Background
Schizophrenia is thought to arise from the interaction of genetically mediated and environmentally triggered abnormalities in brain function. Reduced frontal activation, reported in schizophrenic patients, may be one expression of genetic risk. The present study investigated whether frontal activation in relatives of schizophrenic patients would be related to eye tracking deficits (ETD), which are considered a behavioral marker of risk for schizophrenia.
Methods
Subjects were first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients (n = 17) and controls (n = 11). Relatives were divided into those with normal and abnormal pursuit based on qualitative ratings. Subjects were scanned using positron emission tomography and the H215O bolus subtraction technique while performing smooth pursuit and fixation. Brain areas more active in pursuit than fixation were identified in the 3 groups. Correlations were used to investigate the relationship between activation of pursuit regions and pursuit gain in the relatives.
Results
Controls significantly activated frontal eye fields (FEFs) and posterior areas, including the motion processing area, V5, and cuneus. The 2 groups of relatives activated the same posterior regions as controls, but differed from each other in activation of FEFs. Relatives with normal tracking activated right dorsal FEFs while relatives with ETD did not. Individual subtractions revealed that 90% of controls and 100% of the relatives with normal tracking activated FEFs during pursuit compared with 42% of relatives with ETD (P = .009). Pursuit gain was significantly and selectively associated with percent activation of right dorsal FEFs (r = 0.74).
Conclusions
Subtle frontal dysfunction seems to be a pathophysiological substrate of ETD in relatives of schizophrenic patients, and may be one aspect of genetically mediated differences in brain function relevant to schizophrenia.
NEURODEVELOPMENTAL models of schizophrenia have postulated both a genetic and an environmental contribution to the development of the disorder. First-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients are at increased risk of developing schizophrenia, and can transmit the risk without having expressed the clinical syndrome.1 Several deficits in cognitive and motor tasks found in schizophrenic patients have been identified as well in healthy first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Because they occur at elevated rates in the schizophrenic patients and their relatives but not in the relatives of patients with other psychiatric disorders, they are thought to represent behavioral markers of genetic risk (otherwise conceived of as alternative phenotypic expressions of the disease).2,3 Abnormalities in smooth pursuit eye tracking are one of the best-documented behavioral markers. Between 50% and 80% of schizophrenic patients have eye tracking deficits (ETD), compared with about 8% of the general population.4 Populations at high risk for schizophrenia, including first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients and individuals with schizophrenia spectrum personality disorders, also have elevated rates of ETD4-7; first-degree relatives of patients with other psychotic disorders do not.4
Many schizophrenic patients show a pattern of reduced frontal activation during frontal tasks.8,9 Levin10 advanced the hypothesis that pursuit deficits in schizophrenia were related to abnormal function of the frontal eye fields (FEFs), a frontal region involved in smooth pursuit. Many authors have tested this hypothesis indirectly by evaluating the relationship between pursuit quality and performance on tasks thought to tap FEFs or adjacent frontal areas.11-14 Results generally support an association between measures of frontal integrity and smooth pursuit.13,15-18 The findings of a recent neuroimaging study are consistent with the hypothesis that ETD is related to reduced activity in the FEFs in schizophrenic patients.19
An alternative hypothesis is that abnormal smooth pursuit reflects an abnormality in motion processing, mediated by a region in the temporal lobe called V5.20 Recently, several studies have reported that schizophrenic patients have impaired motion perception, and that this is also found in the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients.21-23 Nonetheless, the data on a role for V5 in ETD are conflicting. Studies of saccades to moving targets in schizophrenia have generally reported intact motion perception.12,13,24 One study of smooth pursuit found greater performance deficits when subjects had to predict the target movement (thought to depend on FEF function) than when subjects could use target velocity on the retina (thought to depend on V5 function).25
To our knowledge, no study has used positron emission tomography (PET) to investigate the neural basis of pursuit deficits in populations at risk for schizophrenia. In the current article, we used PET and the H215O bolus subtraction technique to investigate the relationship between individual differences in pursuit quality and regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in healthy first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients. Our specific hypothesis was that subjects with ETD would have reduced activation of FEFs during smooth pursuit.
