Background
Patients with schizophrenia show attention deficits. The frontal P2a and posterior N2b event-related potential components are early indices of activity in neural systems supporting attention and they are reduced in schizophrenia in auditory tasks. However, the auditory P300 is reduced as well. Thus, the P2a and N2b reductions may simply reflect a general event-related potential amplitude reduction. The visual P300, however, is often spared in schizophrenia. If neural systems supporting attention are specifically disrupted in schizophrenia, the attention-sensitive P2a and N2b should be differentially reduced in patients, compared with the P300, in a visual attention task.
Methods
We analyzed 64-channel event-related potentials from 14 schizophrenic patients and 14 control subjects in a visual object–spatial attention task. We examined the amplitude of the P2a, N2b, and P300 components in the target minus standard difference wave to see if there was a differential reduction of the P2a and N2b compared with the P300.
Results
Both the P2a and N2b waveforms were reduced in the patient group (81%[control mean, 1.99 µV; patient mean, 0.38 µV] and 95% [control mean, 0.55 µV; patient mean, 0.03 µV], respectively) while the P300 was not reduced. Measured at the peak of the frontal P2a, the N2b was larger dorsally in the spatial task and larger ventrally in the object task in the control group.
Conclusions
The spatial distribution of the P2a and N2b was consistent with activity in the prefrontal cortex and modality-specific posterior cortex, respectively. The differential reduction of the P2a and N2b waveforms supports the hypothesis of specific disruption in neural systems of visual attention in schizophrenia.
DISRUPTION OF attention and working memory are characteristic of patients with schizophrenia.1-4 Attention allocates resources to task-relevant perceptual representations. The ability to attend to and process task-relevant stimuli is critical for generating goal-directed behavior. When attention is disrupted, the ability to generate goal-directed behavior is impaired. Understanding of the neural bases of attention and working memory disruption in schizophrenia might provide insight into the neuropathologic factors of the disorder.
Primate studies suggest that prefrontal cortex and its interactions with perceptual areas in posterior cortex are important for attention and working memory.5-8 For visual stimuli, perceptual processing is divided into 2 streams: the dorsal"where" spatial location stream, projecting to the posterior parietal cortex, and the ventral "what" object feature stream, projecting to the inferior temporal cortex.9,10 Interaction between the posterior parietal cortex and the prefrontal cortex is necessary for visuospatial working memory8 and similar interaction between the inferior temporal cortex and the prefrontal cortex is needed in visual object working memory.11 If the prefrontal cortex and its connections with the posterior brain support attention and working memory, then disruptions in these neural networks may underlie those cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia.12
Event-related potentials (ERPs) have been used to study the neural systems supporting attention. Early studies focused on the P300 component, a large, centroparietal positive component, occurring approximately 400 milliseconds after the presentation of a stimulus.13-15 However, since, in some cases, the P300 can peak after the execution of the behavioral response,16 it cannot be a direct index of the allocation of processing resources to relevant stimuli.
There are more specific, earlier posterior negative ERP indices of attention, including the N2b.17,18 An N2b has been identified to task-relevant stimuli in auditory,19,20 somatosensory,21,22 and visual modalities.20,23 The N2b usually overlies and has been associated with activity in the cortical areas responsible for perceptual processing.24-26
Some studies report a positivity over frontal sites at about the same latency as the N2b.25-29 The frontal positivity, referred to here as the P2a, is due to different neural generators than the N2b, demonstrated by differences in latency, laterality, and psychological responsiveness.27-30 While the N2b is modulated both by stimulus properties and task demands, the P2a is sensitive only to the task relevance of the stimulus.29 Thus, the P2a and N2b may provide an index of interaction between posterior stimulus representation areas and the frontal executive in the processing of task-relevant stimuli.
