Background
Prior studies of twins reared together suggest that regular tobacco use (RTU) is substantially heritable. However, strong social influences on RTU might have biased these results.
Methods
We examine the self-report lifetime history of RTU in members of 778 male-male and female-female twin pairs, raised together and apart, born from 1890 to 1958 and ascertained through the population-based Swedish Twin Registry.
Results
In men, the pattern of twin resemblance for RTU suggested both genetic and rearing-environmental effects, which, in the best-fit biometrical model, accounted for 61% and 20% of the variance in liability to RTU, respectively. For women, overall results were hard to interpret, but became clearer when divided by birth cohort. In women born before 1925, rates of RTU were low and twin resemblance was environmental in origin. In later cohorts, rates of RTU in women increased substantially, as did heritability. For women born after 1940, heritability of RTU was similar to that seen in men (63%).
Conclusions
Genetic factors play an important etiologic role in RTU. In women, the impact of genetic factors increased in more recent cohorts, suggesting that, as social restrictions on female tobacco use relaxed over time, heritable influences increased in importance.
THE IMPACT of regular tobacco use (RTU) on the health of the American public has been voluminously documented.1-3 The public health problems associated with tobacco use are now reaching epidemic proportions worldwide.4 It is therefore urgent to understand the sources of individual differences in risk for RTU. While various social, economic, and personal factors affect risk, genetic factors also play an important etiologic role.5,6 In a recent review of more than 17,500 reared-together monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs from 14 different studies, model fitting estimated that genetic, familial-environmental, and individual-specific environmental risk factors accounted for 56%, 24%, and 20% of the liability to RTU, respectively.6
However, risk for RTU is also influenced by psychosocial factors, among which peer group influences are particularly potent.7-9 During adolescence (when RTU commonly begins), MZ twins spend more time together and share peer groups more closely than do DZ twins.10-12 Studies of reared-together twins may confound genetic and environmental sources of twin resemblance.
Four prior studies have examined tobacco use in reared-apart twins.13-16 These studies, however, suffered from methodologic limitations, including small sample size,13-15 nonsystematic ascertainment,13,15 lack of reared-apart DZ twins13-15 and/or reared-together MZ and DZ twins,13-16 and absence of structural equation modeling.13-16
In this report, we examine lifetime RTU in reared-apart and reared-together MZ and DZ twins ascertained from the population-based Swedish Twin Registry as part of the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging.17,18 Structural equation models that exploit the full power of reared-together and reared-apart twins are employed.
The sample was composed of twins from a subregistry of the Swedish Twin Registry, which includes entries for about 25,000 like-sex twin pairs born in Sweden between 1886 and 1958.19 The subregistry, known as the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging, consists of twins who reported being separated before age 11 years and reared apart and a sample of twins reared together matched on the basis of sex, age, and county of birth.17 The reared-apart and reared-together twins were similar on most variables, including smoking rates.17 The study was initiated in 1984 with a mail-out questionnaire to all surviving individuals from both samples. Questionnaire mail-outs were repeated in 1987, 1990, and 1993. Both members of 778 pairs responded to 1 or more questionnaires. The mean (SD) age at the time of the first mailing was 58.8 (13.6) years; 61.6% of the sample were women, which conforms to the sex expectations for an elderly population.18
Zygosity diagnoses were first made on the basis of physical similarities and confirmed in 368 pairs by serological tests.18 The maximal error rate was 8% and did not differ by rearing status. For those not seen in person or having ambiguous information, zygosity remained unknown. Although twins were considered reared-apart if they were separated by the age of 11 years, the distribution of age at separation is highly skewed: 52%, 69%, and 82% of the reared-apart twins were separated before their first, second, and fifth birthdays, respectively.18 Reasons for separation varied; most were separated because of the death of 1 or both parents and/or economic hardship. For further details on the procedures, sample, and design, see Pedersen et al.18
Assessment of tobacco use
Lifetime tobacco use was assessed by a series of questions in the self-report questionnaire. We used the first questionnaire with available data from each twin, obtaining results from the first through fourth waves for 94.0%, 3.8%, 1.4%, and 0.8% of our sample, respectively. The first question in this section was "Have you ever smoked or used snuff?" Four responses were provided: (1) no, never tried; (2) yes, but only tried; (3) yes, now and then (eg, at parties); and (4) yes, have smoked/smoke (have used snuff/use snuff) regularly. We defined RTU as a response of 4 to this item. Further items allowed us to determine that 71% of tobacco consumption was from cigarettes, 8% from cigars, 1% from cigarillos, 11% from pipes, and 9% from snuff.