Thirty subjects, 17 first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients and 13 controls who were similar in age, sex, IQ, and father's socioeconomic status, participated in the study (Table 1). Relatives were recruited from family studies of schizophrenia at the Douglas Hospital, Verdun, Quebec, and by advertisement in local newspapers and family support newsletters. They met the following inclusion/exclusion criteria: between age 18 and 50 years, normal or corrected-to-normal vision, right-handed,27 no history of Axis I disorder ascertained in a semistructured interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders [SCID-IV]),28 no personal history of neurological disorder, estimated full-scale IQ above 80,29 and medication free. Relatives were required to have a first-degree relative who met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia, as determined by an interview with the proband28 or by review of the proband's inpatient medical records. The control subjects were recruited by advertisement and met the same inclusion/exclusion criteria, except that they were required to have no first-degree relative with a psychotic disorder and to have normal pursuit (see below). Of the 13 consecutively recruited controls, 1 (8%) was excluded for ETD. All subjects passed a physical examination, had no medical contraindications for a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan, and had not previously participated in a PET study. Subjects did not eat or smoke for 3 hours prior to the PET scans, and had abstained from alcohol for a minimum of 24 hours. They underwent a urine toxicology screen sensitive to drugs of abuse (Triage Panel for Drugs of Abuse; Biosite Diagnostics, San Diego, Calif) immediately prior to scanning. Each subject's MRI scan was read blindly by a neuroradiologist; subjects were excluded if the MRI was clinically abnormal. One subject, a control, was excluded for this reason.
The 3 groups did not differ significantly in demographic variables, and no demographic variable was significantly associated with pursuit performance (all P>.10) (Table 1).
The study was approved by the institutional review board at McGill University Medical School, Montreal, Quebec. Subjects gave written informed consent and were compensated for their participation.
A high-speed (250-Hz) infrared video-based pupil tracker (Eyelink; SR Research, Mississauga, Ontario) recorded from the subject's dominant eye (the right for all but 1 control and 1 relative). This system has a spatial resolution of about 0.25° of visual angle. Subjects were asked to track a target that was either moving (pursuit) or immobile (fixation).
The target was a square subtending one-half degree of visual angle. It moved horizontally at 0.4 Hz across 24° of visual angle, and had a sinusoidal velocity profile. At the center of the square, an X changed to an O and back at random intervals. Subjects indicated each change by pressing a button. Monitoring tasks such as this one improve pursuit, and do so equally in normal controls and schizophrenic patients.30 Subjects performed 24 cycles of pursuit in each scan.
The same target remained at the center of the screen for the duration of the scan. This task was essentially pursuit at velocity zero: as with the pursuit task, the subjects kept their eyes on the target and indicated target changes with a button press.
Each subject underwent a high-resolution MRI scan obtained with a 1.5-T Philips scanner (Philips, Eindhoven, the Netherlands) with 160 slices 1-mm thick. Each MRI was transformed using a 3-dimensional image cross-correlation algorithm31 where the MRI is resampled by a linear transform to match the target volume (a database of 305 MRIs transformed to Talairach space through identification of neuroanatomical landmarks).32
Positron Emission Tomography
Subjects were positioned in a head scanner (Scanditronix 2048; Scanditronix, Uppsala, Sweden) that has an intrinsic resolution of 5 × 5 × 6 mm.33 Laser beams were aligned at the orbitomeatal line and the subject's head was stabilized using a molded foam support. An intravenous line was placed in the subject's left arm. The video camera of the pupil tracker was mounted on the head-holder. The tracker controlled data acquisition and was linked to a computer controlling stimulus display.
A 17-in monitor was positioned 57 cm from the subject's eyes. A 10-minute transmission scan was obtained using a rotating rod of 68Ge for attenuation correction. Subsequently, the PET scans (60 seconds long, separated by 10-minute intervals) were conducted using the H215O bolus method with averaged image subtraction.34,35
During each scan, the room was darkened and an opaque black cloth was draped over the gantry. Eye position was calibrated across 24° of visual angle and validated prior to each scan. Eye position was represented linearly across this range. Each eye movement condition was performed twice; conditions were presented in palindromic order with the condition presented first counterbalanced across subjects. Between scans, the cloth was removed and the room was illuminated to prevent dark adaptation.
Fifteen contiguous brain slices were obtained for each subject with a 6.5-mm center-to-center distance (axial field = 97.5 mm) and an intrinsic axial resolution of 6.0-mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM). Image reconstruction was performed using a measured attenuation correction from transmission scans and a Hanning-weighted reconstruction filter to an in-plane resolution of 8.0-mm FWHM. The PET and brain anatomical data were coregistered using the "AIR" program, (an automated 3-dimensional algorithm36 modified at the Montreal Neurological Institute to remove scalp. Functional images were convolved in-plane with a blurring kernel of 18 × 18 mm (FWHM), which improves local signal-noise ratio and reduces the effect of variations in gyral anatomy on image analysis.
