- 21
Google Scholar 25.Thoss
VSPerez
JProbst
A Expression of five somatostatin receptor mRNAs in the human brain and pituitary.
Archeol Pharmacol. 1996;354411- 419
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Araujo
DMLapchak
PACollier
BQuirion
R Evidence that somatostatin enhances endogenous acetylcholine release in the rat hippocampus.
J Neurochem. 1990;551546- 1555
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Mancillas
JRSiggins
GRBloom
FE Somatostatin selectively enhances acetylcholine-induced excitations in rat hippocampus and cortex.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;837518- 7521
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Matsuoka
NKanedo
SSatoh
M Somatostatin augments long-term potentiation of the mossy fiber-CA3 system in guinea-pig hippocampal slices.
Brain Res. 1991;553188- 194
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Matsuoka
NMaeda
NYamaguchi
ISatoh
M Possible involvement of brain somatostatin in the memory formation of rats and the cognitive enhancing action of FR121196 in passive avoidance task.
Brain Res. 1994;64211- 19
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Jaehde
UMasereeu
RDe Boer
AGFricker
GNagelkerke
JFVonderscher
JBreimer
DD Quantification and visualization of the transport of octreotide, a somatostatin analogue, across monolayers of cerebrovascular endothelial cells.
Pharm Res. 1994;11442- 448
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Cutler
NRHaxby
JVNarang
PKMay
CBurg
C Evaluation of an analogue of somatostatin in Alzheimer's disease.
N Engl J Med. 1985;312725
Google Scholar 33.Mouradian
MMBlin
JGiuffra
MHeuser
IJEBaronti
FOwnby
JChase
T Somatostatin replacement therapy for Alzheimer dementia.
Ann Neurol. 1991;30610- 613
Google ScholarCrossref 34.de Leon
MJMcRae
TRusinek
HConvit
ADe-Santi
STarshish
CGolomb
JVolkow
NDaisley
KOrentreich
NMcEwen
B Cortisol reduces hippocampal glucose metabolism in normal elderly, but not in Alzheimer's disease.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;823251- 3259
Google Scholar 35.Lupien
SJde Leon
Mde Santi
SConvit
ATarshish
CNair
NPVThakur
MMcEwen
BSHauger
RLMeaney
MJ Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits.
Nat Neurosci. 1998;169- 73
Google ScholarCrossref 36.Peskind
ERRaskind
MAWingerson
DPascualy
MThal
LJDobie
DJWilkinson
CW Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis responses to physostigmine: effects of Alzheimer's disease and gender.
Biol Psychiatry. 1996;4061- 68
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Brown
MRRivier
CVale
W Central nervous system regulation of adrenocorticotropin secretion: role of somatostatins.
Endocrinology. 1984;1141546- 1549
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Fehm
HLVoigt
KHLang
RBeinert
KERaptis
SPfeiffer
EF Somatostatin: a potent inhibitor of ACTH-hypersecretion in adrenal insufficiency.
Klin Wochenschr. 1976;54173- 176
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Petraglia
FFacchinetti
FD'Ambrogio
GVolpe
AGenazzani
AR Somatostatin and oxytocin infusion inhibits the rise of plasma beta-endorphin and beta-lipotropin and cortisol induced by insulin hypoglycaemia.
Clin Endocrinol. 1986;24609- 616
Google ScholarCrossref