A reared-apart twin design, which combines features of twin and adoption studies, has 4 twin groups: MZ apart (MZA), MZ together (MZT), DZ apart (DZA), and DZ together (DZT). These 4 groups provide direct tests both for the presence of genetic effects and for the impact of rearing environment. Genetic effects on RTU would predict greater similarity for RTU in MZA vs DZA twins and MZT vs DZT twins, because MZ twins share an identical genetic makeup, while DZ twins share on average 50% of their segregating genes. An effect of the rearing environment on risk for RTU would predict that similarity for RTU would be greater in MZT vs MZA and DZT vs DZA twin pairs because reared-together twins were raised in the same family, while the reared-apart twins were not.
We present information about twin resemblance in 3 ways. Probandwise concordance is the percentage of co-twins of twins with RTU who themselves report RTU. The odds ratio (OR) is the ratio of the proportion of RTU among co-twins of twins with RTU and the proportion of RTU among co-twins of twins without RTU. The significance of the ORs and the 95% confidence interval (CI) was estimated by logistic regression. The difference in ORs between twin groups twins is assessed by a log-linear model as operationalized in the SAS routine GENMOD.20 The P values reported are 1-tailed for these analyses because, in studies of familial resemblance, there is a compelling rationale for directional hypotheses. That is, we are, in almost all instances, interested only in excess resemblance in relatives beyond chance expectations. In this study, we begin with robust directional hypotheses of positive correlations in twins for risk to RTU and greater resemblance in MZ vs DZ and reared-together vs reared-apart twins.
We used a liability-threshold model to estimate the genetic and environmental contributions to twin resemblance. For categorical characteristics like RTU, the estimates are for the resemblance of twins in a pair for their liability to develop RTU.21 Liability is assumed to be continuous and normally distributed in the population, with individuals who exceed a theoretical threshold developing RTU. We also present the tetrachoric correlation, defined as the correlation in members of twin pairs for the liability to RTU.21,22
Like studies of twins reared together (see Kendler23 and Neale and Cardon24 for more detailed descriptions), studies of reared-apart twins can estimate the percentage of variance in liability due to additive genetic (A) and individual-specific environmental effects (E). However, because we have twins who were and were not reared together, we can also estimate 2 additional environment components, termed shared environment (S) and correlated environment (C), respectively. Shared environment, which might also be termed rearing environment, contributes to twin resemblance for reared-together but not reared-apart twins, and reflects the effect of being raised in the same household and community. Correlated environment refers to experiences that occur in common both with twins reared apart and twins reared together, and would include the uterine environment, the impact of contact between the twins that occurs after separation, and, because twins are identical in age, their exposure to the same social, cultural, and historical influences on tobacco use.
Therefore, in these analyses, we fit models with 4 sources of variance in liability to RTU: A, S, C, and E. As in studies of reared-together twins, E, or individual-specific environment influences, are those factors (eg, accidents, illnesses, divergent exposure to peer groups before or after leaving home) that make members of a twin pair different from one another with respect to their liability to RTU.
Models were fit directly to the contingency tables by the method of maximum likelihood using the Mx structural modeling program.25 Model fit is evaluated according to the principle of parsimony. Models with fewer parameters are considered preferable if they do not provide significantly worse fit. We operationalize parsimony by the Akaike information criteria (AIC) statistic26 (which has been recently validated27), calculated as the model χ2 minus 2 times the degrees of freedom (df). We then present parameter estimates from the best-fitting models, where a2 equal the percentage of variance in liability to RTU caused by additive genetic effects and s2, c2, and e2 equal the percentage in variance in liability to RTU caused by shared, correlated, and individual-specific environmental effects, respectively.
A possible difference in the probability of RTU across birth cohorts can be approached in 2 ways. To remove the effect of birth cohort (which should contribute to the estimate for C), the impact of year of birth (Ag for age) on liability to RTU can be added to the model.24 To evaluate the hypothesis that the sources of twin resemblance for RTU change over cohorts requires that we first fit a relaxed model in which the parameters (eg, A, S, C, E) are estimated separately in each age cohort and then fit a constrained model to the same data in which the parameter estimates are set equal across the cohorts. We can then compare the fit of the relaxed vs constrained model, both by a χ2 difference test and by the AIC.