The PET data were normalized for global differences in rCBF using a proportional model and dividing all voxel values by the mean voxel value. The mean difference in rCBF between conditions was obtained by averaging the difference images for all subjects. The mean difference volume was converted to a z-statistic volume, dividing each voxel by the mean SD in normalized rCBF for all intracerebral voxels.37
Qualitative and quantitative analyses of the pursuit traces were conducted. For the qualitative ratings, a modified version of a published rating scale38 was used to assign ratings on a 5-point scale to each subject's tracings. Qualitative ratings reliably differentiate between groups of subjects at low and high risk for schizophrenia, and global measures have been reported to be superior to single quantitative ratings.6,39 The ratings were made by 2 independent raters (G.O'D. and A.V.G.W.) who were blinded to subject identity and who achieved an interrater reliability (intraclass correlation) of 0.92. Subjects were blindly assigned to the ETD group based on the consensus of the 2 raters; subjects in the ETD group received from both raters a rating of 4 to 5 (abnormal) on at least 1 of their 2 60-second pursuit traces.
For the quantitative analysis, semiautomated custom analysis software (SR Research) was used to analyze the pursuit tracings. The first half-cycle of each trace was excluded, as were the 200 milliseconds before and after each blink. Saccades were automatically identified using criteria that take into account the velocity of ongoing pursuit in a 100-millisecond window prior to the saccade. The acceleration criterion was 3500°/s2 and the velocity criterion was 22°/s greater than the average eye velocity in that window. Saccades and blinks were excluded from gain analyses. Gain (eye velocity/target velocity) was then calculated based on the smooth eye movement in a 650-millisecond window centered on the peak target velocity in each half cycle. Average gain was calculated across all half-cycles in each scan. Test-retest reliability was calculated based on performance in the scanner and performance in the laboratory.
Saccades were identified according to acceleration (4000°/s2) and velocity criteria (22°/s). The number of saccades of amplitude greater than 2° (that is, the number of saccades that were not refixation saccades) were counted, and the average was compared across groups, using a 1-way analysis of variance.
Pursuit-Related Changes in rCBF
Activity during fixation was subtracted from activity during pursuit to define brain areas more active during pursuit. Two a priori regions were defined, the FEFs and V5. The FEFs have generally been reported to lie between Talairach coordinates40 +42 and +58 mm in the z-axis and between +2 and −12 mm on the y-axis, at approximately the junction of the superior frontal sulcus and the precentral sulcus in the middle frontal gyrus.41-44 The V5 region is at the temporal-occipital border (Brodmann areas 19/37), generally within the volume defined by Talairach coordinates ±30 and ±50 on the x-axis, −60 to −80 on the y-axis, and −4 to +10 on the z-axis.45-49 Statistical significance was calculated based on the highest z value in a peak, with a correction for multiple comparisons applied based on the number of resels (resolution elements) in the a priori regions of interest and on the total number of resels in the brain in post hoc regions. In FEF and V5, a z value of 3.0 corresponded to an adjusted P<.05 (based on a search volume of 2.2 resels; unadjusted P = .001). In other regions, a z value of 4.5 corresponded to an adjusted P<.05 (based on a whole-brain search volume of 554 resels; unadjusted P<.000003). The voxel with the highest z value was localized using x, y, and z coordinates.40
rCBF Activation vs Pursuit Gain
Pursuit-related changes in rCBF were regressed against pursuit gain in the relatives. The voxel with the highest correlation with pursuit gain was identified in both a priori and post hoc pursuit regions. Individual values for each subject were calculated by averaging the percent activation in a sphere of 10-mm radius centered on the voxel of maximum correlation in each region. These values were correlated with gain (and are presented in the scatterplot). Correlations were evaluated both with and without sex and smoking status as covariates.