The most commonly reported ERP finding in schizophrenia is an amplitude reduction of the auditory P300.31-33 Some studies have shown a left-sided lateralization of this reduction and this has been linked to structural abnormalities in the left temporal lobe.34 The visual P300 seems to be less affected in schizophrenia, with most studies showing either no reduction or less reduction of the visual than the auditory P300.31,32 There have been a few reports of auditory N2 reduction in schizophrenia.30,35,36 To our knowledge, there are only 2 reports of visual N2 reduction in schizophrenia, both of which found the reduction despite a P300 of normal amplitude.35,37 The single report, by our group, of the auditory frontal P2a in schizophrenia, found it reduced.30 We know of no studies of the visual P2a or related components in schizophrenia. Thus, it is unclear if there is a specific disruption of the neural networks supporting visual attention in schizophrenia, or whether the disruption is confined to the auditory modality.
This study used a visual selective attention task, where targets were defined either by their spatial location or their visual object features. This task has been shown to elicit a P2a and N2b with a spatiotemporal distribution consistent with interaction between prefrontal cortex and the dorsal where pathway in location selection and the ventral what pathway in object selection.26 We compared the ERPs from a 64-channel recording array, which provided the spatial resolution needed to assess activity in specific processing pathways. Reduction of the frontal P2a and posterior N2b in the patients, despite sparing of the P300, would provide evidence of specific disruption of the neural systems supporting visual attention in schizophrenia.
The patients (n = 21) were recruited from outpatient treatment and inpatient wards at the Brockton Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Brockton, Mass. Diagnoses were made from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) under the supervision of a licensed clinical psychologist trained in SCID administration (κ interrater reliability in our laboratory has been 0.99 for schizophrenic vs other diagnoses over the last 5 years). Subjects were between the ages of 19 and 58 years, had no history of electroconvulsive treatment or neurological illness, no alcohol or other drug abuse in the last5 years or a lifetime history of addiction, no alcohol use 24 hours prior to testing, and the desire to participate as evidenced by giving written informed consent. Control subjects (n = 22) were recruited by newspaper advertisement using the same exclusion criteria with the addition of no lifetime history of mental illness.
Subjects who performed at less than 90% accuracy in any task or who had fewer than 20 artifact-free electroencephalographic (EEG) trials in any condition were excluded, leaving 14 patients and 18 control subjects. An additional4 controls were excluded to make equal-subpopulation (n) groups, selected such that the groups were not significantly different for age. The groups did differ on verbal IQ, parental socioeconomic status, and years of education(Table 1). One control and 2 patients were left-handed; all were male. The diagnostic conditions of the patients were as follows: 6 were paranoid, 4 undifferentiated, 2 residual, and 2 schizoaffective. All patients were medicated at the time of testing, 7 with typical antipsychotic medications, 6 with atypical antipsychotic medications, and 1 with a combination therapy. Seven of the patients were also taking anticholinergic medication.
Stimuli were presented using a personal computer (Macintosh Centris650; Apple Computer, Cupertino, Calif) running PsyScope software40 communicating with the EEG amplifiers via a Button Box (New Micros, Dallas, Tex). During the experiment, there was a fixation dot at the center of the screen and 4 boxes at the 4 corners of an invisible square. Each box subtended approximately 2° of visual angle and was approximately 6° from fixation. On each trial, 1 of 4 objects would appear in 1 of the boxes and remain onscreen for 150 milliseconds with a 1250-millisecond intertrial interval. The 4 objects were an "X," a "T," a "check mark," and a "triangle" (96 pixels per object). The object and location on any given trial were equiprobable and selected randomly without replacement from a pool of trial types (Figure 1).
Subjects performed 2 blocked target detection tasks: selection by location and selection by object. In the location task, 1 of the 4 boxes was designated the target location. Subjects were instructed to press a key whenever any object appeared at the target location. In the object task, 1 of the 4 objects was designated the target. Subjects were instructed to press a key any time the target object appeared at any location. The target objects and target locations were randomly selected for each subject. Halfway through each run, a new location and object were selected as targets, constrained such that each subject had a target location in each visual field. Each subject participated in 4 task blocks of 200 trials each, 2 location and 2 object blocks, with a break every 100 trials. Task order and response hand were counterbalanced across subjects.