We used logistic regression to predict risk for RTU from year of birth and to test whether, in reared-apart twins, pair concordance vs discordance for RTU is predicted by age at separation.
Sample, reliability, and test for biases
Information on RTU was available for both members of 778 same-sex twin pairs. Regular tobacco use was substantially more common in men than in women (Table 1). Age at smoking initiation (mean ± SD) in the entire sample was 20.1 ± 7.5 years and was significantly lower in men (18.5 ± 6.1) than in women (22.4 ± 8.8) (test for unequal variances: t451.4 = 6.42; P<.0001). In the 370 complete reared-apart twin pairs, the mean ± SD age at separation (2.7 ± 3.0 years) was much younger than age at smoking initiation (19.7 ± 7.4 years). In only 1 of these 740 separated twins (from a male DZA pair) was the reported age at smoking initiation (age 10 years) before age at separation. In reared-apart twin pairs, no association was seen between age at separation and concordance for RTU (χ21 = 0.09, P = .77).
The distribution of these pairs by sex, zygosity, and rearing is shown in Table 1. The percentage of men and women in each cohort who reported RTU is shown in Table 2. Year of birth was unrelated to the probability of RTU in men (OR, 1.00; 95% CI, 0.99-1.01; χ21 = 0.23; P = .06), but strongly related in women (OR, 1.06; 95% CI, 1.05-1.07; χ21 = 139.9; P<.0001) (Figure 1).
Reliability of our measure of lifetime RTU was assessed in 1160 twins who responded to the questionnaire in both 1987 and 1990. The agreement rate and κ coefficients28 were 95% and +0.87 (95% CI, 0.84-0.91), respectively.
Twin resemblance for RTU was substantial in the 55 pairs of MZA male twins (OR, 8.3; 95% CI, 2.1-33.1; P = .001; r = +0.66) (Table 1). In addition, the pattern of twin resemblance in the male-male pairs suggested both a genetic effect (MZA>DZA and MZT>DZT) and an effect of shared rearing environment (MZT>MZA and DZT>DZA). Examining the difference in the ORs, the test for genetic effects in the reared-apart twins (MZA vs DZA) was significant (χ21 = 4.0, P = .02) and the test in reared-together twins was nearly significant (MZT vs DZT) (χ21 = 2.4, P = .06). The test for shared environment was not significant in MZ pairs (MZT vs MZA) (χ21 = 0.5, P = .25) but was in DZ pairs (DZT vs DZA) (χ21 = 3.0, P = .04).
Twin resemblance for RTU was also evident in the 56 pairs of MZA female twins (OR, 3.8; 95% CI, 1.0-14.2; P = .03; r = +0.45). However, a readily interpretable pattern of results was not seen in all the female-female pairs. The DZA correlation for RTU exceeded the MZA correlation, while the MZT correlation was modestly greater than the DZT correlation. One of these 4 comparisons of ORs was significant (MZT vs MZA twins: χ21 = 3.2, P = .04).
As outlined in Table 3, we began by fitting a full model containing A, S, C, and E. For men, the full model (model 1) fit well (χ21 = 0.58, P = .46). We next, in turn, set the parameters C, S, and A to 0 in models 2, 3, and 4, respectively. Of these 3 models, model 2 fit as well as the full model, with 1 less parameter, and therefore had the best AIC. Then, we tried to further simplify model 2 by setting the parameters S and A to 0 in models 5 and 6, respectively; neither improved the AIC value. In particular, the SE model fit quite poorly and could be strongly rejected against the ASE model (χ21 = 23.0, P<.0001), providing evidence for the importance of genetic effects on RTU in men. The parameter estimates and 95% CIs for the best-fitting model, model 2, are seen in Table 4. These results suggest that in Swedish men, approximately 60% of the variance in liability to RTU is genetic, while shared rearing environment and individual-specific environment each account for approximately 20% of the variance in liability.
The results in women (Table 3) were different. The full model (model 1) did not fit well (χ21 = 8.4, P = .004). Model 2 (which dropped C) produced a large deterioration in fit, indicating that correlated environmental effects were important. Both model 3 (which dropped S) and model 4 (which dropped A) produced an improvement in the AIC over model 1, although model 3 was modestly superior. Attempts at simplifying model 3 by dropping C (model 5) or A (model 6) did not result in further AIC improvement. As seen in Table 4, the best-fit model, model 3, suggested that 63% of the variance in liability to RTU in Swedish women was because of the correlated environment, while genetic and individual-specific environmental factors were responsible for 15% and 22% of the variance, respectively.