Individual subtractions were conducted to determine the proportion of subjects in the 3 groups significantly activating each region of interest. This was done to ensure that the results of correlation analyses were not driven by subjects with ETD activating different areas within the pursuit regions than the subjects with normal tracking. One subject, a control, did not press the button during fixation but did press the button during pursuit. This subject was therefore excluded from individual and group analyses of the PGT data. We used a liberal activation threshold of 2.5 to ensure that even modest activation would be detected. We used previous studies of interindividual variability in peak location to define the search space.46,50 These studies have indicated that variation in the location of individual peaks is distributed approximately equally in the 3 axes, with an SD of 5 to 6 mm, such that 99% of individual peaks should be within 18 mm of the group center. For FEFs and V5, we defined the group center as the center of the a priori region (for FEFs ±32.5, −5, and +50; for V5 ±40, −70, and +3). For post hoc regions, the center of the search space was the location of the peak for that region for the individual's own group. Proportions were compared using a Kruskal-Wallis test.
Smooth Pursuit Eye Tracking
Ten relatives were rated blindly as having normal pursuit and 7 with abnormal pursuit (Table 1). Pursuit gain was highly correlated with qualitative ratings (r = −0.86, P<.001); relatives with ETD had lower gain than both comparison groups (Figure 1). Test-retest reliability was high: the subjects returned to the laboratory on average 1.1 years (SD, ±0.6 years) after their PET scan and the intraclass correlation coefficient between the pursuit gain in the scanner and the pursuit gain in the laboratory was 0.76 (P<.001). All relatives assigned to the ETD group based on pursuit during the PET scan also received a blind qualitative rating of abnormal pursuit in the laboratory. At the follow-up, it was found that 1 subject with ETD had developed her first psychotic symptoms 5 months after her scan. She was receiving neuroleptic medication; thus, her pursuit data were excluded from the reliability analyses.
All subjects fixated the target for the duration of the scan. No subject in any group broke fixation to make a saccade of more than 2°.
PURSUIT-RELATED CHANGES IN rCBF
Smooth pursuit significantly activated visual and oculomotor areas in normal controls (Table 2). This included activation of a dorsal and ventral region in the FEFs bilaterally (location of highest FEF activation in controls is shown in Figure 2). The greatest activations were in the cuneus in area 18 and area 19, and in the medial occipitotemporal area. The next highest activations were in V5 bilaterally. Subthreshold activations (unadjusted P<.001, adjusted P not significant) were found in the left supplementary eye fields,51 right superior parietal cortex, and left lateral occipitotemporal cortex.
Relatives of schizophrenic patients with normal pursuit activated the same region of FEFs that had the highest level of activation in controls. They also activated all posterior regions that were significantly activated in controls (Table 3). Relatives with ETD did not significantly activate FEFs (Table 4). The greatest FEF activation in this group was in the right ventral FEF (z = 1.9). The largest z score in the right dorsal FEFs, the region most activated by controls and relatives with normal tracking, was 0.8. Relatives with ETD did activate all the posterior regions significantly activated by controls, including V5.
rCBF CORRELATIONS WITH PURSUIT GAIN
Of the pursuit regions, only activation in right dorsal FEFs was significantly correlated with pursuit gain in relatives (r = 0.74, n = 17, P = .001) (Figure 3). Activation in this area had the highest correlation with pursuit gain in the entire brain. The peak correlation was centered at x = 28.1, y = −7.4, z = 58, less than 1 resolution element from the center of highest FEF activation in controls, and 3 mm from the maximum FEF activation in relatives with normal tracking. The correlation was not attributable to subjects' sex or smoking status (partial correlation between FEF activation and gain, covarying for sex, r = 0.69, P = .03; partial correlation between FEF activation and gain, covarying for smoking, r = 0.79, P = .006). The correlation between gain and activity in this region did not obtain in controls (r<0.1).
Percent activation in V5 did not significantly predict pursuit gain (left, r = 0.31, P = .23; right, r = 0.22, P = .39) with or without the covariates. Correlations between gain and activation in other posterior pursuit regions were also nonsignificant (all r<0.13, all P>.60).
Individual subtractions revealed that 90% of control subjects and 100% of the relatives with normal tracking had detectable activation of FEFs during pursuit. In contrast, only 42% of the relatives with ETD activated any region of FEFs. This difference was significant (χ22 = 9.3, P = .009). There were no significant differences between groups in the proportion of subjects activating V5 or any of the post hoc regions (all P>.20).