Eeg recording and processing
The EEG was referenced to the vertex and sampled at 250 Hz using 2 linked32-channel EEG amplifiers (SynAmps; Neuroscan Labs, Herndon, Va) and 64-channel electrode nets (Geodesic Sensor Nets; Electrical Geodesics Inc, Eugene, Ore). Epochs were 1000 milliseconds, including a 200-millisecond prestimulus baseline. The epochs were scanned by an artifact detection algorithm and trials with eye blinks or out-of-range data (0 ± 75 µV) excluded from further analysis. The EEG was digitally low-pass filtered at 30 Hz to eliminate high-frequency noise. The EEG epochs were averaged by stimulus type (nontarget, target), visual field (left, right), and task block (spatial, object) to create the ERP waveforms, then transformed into an average reference representation to attenuate spatial distortions due to choice of reference sensor.41 Grand average waveforms were created by averaging together the individual subject averages for each group (schizophrenic, control) in each condition. Since the psychological operation of interest was the detection of the task-relevant targets, the ERP components in common to both the targets and nontargets were removed by creating target minus nontarget difference waves. Target and nontarget waveforms comparing the spatial and object tasks for the control and patient groups at the frontal, dorsal, and ventral regions of interest (ROIs) are shown in Figure 2. Target minus nontarget waveforms are shown in Figure 3.
Temporal windows were selected around the P2a, N2b, and P300 peaks in the difference wave by inspection of the waveforms and a peak latency analysis was performed. Where significant effects on latency were found between groups or conditions, latency adjustments were made to the windows and the mean amplitude was extracted (latencies below). To reduce the dimensionality of an electrode factor, ROIs were identified containing subsets of the electrodes.42,43 These ROIs were frontal (bilateral electrode pairs 2-59, 4-62, and 5-58) for the P2a; dorsal (8-34, 13-46, and61-49) and ventral (20-38, 26-37, and 27-32) for the N2b; and centroparietal(8-34, 20-38, 24-33) and temporal (15-47, 16-45, 18-43, and 23-42) for the P300. The frontal, dorsal, and ventral ROIs are shown on a map of the electrode locations in Figure 4. Six independent repeated-measures analysis of variances (ANOVAs) were performed, a peak latency and a mean amplitude analysis for each component window, with the within factors task (spatial, object), visual field (left, right), and hemisphere (left, right), plus an ROI factor (dorsal, ventral) in the N2b analysis, and a between factor group (control, schizophrenic). A seventh ANOVA was performed on the amplitude only at the temporal lobe ROI to test for laterality differences as seen in the auditory P300 with the same electrode array.30 An ANOVA was also performed on the reaction time data. Correlations were computed between the frontal P2a and the N2b at the 2 posterior ROIs and probabilities computed with the Fisher r-to-z test. Tests for differences between groups on demographic variables were performed using 2-tailed t tests. An α-level of .05 was used for all comparisons.
The groups differed on reaction time, F1,23 = 19.32, P<.001, with the patient group (mean [SD], 593.99 [92.32] milliseconds) about 120 milliseconds slower than the control group (mean,471.41 [83.04] milliseconds). There was also a significant difference in reaction time between the tasks, F1,23 = 89.95, P<.001, with the reaction time in the spatial task (mean, 488.43 [84.05] milliseconds) about 93 milliseconds faster than in the object task (mean, 580.91 [108.79] milliseconds). Mean accuracy was 99% (792/800) for controls in the spatial task and 97% (776/800) in the object task; the patients were 97% (775/800) in the spatial task and 94% (750/800) in the object task.