The discrepancy between the results in men and women was perplexing. Given the strong relationship between year of birth and RTU in women, we refit the data to a model containing year of birth. As outlined in Table 3, the best-fit model (model 4) included year of birth, as well as A, C, and E. The parameter estimates for this best-fit model (Table 4) did not change for a2 or for e2, but the estimate for c2 declined from 63% to 43%, while 20% of the variance was now ascribed to Ag. We also fit this model to the results for male twins. The best-fit model was identical to that obtained in our previous analyses.
Gene by cohort interaction models
Including year of birth accounts only for main effects of age on RTU and not for interactions between age and genetic or environmental sources of twin resemblance. Given the rapid increase in rates of RTU in the Swedish women, could the etiologic role of genetic or environmental factors have changed over time? To explore this possibility, we began by fitting our full ASCE model to the 3 age cohorts in men and women. Sample sizes with the earliest birth cohort (1890-1909) were too small in both men and women to obtain stable estimates (Table 2). For men, estimates of heritability were stable across the 3 remaining cohorts, ranging from 0.51 to 0.58. However, some instability was seen across cohorts in the environmental sources of liability for RTU.
The picture was quite different in women. Heritability substantially increased across the 3 cohorts, from 0 in women born from 1910 to 1924, to 0.21 from those born in 1925 to 1939, to 0.64 for those born from 1940 to 1958. A concomitant decrease was seen in the role of S and C in the etiology of RTU.
We evaluated these results by first fitting a relaxed model, which allowed the genetic and environmental parameter estimates to differ across cohorts, and then a constrained model, in which these parameters were set equal across the 3 cohorts. For men, the constrained model (χ230 = 30.0) could not be rejected against the relaxed model (χ224 = 24.7) (difference test: χ26 = 5.3, P = .51) and the constrained model had the superior AIC (−30.0 vs −23.3).
For women, the constrained model (χ230 = 33.2) could be rejected against the relaxed model (χ224 = 19.1) (difference test: χ26 = 14.1; P = .03) and the relaxed model had the superior AIC (−28.9 vs −26.8). For women, but not for men, the etiologic role of genetic and environmental factors for RTU differed significantly across birth cohorts. The relationship between the changes in prevalence and heritability of RTU in the 3 birth cohorts of Swedish male and female twins is illustrated in Figure 1.
Strengths and limitations of the study
The major strength of this study is the inclusion of both reared-together and reared-apart MZ and DZ twins. This combination of twin and adoption designs is especially powerful at resolving the genetic and environmental sources of individual differences. Furthermore, this sample of twins is the largest systematically ascertained sample of reared-apart and reared-together twins currently available.
Three limitations are noteworthy. First, age at separation was variable, although more than two thirds were separated by their second birthday. We showed no correlation, among the separated twin pairs, between age at separation and similarity for RTU; only 1 twin began tobacco use before separation from his co-twin. Second, although, to our knowledge, ours is the largest available systematic sample of twins reared together and apart, our sample size was still only moderate, as indicated by the CIs for the parameter estimates. Third, we relied solely on self-report measures of tobacco use; however, such measures have been found to be relatively reliable and valid29,30 and had good test-retest reliability in our sample.
As would be expected if genetic factors were of etiologic importance in RTU, MZA male twins were highly correlated in their liability to RTU (OR, 8.3; 95% CI, 2.1-33.1; r = +0.66). Furthermore, the correlation in risk to RTU was higher in MZA than in DZA twins and higher in MZT than in DZT twins. Model fitting estimated the heritability of liability to RTU in male Swedish twins to be 0.61, quite close to the 0.56 estimate from our recent review of 14 studies of twins reared together.6
In addition, the correlation in liability to RTU was apparently influenced by rearing status, being higher in MZT than MZA twins and higher in DZT than DZA twins. This pattern suggests that being reared in the same family influences risk for RTU. This impression was supported by the model fitting, which estimated that 20% of the variance in liability to RTU in Swedish male twins resulted from shared rearing environment, similar to the 24% estimated from our recent review.6
As would be predicted if genetic factors influenced risk for RTU, female MZA twins were significantly correlated in their liability to RTU (OR, 3.8; 95% CI, 1.0-14.2; r = 0.45). However, the similarity in risk was higher in DZA than MZA twins and only slightly greater in MZT than DZT pairs. Modeling suggested that most twin resemblance was because of correlated environment and age, with genes playing only a modest role.