During smooth pursuit, control subjects significantly activated FEFs and V5 bilaterally, along with 2 regions in the cuneus and the medial occipitotemporal area. That FEFs and V5 play a role in pursuit is consistent with stimulation and single-unit data in the monkey,52-55 and with reports of pursuit abnormalities in patients with lesions to these regions.56-59 The large activation (10% ± 7.5%) in the dorsal cuneus in the region of the parieto-occipital sulcus is consistent with our previous results comparing pursuit with reflexive saccades.45
Differences in pursuit-related activation in relatives were not distributed throughout the brain, but were focused in frontal oculomotor regions. Relatives with normal tracking activated right dorsal FEF and the same posterior regions as controls. Relatives with ETD did not significantly activate FEFs. They did activate the same posterior regions as the other 2 groups. Of the pursuit regions, only activation in the right dorsal FEF was significantly associated with pursuit gain in relatives. This was the highest correlation with pursuit gain in the entire brain.
Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that smooth pursuit deficits in schizophrenic patients and their relatives reflect FEF dysfunction. Neural activity in the pursuit region of FEFs is correlated with pursuit eye velocity,60 and electrical stimulation of this region accelerates the eye smoothly, in proportion to the intensity of stimulation.61 Lesions of the FEFs produce pursuit deficits similar to those seen in schizophrenia.15 The region of FEFs, in the present study, that is best correlated with gain seems to be similar to the region activated by the antisaccade task, a test of saccade inhibition.43 Pursuit gain and the number of saccades made during pursuit are naturally negatively correlated since low-velocity smooth eye movements must be compensated for by catch-up saccades. Nonetheless, it is interesting to note that a recent study reported that neurons in the FEFs that can suppress saccades seem to be localized in the pursuit region.62 Thus activation of these overlapping regions could serve both to generate smooth eye movements and inhibit saccades during pursuit.
The fact that relatives with ETD did not significantly activate right dorsal FEF is unlikely to reflect a problem of statistical power since the effect size of activation in this region was 0.33 (compared with 1.1 for controls and 0.96 for normal relatives). With this effect size, 82 subjects with ETD would be required to reach a significance threshold of .05.63 That the relatives with ETD were activating a different region of FEF was also not supported. The 2 largest z scores in the region of the FEF were 1.9 (right ventral FEF) and 1.32 (left ventral FEF). These were the largest z values anywhere in the middle frontal and prefrontal gyri. Analysis of individual subtractions revealed that this was not attributable to the individual activations occurring at different locations. A significantly smaller proportion of subjects in the group with ETD activated any region of FEFs than in the other 2 groups.
Significant V5 activation was observed in all 3 groups. Average V5 activation was not significantly correlated with gain in the relatives. Thus this area would seem to play less of a role in ETD than the FEFs. However, it is worth noting that, for V5, percent activation may not be the best measure of functional integrity because the activation reflects motion signal (increased firing of neurons sensitive to the target speed) and background noise (increased firing of neurons sensitive to other velocities).
Our interpretation of the data is constrained by the study's limitations. First, subjects with ETD generated lower gain pursuit and more saccades during the scan; therefore, it is possible that activation differences simply reflect a failure of these subjects to engage in the required task. We do not believe this to be the case because eye movement recordings indicated that all subjects tracked the target without attentional lapses, and that subjects with ETD generated smooth eye movements that were, on average, 90% of the target speed. However, subjects with low gain did generate more saccades during pursuit, and during these saccades (epochs of about 20 milliseconds) they may not have been activating the pursuit area of FEFs. Our study cannot differentiate between the possibility that lower FEF activation reflects less time spent in pursuit, and the possibility that lower FEF activation is the cause of low gain pursuit. It is also not possible to determine from our data whether reduced prefrontal activation in relatives with ETD is specific to schizophrenia or whether it would be found in any group with abnormal pursuit. The inclusion of a group of normal subjects with ETD, or another group of neuropsychiatric patients with ETD, would be necessary to address this question.
Finally, there is some overlap between the percent activation of frontal regions in the relatives with ETD and the group with normal tracking. This is common in neuroanatomical studies comparing schizophrenic subjects with control populations. Future studies might include siblings from the same family, one with a behavioral marker for schizophrenia and the other without. The approach of using members of the same family or twins discordant for the disease has, in the past, strengthened the power of studies to detect subtle morphological changes related to the disease process.64 The same approach could be applied to detect subtle differences associated with behavioral markers of risk.
Some brain imaging data that support a role of genetic differences in brain development in risk for schizophrenia have already been provided by Cannon et al.65 This group showed that the size of the cortical sulci was a function of the degree of genetic vulnerability of the subject. Our data build on these observations by providing physiological evidence of reduced frontal cortical function in subjects who have markers of risk. We cannot say at this point whether relatives with ETD (or any other behavioral marker) are carrying genes involved in schizophrenia. However, the literature suggesting that ETD may be a behavioral manifestation of abnormalities in brain function related to schizophrenia is robust.4 It is of interest, in this context, that the one subject in our sample who later developed psychotic symptoms was in the group with ETD.