Electrophysiological measures
Effects for task and group were used for latency correction. The latency window for the P2a and N2b was from 160 to 400 milliseconds For the P2a there was a main effect for task, F = 46.93, P<.001, with the spatial task peak (mean, 263.26 [73.12] milliseconds) about 51 milliseconds earlier than the object task peak (mean, 313.96 [60.19] milliseconds). There was an effect for task on the N2b, F = 81.37, P<.001, showing a faster latency in the spatial task (mean, 248.82 [56.360] milliseconds) than in the object task (mean, 296.67 [68.16] milliseconds). The latency window for the P300 was from 320 to 600 milliseconds. There was an effect for task, F = 78.43, P<.001, showing shorter peak latency in the spatial task (mean, 455.81 [69.64] milliseconds) than in the object task (mean, 520.12 [63.13] milliseconds). There was also an effect for group, F = 5.10, P = .03, showing a faster latency in the control group (mean, 469.26 [75.66] milliseconds) than in the patient group(mean, 506.67 [66.95] milliseconds).
Figure 5 shows ERP from a midline electrode between Cz and Pz (electrode 21) comparing the patient and control groups with the amplitude windows for the P2a/N2b and P300. The latency corrected amplitude windows for the P2a and N2b were 160 to 300 milliseconds for the spatial task and 228 to 368 milliseconds for the object task. There was an effect on the P2a for group, F = 34.25, P<.001, showing reduced amplitude in the patient group. For the N2b, there was a main effect for group, F1,26 = 10.13, P = .004, showing a smaller amplitude in the patient group and an effect for hemisphere, F = 31.97, P<.001, which was modified by group, F = 26.89, P<.001, showing a larger amplitude over the left hemisphere in the controls only. Task × ROI × Group was significant, F = 14.22, P<.001, showing a larger N2b in the ventral path in the object task and a larger N2b in the dorsal path in the spatial task in the control group (Figure 6). Task × ROI × Field was significant, F = 7.67, P = .01, as were the Task × ROI × Field × Group, F = 6.63, P = .016, and Task × ROI × Hemisphere × Group, F = 4.77, P = .04 interactions, indicating that the larger dorsal N2b in the spatial task and ventral N2b in the object task in the control group was mostly for stimuli in the left visual field and over the left hemisphere.
The P300 amplitude window was latency corrected for differences in group and task. For the spatial task the window was from 360 to 520 milliseconds for the controls and 392 to 552 milliseconds for the patients; for the object task the window was from 416 to 576 milliseconds for the controls and 464 to 624 milliseconds for the patients. In the centroparietal ROI there was an effect for task, F = 6.10, P = .02, showing a larger P300 in the spatial task. The Field × Hemisphere × Group interaction was significant, F = 7.29, P = .01, suggesting a larger P300 contralateral to the visual field of the stimulus for the control subjects. In the temporal lobe ROI, there was a Field × Hemisphere interaction, F = 5.00, P = .03, showing the P300 larger contralateral to the visual field of the stimulus. Task × Hemisphere × Group interaction was significant, F = 6.30, P = .02, indicating that, for the controls, the P300 in the spatial task was larger over the left hemisphere and in the object task was larger over the right hemisphere.
For the controls, the strongest correlations were between the frontal and ventral ROIs in both tasks (object task: r = −0.34, P<.001; spatial task: r = −0.39, P<.001). However, the P2a was significantly correlated with the dorsal N2b only in the spatial task, r = −0.23, P = .003; in the object task the correlation only approached significance, r = −0.15, P = .06. In the patients, the correlation was actually stronger between the frontal P2a and ventral N2b in the object task that for the controls, r = −0.48, P<.001. However, for the patients, the correlation between the P2a and ventral N2b in the spatial task was reduced compared with the controls, r = −0.22, P = .005, and the correlation between the P2a and dorsal N2b was not significant in the spatial task, r = −0.12, P = .11.
In this visual attention study, the patients with schizophrenia showed a reduced P2a and a reduced N2b, despite a P300 of normal amplitude and spatial distribution. This suggests that the neural systems and cognitive operations indexed by the P2a and the N2b were differentially affected in the patient group.