When women were examined by birth cohort, a large secular change was observed in rates of RTU. Given the widely different social context of smoking for women in Sweden over this period, the pattern of genetic and environmental risk factors for RTU might also have changed over time. Analyzing 3 age cohorts separately revealed a dramatic increase in heritability over this historical period, from 0 in those born before 1924 to levels comparable with that seen in men for those born after 1940.
This gene-by-cohort interaction with increasing heritability in twins from more recent cohorts has been described for other behavioral and health-related traits: age at intercourse in Australian twins,31 alcohol consumption in Finnish twins,32 and educational level in Norwegian33 and Swedish twins.34 However, P. Madden, PhD (written communication, November 1999), did not find this pattern for smoking initiation in Australian, Swedish, and Finnish twins, although none of these samples included twins born before 1920. Also, we found no evidence for changes in heritability of alcohol abuse in Swedish men born from 1902 to 1949.35
The most plausible explanation for our finding is that a reduction in the social restrictions on smoking in women in Sweden as the 20th century progressed permitted genetic factors influencing the risk for RTU to increasingly express themselves. This interpretation is consistent with the findings of educational attainment for women in the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging.34
Comparison with other studies of tobacco use in reared-apart twins
We are aware of 4 prior studies of RTU in reared-apart twins. Fisher13 reported that the concordance rate for "smoking habit" in 27 MZ twins reared apart, ascertained through the Maudsley Psychiatric Hospital, was 85%. In 12 pairs of systematically ascertained MZ twins from Denmark who were reared apart (Raaschou-Nielsen14), the concordance rate for smoking was 75%. In the largest previous sample of MZA twins (42 pairs), ascertained mostly through a television appeal in England,15 the concordance rate for smoking was 77%. In a systematic study of Finnish reared-apart twins,16 which included both MZ and DZ pairs, probandwise concordance for lifetime cigarette smoking was similar in MZ (29 pairs) (67%) and DZ twins (88 pairs) (68%). In our sample of 111 MZ twin pairs reared apart, we found the probandwise concordance rate for lifetime RTU to be 81%, similar to that reported in previous samples. Concordance rates in our 259 pairs of DZA twins (68%) was identical to that found in the Finnish sample.16
Level of tobacco comsumption among regular users
Substance use is likely a 2-stage phenomenon. One set of risk factors influences initiation. Another, possibly correlated set of factors affects the risk for high levels of use or misuse given initiation. We found this pattern with regular smoking and nicotine dependence in female-female Virginia twin pairs.36 Although we had no direct measures of nicotine dependence in this Swedish sample, we could examine average tobacco intake—a rough proxy for levels of dependence.36 Twin correlations for amount of tobacco consumed were significant in male and female MZA (+0.36 [P = .05] and +0.83 [P = .01], respectively) and MZT pairs (+0.62 [P<.001] and +0.76 [P<.001], respectively) but not in male and female DZA (+0.27 and +0.01, respectively) and DZT pairs (+0.24 and +0.20, respectively). In accordance with our prior findings,36 these results suggest that genetic factors not only play an important etiologic role in the initiation of RTU, but also affect the subsequent level of consumption among regular users.
Implications for research
Twin studies have played an increasingly central role in attempts to clarify the role of genetic and environmental risk factors in the etiology of psychiatric and substance use disorders.23 However, a key assumption in such studies is that MZ and DZ twins are equally correlated in their exposure to etiologically relevant environmental risk factors. This "equal environment assumption" has been empirically supported for psychiatric conditions by most,11,12,37-39 but not all investigations.40,41 Regular tobacco use is likely to be a particularly problematic trait for standard twin studies because of the strong peer group influences on risk for smoking initiation.8,9 Furthermore, in childhood and adolescence, MZ twins spend more time together and share peer groups more closely than do DZ twins.10-12 Studies of reared-together twins may, therefore, produce estimates of heritability that are inflated because MZ twins are more highly correlated than DZ twins for peer-group influences on RTU. In a study of Virginia reared-together twins, the extent of social contact in adolescence predicted twin resemblance only for smoking initiation among the 8 psychiatrically relevant traits examined.12 In the reared-apart twins from Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging, Pedersen et al42 found a small but significant relationship between degree of contact and twin similarity for alcohol consumption.