Accepted for publication August 17, 1999.
This work was supported in part by a National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression Young Investigator Award, an operating grant from the Medical Research Council, Ottawa, Canada (Dr O'Driscoll), a grant from the Scottish Rite Schizophrenia Research Program, Lexington, Mass (Drs O'Driscoll and Benkelfat), and a Chercheur-Boursier from the Fonds de Recherche en Santé de Québec, Montreal (Dr Benkelfat).
Preliminary results were presented at the Biological Psychiatry Annual Meeting, New York, NY, May 1996, and at the Society for Neurosciences Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, November 19, 1996.
We thank Eyal Reingold, PhD, and David Stampe, PhD, for adapting the Eyelink system and the software for our use; AMI Quebec and Suzanne King, PhD, for assistance with subject recruitment; John Grivell, BSc, for assistance with subject testing; Sylvain Milot, MSc, and Peter Neelin, MSc, for frequent consultations on the positron emission tomography analysis; and Gabriel Léger, MD, and Joseph Thavundayil, MD, for conducting the medical screening. We also thank Philip S. Holzman, PhD, Steven W. Matthysse, PhD, Deborah L. Levy, PhD, and Yue Chen, PhD, for valuable discussions.
Reprints: Gillian O'Driscoll, PhD, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Stewart Biological Sciences Building, 1205 Dr Penfield Ave, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H3A 1B1.
1.Gottesman
IIBertelsen
A Confirming unexpressed genotypes for schizophrenia: risks in the offspring of Fischer's Danish identical and fraternal discordant twins.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46867- 872
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Freedman
RAdler
LELeonard
S Alternative phenotypes for the complex genetics of schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45551- 558
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Callicott
JHEgan
MFBertolino
AMattay
VSLangheim
FJFrank
JAWeinberger
DR Hippocampal
N-acetyl aspartate in unaffected siblings of patients with schizophrenia: a possible intermediate neurobiological phenotype.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44941- 950
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Levy
DLHolzman
PSMatthysse
SMendell
NR Eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective.
Schizophr Bull. 1993;19461- 505
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Holzman
PSProctor
LRLevy
DLYasillo
NJMeltzer
HYHurt
SW Eye-tracking dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients and their relatives.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31143- 151
Google ScholarCrossref 6.Keefe
RSESilverman
JMMohs
RC
et al. Eye tracking, attention, and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54169- 176
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Siever
LJKeefe
RSEBernstein
DPCoccaro
EFKlar
HMZemishlany
ZPeterson
AEDavidson
MMahon
THorvath
T Eye tracking impairment in clinically identified patients with schizotypal personality disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 1990;6740- 745
Google Scholar 8.Schroeder
JBuchsbaum
MSSiegel
BVGeider
FJHaier
RJLohr
JWu
JPotkin
SG Patterns of cortical activity in schizophrenia.
Psychol Med. 1994;24947- 955
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Weinberger
DRAloia
MSGoldberg
TEBerman
KF The frontal lobes and schizophrenia.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6419- 427
Google Scholar 10.Levin
S Frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia, I: eye movement impairments.
J Psychiatr Res. 1984;1827- 55
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Katsanis
JIacono
WG Clinical, neuropsychological and brain structural correlates of smooth-pursuit eye tracking performance in chronic schizophrenics.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100526- 534
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Thaker
GKRoss
DEBuchanan
RWMoran
MJLahti
AKim
CMedoff
D Does pursuit abnormality in schizophrenia represent a deficit in the predictive mechanism?
Psychiatry Res. 1996;59221- 237
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Sweeney
JALuna
BSrinivasagam
NMKeshavan
MSSchooler
NRHaas
GLCarl
JR Eye tracking abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence for dysfunction in the frontal eye fields.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44698- 708
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Sereno
ABHolzman
PS Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37394- 401
Google ScholarCrossref 15.MacAvoy
MGBruce
CJ Comparison of smooth eye tracking disorder of schizophrenics with that of non-human primates with specific brain lesions.
Int J Neurosci. 1995;80117- 151
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Radant
ADClaypoole
KWingerson
DKCowley
DSRoy-Byrne
PP Relationships between neuropsychological and oculomotor measures in schizophrenia patients and normal controls.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42797- 805
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Clementz
BA Psychophysiological measures of (dis)inhibition as liability indicators for schizophrenia.