Attention requires the direction of processing resources to task-relevant perceptual representations. Prior research indicates that the N2b indexes the formation of a perceptual representation in modality specific areas of posterior cortex.44 The P2a, with its inferior prefrontal distribution and enhancement to target stimuli, may index activity in orbitofrontal cortical areas of task-relevance computation or salience evaluation.26 One of the functions of orbitofrontal cortex appears to be to evaluate the motivational value of a stimulus, independent of its physical features.45 The topographic distribution and psychological responsiveness of the P2a and N2b are consistent with simultaneous activity in posterior cortical areas of perceptual representation and prefrontal cortical areas of salience evaluation.26,29 For the controls, at the peak of the P2a, the N2b was larger at the ventral ROI when target detection was based upon object feature and larger at the dorsal ROI when target detection was based upon stimulus location (Figure 3 and Figure 6). This relationship was not true for the patients, indicating a disruption in the visual attention network between prefrontal cortex and stimulus-specific posterior cortex. The patients were able to perform the task, thus, there had to be some level of activity in the network. However, if the activity was sufficiently degraded, it might be unable to generate a scalp detectable P2a and N2b. In a more difficult task, the performance of the patients might start to decline more precipitously than that of the controls as the network became more challenged.
From the data presented here it is impossible to determine if the location of the physiological dysfunction is in one of the contributing cortical areas(prefrontal cortex or multiple posterior areas) or in the connections between the areas. There is substantial evidence that structural and functional abnormality in the frontal brain contributes to cognitive disruption in schizophrenia.46-52 There are also hypotheses that posit functional disconnections in the brain in schizophrenia,53-55 one of which proposes a disruption in communication between prefrontal and posterior cortex.56,57 There is less evidence of distributed posterior dysfunction. The correlation analysis here is ambiguous, suggesting disrupted communication in the spatial task but not in the object task, consistent with behavioral data suggesting differential disruption in the dorsal pathway in schizophrenia.58
This study used a heterogenous group of long-term, medicated patients, and there were significant differences between the control and patient groups. The ERP effects might be medication induced, although a prior study found visual N2 reduction in unmedicated patients.50,59 Despite these limitations, the large and differential degradation of the P2a and N2b in the patient group provides evidence of specific disruption of the neural system supporting detection of task relevant visual stimuli in schizophrenia.
Submitted for publication October 4, 2000; final revision received September7, 2001; accepted October 1, 2001.
This study was supported by a young investigator's award from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, Great Neck, NY (Dr Potts). Additional support was provided by grant MH40799-09 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr McCarley) and by the Veterans Administration through the Research Center for Basic and Clinical Neuroscience Studies of Schizophrenia, Washington, DC (Dr McCarley).
Corresponding author and reprints: Geoffrey F. Potts, PhD, Psychology, MS-25, Rice University, 6100 Main St, Houston, TX 77005 (e-mail: gpotts@rice.edu).
1.Braff
DL Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia[review].
Schizophr Bull. 1993;19233- 259
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Goldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Thought disorder, working memory and attention: interrelationships and the effects of neuroleptic medications [review].
Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 1995;10399- 104
Google Scholar 3.Goldman-Rakic
PS Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: the relevance of working memory. Carroll
BJBarrett
JEeds.
Psychopathology and the Brain. New York, NY Raven Press1991;1- 24
Google Scholar 4.Nuechterlein
KHDawson
MEGreen
MF Information-processing abnormalities as neuropsychological vulnerability indicators for schizophrenia [review].
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1994;38471- 79
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Desimone
RMiller
EKChelazzi
L The interaction of neural systems for attention and memory. Koch
CDavis
JLeds.
Large-Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain: Computational Neuroscience. Cambridge, Mass MIT Press1994;75- 91
Google Scholar 6.Fuster
JM The Prefrontal Cortex. New York, NY Raven1989;
8.Goldman-Rakic
PS Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex.
Annu Rev Neurosci. 1988;11137- 156
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Ungerleider
LGMishkin
M Two cortical visual systems. Ingle
DJGoodale
MAMansfield
RJW eds.
Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, Mass MIT Press1982;549- 586
Google Scholar 10.Ungerleider
LGHaxby
JV "What" and "where" in the human brain [review].
Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4157- 165
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Wilson
FAWScalaidhe
SPGoldman-Rakic
PS Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex.