It is, therefore, of particular interest that in these twins reared together and apart, the estimated heritabilities of RTU for all men and for women born after 1940 were very similar to those found in prior studies of twins reared together. Similarly, at least in men, we also replicated the evidence from studies of twins reared together that rearing environment significantly contributes to twin resemblance for RTU. Our ability, in this combined twin-adoption design, to replicate closely the results from studies of twins reared together suggests that this traditional twin design, when applied to psychiatric and substance use–related phenotypes, is likely to provide relatively accurate answers and not to be substantially biased.
Finally, the results presented here further strengthen the evidence that genetic factors in humans play an important role in influencing the vulnerability to RTU as well as to quantity of tobacco consumed among regular users. These findings, therefore, should provide further impetus to efforts to identify the specific loci and alleles that influence these vulnerabilities (eg, Sabol et al43 and Straub et al44).
Accepted for publication April 11, 2000.
Supported in part by grants DA-10228, DA-11287, MH-01227, and MH-54150 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Kendler). The Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging is supported by grants AG 04563 and 10175 from the National Institutes on Aging, Bethesda, Md, a grant from the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging, Chicago, Ill, and grant 97:0147:1B from the Swedish Council for Social Research, Stockholm, Sweden.
Corresponding author: Kenneth S. Kendler, MD, Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia, Virginia Commonwealth University, PO Box 980126, Richmond, VA 23298-0126 (e-mail: kendler@hsc.vcu.edu).
1.Schoenborn
CABoyd
GM Smoking and other tobacco use.
Vital Health Stat 10. 1989;1691- 79
Google Scholar 2.Not Available, The Health Consequences of Smoking: Cardiovascular Disease: A Report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC Public Health Service, Office on Smoking and Health1983;DHHS publication (PHS) 84-50204
4.World Health Organization, Tobacco or Health: A Global Status Report. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1997;
5.Swan
GECarmelli
D Behavior genetic investigations of cigarette smoking and related issues in twins. Blum
KNoble
EPeds.
Handbook of Psychiatric Genetics. New York, NY CRC Press1997;387- 406
Google Scholar 7.Swan
GECreeser
RMurray
M When and why children first start to smoke.
Int J Epidemiol. 1990;19323- 330
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Friedman
LSLichtenstein
EBiglan
A Smoking onset among teens: an empirical analysis of initial situations.
Addict Behav. 1985;101- 13
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Urberg
KAShyu
SJLiang
J Peer influence in adolescent cigarette smoking.
Addict Behav. 1990;15247- 255
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Loehlin
JCNichols
RC Heredity, Environment and Personality: A Study of 850 Sets of Twins. Austin University of Texas Press1976;
11.Kendler
KSHeath
ACMartin
NGEaves
LJ Symptoms of anxiety and depression in a volunteer twin population: the etiologic role of genetic and environmental factors.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43213- 221
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Kendler
KSGardner
CO
Jr Twin studies of adult psychiatric and substance dependence disorders: are they biased by differences in the environmental experiences of monozygotic and dizygotic twins in childhood and adolescence?
Psychol Med. 1998;28625- 633
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Raaschou-Nielsen
E Smoking habits in twins.
Dan Med Bull. 1960;782- 88
Google Scholar 15.Shields
J Monozygotic Twins Brought Up Apart and Brought Up Together. London, England Oxford University Press1962;
16.Kaprio
JKoskenvuo
MLanginvainio
H Finnish twins reared apart, IV: smoking and drinking habits: a preliminary analysis of the effect of heredity and environment.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma). 1984;33425- 433
Google Scholar 17.Pedersen
NLFriberg
LFloderus-Myrhed
BMcClearn
GEPlomin
R Swedish early separated twins: identification and characterization.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma). 1984;33243- 250
Google Scholar 18.Pedersen
NLMcClearn
GEPlomin
RNesselroade
JRBerg
SdeFaire
U The Swedish Adoption Twin Study of Aging: an update.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma). 1991;407- 20
Google Scholar 19.Cederlof
RLU The Swedish Twin Registry. Nance
WEAllen
PParisi
Peds.