Psychophysiology. 1998;35648- 668
Google ScholarCrossref 18.O'Driscoll
GALenzenweger
MFHolzman
PS Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye tracking and schizotypy.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55837- 843
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Ross
DEThaker
GKHolcomb
HHCascella
NGMedoff
DR Abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenic patients are associated with cerebral glucose metabolism in oculomotor regions.
Psychiatry Res. 1995;5853- 67
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Zeki
SWatson
JDLueck
CJFriston
KJKennard
CFrackowiak
RS A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex.
J Neurosci. 1991;11641- 649
Google Scholar 21.Stuve
TAFriedman
LJesberger
JAGilmore
GCStrauss
MEMeltzer
HY The relationship between smooth pursuit performance, motion perception and sustained visual attention in patients with schizophrenia and normal controls.
Psychol Med. 1997;27143- 152
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Chen
YNakayama
KLevy
DLMatthysse
SWHolzman
PS Psychophysical isolation of a motion-processing deficit in schizophrenics and their relatives and its association with impaired smooth pursuit.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;964724- 4729
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Chen
YPalafox
GPNakayama
KLevy
DLMatthysse
SHolzman
PS Motion perception in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56149- 154
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Clementz
BA Saccades to moving targets in schizophrenia: evidence for normal posterior cortex functioning.
Psychophysiology. 1996;33650- 654
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Thaker
GKRoss
DECassady
SLAdami
HMLaPorte
DMedoff
DRLahti
A Smooth pursuit eye movements to extraretinal motion signals: deficits in relatives of patients with schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55830- 836
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Hollignshead
ARedlich
F Social Class and Mental Illness. New York, NY Wiley and Sons Inc1958;
27.Oldfield
RC The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
Neuropsychologia. 1971;997- 113
Google ScholarCrossref 28.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1996;
29.Silverstein
AB Reappraisal of the validity of a short short form of Wechsler's scales.
Psychol Rep. 1970;26559- 561
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Amador
XFSackeim
HAMukherjee
SHalperin
RNeeley
PMaclin
ESchnur
D Specificity of smooth pursuit eye movement and visual fixation abnormalities in schizophrenia: comparison to mania and normal controls.
Schizophr Res. 1991;5135- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Collins
DLNeelin
PPeters
TMEvans
AC Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space.
J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1994;18192- 205
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Evans
ACCollins
DLMills
SRBrown
EDKelly
RL 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes.
IEEE Conference Record, Nuclear Sciences Symposium and Medical Imaging Conference. San Francisco, Calif IEEE1993;1813- 1817
Google Scholar 33.Evans
ACThompson
CJMarrett
SMeyer
EMazza
M Performance characteristics of the PC-2048: a new 15-slice encoded crystal PET scanner for neurological studies.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 1991;1090- 98
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Fox
PTMintun
MAReiman
EMRaichle
ME Enhanced detection of focal brain responses using intersubject averaging and change-distribution analysis of subtracted PET images.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1988;8642- 653
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Raichle
MEMartin
WRHerscovitch
PMintun
MAMarkham
J Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H2(15)O, II: implementation and validation.
J Nucl Med. 1983;24790- 798
Google Scholar 36.Woods
RPMazziotta
JCCherry
SR MRI-PET registration with automated algorithm.
J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1993;17536- 546
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Worsley
KJEvans
ACMarrett
SNeelin
P A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1992;12900- 918
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Benitez
JT Eye-tracking and optokinetic tests: diagnostic significance in peripheral and central vestibular disorders.
Laryngoscope. 1970;80834- 848
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Friedman
LJesberger
JASiever
LJThompson
PMohs
RMeltzer
HY Smooth pursuit performance in patients with affective disorders or schizophrenia and normal controls: analysis with specific oculomotor measures, RMS error and qualitative ratings.
Psychol Med. 1995;25387- 403
Google ScholarCrossref 40.Talairach
JTournoux
P Co-Planar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain. Stuttgart, Germany Thieme1988;
41.Anderson
TJJenkins
IHBrooks
DJHawken
MBFrackowiak
RSKennard
C Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man: a PET study.
Brain. 1994;1171073- 1084
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Sweeney
JAMintun
MAKwee
SWiseman
MBBrown
DLRosenberg
DRCarl
JR Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory.