Science. 1993;2601955- 1958
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Fleming
KGoldberg
TEGold
JM Applying working memory constructs to schizophrenic cognitive impairment. David
ASCutting
JCeds.
The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia: Brain Damage, Behaviour and Cognition Series. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates Inc1994;197- 213
Google Scholar 13.Donchin
EColes
MG Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?
Behav Brain Sci. 1988;11357- 427
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Johnson
R
Jr The amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related potential: review and synthesis.
Adv Psychophysiol. 1988;369- 137
Google Scholar 15.Verleger
R Event-related potentials and cognition: a critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3.
Behav Brain Sci. 1988;11343- 427
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Kutas
MMcCarthy
GDonchin
E Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time.
Science. 1977;197792- 795
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Näätänen
R Attention and Brain Function. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates Inc1992;
18.Wijers
AAMulder
GGunter
TCSmid
HG Brain potential analysis of selective attention. Neumann
OSanders
AF eds.
Handbook of Perception and Action. Tullamore, Ireland: Academic Press; 1996:333-387. Attention 3
Google Scholar 19.Sams
MPaavilainen
PAlho
KNaatanen
R Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;62437- 448
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Simson
RVaughan
HGRitter
W The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual discrimination tasks.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1977;42528- 535
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Josiassen
RCShagass
CRoemer
RAErcegovac
DVStraumanis
JJ Somatosensory evoked potential changes with a selective attention task.
Psychophysiology. 1982;19146- 159
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Kekoni
JHaemaelaeinen
HMcCloud
VReinikainen
KNaatanen
R Is the somatosensory N250 related to deviance discrimination or conscious target detection?
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;100115- 125
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Harter
MRAine
CJ Brain mechanisms of visual selective attention. Parasuraman
RDavies
DReds.
Varieties of Attention. London, England Academic Press Inc1984;293- 321
Google Scholar 24.Luck
SJGirelli
MMcDermott
MTFord
MA Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: an ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention.
Cognit Psychol. 1997;3364- 87
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Potts
GFDien
JHartry-Speiser
ALMcDougal
LMTucker
DM Dense sensor array topography of the event-related potential to task-relevant auditory stimuli.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;106444- 456
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Potts
GFTucker
DM Frontal evaluation and posterior representation in target detection.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001;11147- 156
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Kenemans
JLKok
ASmulders
FT Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;8851- 63
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Makeig
SWesterfield
MJung
TPCovington
JTownsend
JSejnowski
TJCourchesne
E Functionally independent components of the late positive event-related potential during visual spatial attention.
J Neurosci. 1999;192665- 2680
Google Scholar 29.Potts
GFLiotti
MTucker
DMPosner
MI Frontal and inferior temporal cortical activity in visual target detection: evidence from high spatially sampled event-related potentials.
Brain Topogr. 1996;93- 14
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Potts
GFHirayasu
YO'Donnell
BFShenton
MEMcCarley
RW High-density recording and topographic analysis of the auditory oddball event-related potential in patients with schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44982- 989
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Bruder
GETenke
CERabinomicz
ETowey
JPMalastina
DAmador
XKaufman
CAGorman
JM Electrophysiological studies of brain activity in schizophrenia. Kaufman
CAGorman
JMeds.
Schizophrenia: New Directions for Clinical Research and Treatment. Larchmont, NY Mary Ann Liebert Inc1996;17- 33
Google Scholar 33.McCarley
RWO'Donnell
BFNiznikiewicz
MASalizbury
DFPotts
GFHirayasu
YNestory
PGShenton
ME Update on electrophysiology in schizophrenia.
Int Rev Psychiatry. 1997;9373- 386
Google ScholarCrossref 34.McCarley
RWShenton
MEO'Donnell
BFFaux
SFKikinis
RNestor
PGJolesz
FA Auditory P300 abnormalities and left posterior superior temporal gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50190- 197
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Ford
JMWhite
PMCsernansky
JGFaustman
WORoth
WTPfefferbaum
A ERPs in schizophrenia: effects of antipsychotic medication.