Twin Research Biology and Epdidemiology. New York, NY Alan R Liss1978;189- 195
Google Scholar 20.SAS Institute, SAS/STAT User's Guide, Version 6. 4th ed.Vols 1 and 2 Cary, NC SAS Institute Inc1990;
21.Falconer
DS The inheritance of liability to certain diseases, estimated from the incidence among relatives.
Ann Hum Genet. 1965;2951- 76
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Pearson
K Mathematical contributions to the theory of evolution, VIII: on the correlation of characters not quantitatively measurable.
Proc R Soc. 1901;66241- 244
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Kendler
KS Twin studies of psychiatric illness: current status and future directions.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50905- 915
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Neale
MCCardon
LR Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Dordrecht, the Netherlands Kluwer Academic Publishers BV1992;
25.Neale
MC Statistical Modelling With Mx. Richmond, Va Dept of Psychiatry1991;
27.Williams
LJHolahan
PJ Parsimony-based fit indices for multiple-indicator models: do they work?
Structural Equation Modeling. 1994;1161- 189
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Luepker
RVPallonen
UEMurray
DMPirie
PL Validity of telephone surveys in assessing cigarette smoking in young adults.
Am J Public Health. 1989;79202- 204
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Slattery
MLHunt
SCFrench
TKFord
MHWilliams
RR Validity of cigarette smoking habits in three epidemiologic studies in Utah.
Prev Med. 1989;1811- 19
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Dunne
MPMartin
NGStatham
DJSlutske
WSDinwiddie
SHBucholz
KKMadden
PAHeath
AC Genetic and environmental contributions to variance in age at first sexual intercourse.
Psychol Sci. 1997;8211- 216
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Kaprio
JRose
RJRomanov
KKoskenvuo
M Genetic and environmental determinants of use and abuse of alcohol: the Finnish twin cohort studies.
Alcohol Alcohol Suppl. 1991;1131- 136
Google Scholar 33.Heath
ACBerg
KEaves
LJSolaas
MHCorey
LASunder
JMagnus
PNance
WE Education policy and the heritability of educational attainment.
Nature. 1985;314734- 736
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Lichtenstein
PPedersen
NLMcClearn
GE The origins of individual differences in occupational status and educational level.
Scand Sociol Assoc. 1992;3513- 31
Google Scholar 35.Kendler
KSPrescott
CANeale
MCPedersen
NL Temperance board registration for alcohol abuse in a national sample of Swedish male twins, born 1902 to 1949.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54178- 184
Google ScholarCrossref 36.Kendler
KSNeale
MCSullivan
PFCorey
LAGardner
COPrescott
CA A population-based twin study in women of smoking initiation and nicotine dependence.
Psychol Med. 1999;29299- 308
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Kendler
KS Overview: a current perspective on twin studies of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1983;1401413- 1425
Google Scholar 38.Morris-Yates
AAndrews
GHowie
PHenderson
S Twins: a test of the equal environments assumption.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1990;81322- 326
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Heath
ACJardine
RMartin
NG Interactive effects of genotype and social environment on alcohol consumption in female twins.
J Stud Alcohol. 1989;5038- 48
Google Scholar 40.Clifford
CAHopper
JLFulker
DMurray
RM A genetic and environmental analysis of a twin family study of alcohol use, anxiety, and depression.
Genet Epidemiol. 1984;163- 79
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Kaprio
JKoskenvuo
MRose
RJ Change in cohabitation and intrapair similarity of monozygotic (MZ) cotwins for alcohol use, extraversion, and neuroticism.
Behav Genet. 1990;20265- 276
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Pedersen
NLMcClearn
GEPlomin
RNesselroade
JR Effects of early rearing enviromnent on twin similarity in the last half of the life span.
Br J Dev Psychol. 1992;10255- 267
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Sabol
SZNelson
MLFisher
CGunzerath
LBrody
CLHu
SSirota
LAMarcus
SGreenberg
BDLucas
FR
IVBenjamin
JMurphy
DLHamer
DH A genetic association for cigarette smoking behavior.
Health Psychol. 1999;187- 13
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Straub
RESullivan
PFMa
YMyakishev
MVHarris-Kerr
CWormley
BKadambi
BSadek
HSilverman
MAWebb
BTNeale
MCBulik
CMJoyce
PRKendler
KS Susceptibility genes for nicotine dependence: a genome scan and followup in an independent sample suggest that regions on chromosomes 2, 4, 10, 16, 17 and 18 merit further study.
Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4129- 144
Google ScholarCrossref