J Neurophysiol. 1996;75454- 468
Google Scholar 43.O'Driscoll
GAAlpert
NMMatthysse
SWLevy
DLRauch
SLHolzman
PS Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92925- 929
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Paus
T Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review.
Neuropsychologia. 1996;34475- 483
Google ScholarCrossref 45.O'Driscoll
GAStrakowski
SMAlpert
NMMatthysse
SWRauch
SLLevy
DLHolzman
PS Differences in cerebral activation during smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements using positron emission tomography.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44685- 689
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Watson
JDMyers
RFrackowiak
RSHajnal
JVWoods
RPMazziotta
JCShipp
SZeki
S Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging.
Cereb Cortex. 1993;370- 94
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Tootell
RBReppas
JBKwong
KKMalach
RBorn
RTBrady
TJRosen
BRBelliveau
JW Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging.
J Neurosci. 1995;153215- 3230
Google Scholar 48.Orban
GADupont
PDebruyn
BVandenberghe
RRosier
AMortelmans
L Human brain activity related to speed discrimination tasks.
Exp Brain Res. 1998;1229- 22
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Barton
JJSimpson
TKiriakopoulos
EStewart
CCrawley
AGuthrie
BWood
MMikulis
D Functional MRI of lateral occipitotemporal cortex during pursuit and motion perception.
Ann Neurol. 1996;40387- 398
Google ScholarCrossref 50.Hasnain
MKFox
PTWoldorff
MG Intersubject variability of functional areas in the human visual cortex.
Hum Brain Mapping. 1998;6301- 315
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Schlag
JSchlag-Rey
M Evidence for a supplementary eye field.
J Neurophysiol. 1987;57179- 200
Google Scholar 52.MacAvoy
MGGottlieb
JPBruce
CJ Smooth pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field.
Cereb Cortex. 1991;195- 102
Google ScholarCrossref 53.Tian
JRLynch
JC Functionally defined smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions in the frontal eye field of Cebus monkeys.
J Neurophysiol. 1996;762740- 2753
Google Scholar 54.Groh
JMBorn
RTNewsome
WT How is a sensory map read out? effects of microstimulation in visual area MT on saccades and smooth pursuit eye movements.
J Neurosci. 1997;174312- 4330
Google Scholar 55.Bremmer
FIlg
UJThiele
ADistler
CHoffmann
KP Eye position effects in monkey cortex, I: visual and pursuit-related activity in extrastriate areas MT and MST.
J Neurophysiol. 1997;77944- 961
Google Scholar 56.Morrow
MJSharpe
JA Retinotopic and directional deficits of smooth pursuit initiation after posterior cerebral hemispheric lesions.
Neurology. 1993;43595- 603
Google ScholarCrossref 57.Rivaud
SMuri
RGaymard
BVermersch
APierrot-Deseilligny
C Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans.
Exp Brain Res. 1994;102110- 120
Google ScholarCrossref 58.Morrow
MJSharpe
JA Deficits of smooth-pursuit eye movement after unilateral frontal lobe lesions.
Ann Neurol. 1995;37443- 451
Google ScholarCrossref 59.Heide
WKurzidim
KKompf
D Deficits of smooth pursuit eye movements after frontal and parietal lesions.
Brain. 1996;1191951- 1969
Google ScholarCrossref 60.Gottlieb
JPBruce
CJMacAvoy
MG Smooth eye movements elicited by microstimulation in the primate frontal eye field.
J Neurophysiol. 1993;69786- 799
Google Scholar 61.Gottlieb
JPMacAvoy
MGBruce
CJ Neural responses related to smooth-pursuit eye movements and their correspondence with electrically elicited smooth eye movements in the primate frontal eye field.
J Neurophysiol. 1994;721634- 1653
Google Scholar 62.Burman
DBruce
CJ Suppression of task-related saccades by electrical stimulation in the primate's frontal eye field.
J Neurophysiol. 1997;772252- 2267
Google Scholar 63.Rosenthal
RRosnow
RL Essentials of Behavioural Research: Methods and Data Analysis. New York, NY McGraw-Hill1984;
64.Suddath
RLChristison
GWTorrey
EFCasanova
MFWeinberger
DR Anatomical abnormalities in the brains of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia.
N Engl J Med. 1990;322789- 794
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Cannon
TDMednick
SAParnas
JSchulsinger
FPraestholm
JVestergaard
A Developmental brain abnormalities in the offspring of schizophrenic mothers, I: contributions of genetic and perinatal factors.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50551- 564
Google ScholarCrossref