Biol Psychiatry. 1994;36153- 170
Google ScholarCrossref 36.O'Donnell
BFShenton
MEMcCarley
RWFaux
SFSmith
RSSalisbury
DFNestor
PGPollak
SDKikinis
RJolesz
FA The auditory N2 component in schizophrenia: relationship to MRI temporal lobe gray matter and to other ERP abnormalities.
Biol Psychiatry. 1993;3426- 40
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Bruder
GKayser
JTenke
CRabinowicz
EFriedman
MAmador
XSharif
ZGorman
J The time course of visuospatial processing deficits in schizophrenia: an event-related brain potential study.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107399- 411
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Kay
SRFiszbein
AOpler
LA The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1987;13261- 276
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Hollingshead
AB Two Factor Index of Social Position. New Haven, Conn Yale University Press1965;
40.Cohen
JDMacWhinney
BFlatt
MProvost
J PsyScope: a new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1993;25257- 271
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Dien
J Issues in the application of the average reference: review, critiques, and recommendations.
Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1993;25257- 271
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Curran
TTucker
DMKutas
MPosner
MI Topography of the N400: brain electrical activity reflecting semantic expectancy.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;88188- 209
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Gevins
ASmith
MELe
JLeong
HBennett
JMartin
NMcEvoy
LDu
RWhitfield
S High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;98327- 348
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Doniger
GMFoxe
JJMurray
MMHiggins
BASnodgrass
JGSchroeder
CEJavitt
DC Activation timecourse of ventral visual stream object-recognition areas: high density electrical mapping of perceptual closure processes.
J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12615- 621
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Damasio
A The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Roberts
ACRobbins
TW
et al. eds.
The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. New York, NY Oxford UniversityPress1998;36- 50
Google Scholar 46.Andreasen
NNasrallah
HADunn
VOlson
SCGrove
WMEhrhardt
JCCoffman
JACrossett
JH Structural abnormalities in the frontal system in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43136- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Curtis
VABullmore
ETBrammer
MJWright
ICWilliams
SCMorris
RGSharma
TSMurray
RMMcGuire
PK Attenuated frontal activation during a verbal fluency task in patients with schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;1551056- 1063
Google Scholar 50.Kishimoto
HYamada
KIseki
EKosaka
KOkoshi
TJ Brain imaging of affective disorders and schizophrenia [review].
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;52S212- S214
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Toone
BK Frontal lobe structural abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence from neuroimaging. Ron
MADavid
AS eds.
Disorders of Brain and Mind. New York, NY Cambridge University Press1998;35- 53
Google Scholar 52.Van Horn
JDBerman
KFWeinberger
DR Pathophysiology of schizophrenia: insights from neuroimaging. Watson
SJed.
Biology of Schizophrenia and Affective Disease. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press Inc1996;393- 419
Google Scholar 53.Weinberger
DRLipska
BK Cortical maldevelopment, anti-psychotic drugs, and schizophrenia: a search for common ground [review].
Schizophr Res. 1995;1687- 110
Google ScholarCrossref 54.Young
CEArima
KXie
JHu
LBeach
TGFalkai
PHoner
WG SNAP-25 deficit and hippocampal connectivity in schizophrenia.
Cereb Cortex. 1998;8261- 268
Google ScholarCrossref 55.Gruzelier
J Cerebral laterality and schizophrenia: a review of the interhemispheric disconnection hypothesis. Glass
A ed.
Individual Differences in Hemispheric Specialization. NATO ASI series A
Life Sciences 130 New York, NY Plenum Press1987;357- 375
Google Scholar 56.Friston
KJFrith
CD Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome [review]?
Clin Neurosci. 1995;389- 97
Google Scholar 57.Friston
KJ Schizophrenia and the disconnection hypothesis.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1999;39568- 79
Google ScholarCrossref 58.O'Donnell
BFSwearer
JMSmith
LTNestor
PGShenton
MEMcCarley
RW Selective deficits in visual perception and recognition in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153687- 692
Google Scholar