[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Successful mood induction (significant interaction between condition, task, and scale) occurred during STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) but not during STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off). No differences between STIM ON and MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation) (A, n = 12) were found. Mood induction effect (MI) = MIHappy − MISad. Memory advantage for emotional material was similarly better during STIM ON than during STIM OFF (B, n = 12). Here, emotional material was retained and recalled more easily than neutral material during STIM ON and similarly during MED. The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

Successful mood induction (significant interaction between condition, task, and scale) occurred during STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) but not during STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off). No differences between STIM ON and MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation) (A, n = 12) were found. Mood induction effect (MI) = MIHappy − MISad. Memory advantage for emotional material was similarly better during STIM ON than during STIM OFF (B, n = 12). Here, emotional material was retained and recalled more easily than neutral material during STIM ON and similarly during MED. The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

Figure 2. 
Performance of patients (n = 12) on both tasks of verbal fluency (recall of words of a certain category [standard= correct items] and of 2 alternating categories [flexibility = pairs of correct items]) as well as on visual recognition (Benton test) under the different stimulation conditions. MED indicates best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation; STIM OFF, medication off/stimulation off; and STIM ON, medication off/stimulation on.

Performance of patients (n = 12) on both tasks of verbal fluency (recall of words of a certain category [standard= correct items] and of 2 alternating categories [flexibility = pairs of correct items]) as well as on visual recognition (Benton test) under the different stimulation conditions. MED indicates best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation; STIM OFF, medication off/stimulation off; and STIM ON, medication off/stimulation on.

Figure 3. 
A strong beneficial effect of STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) was found for motor functions (n = 12): motor performances during finger tapping (A) as well as for the walking test (B) showed marked improvements during STIM ON compared with STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off). Motor functions during STIM ON were comparable with MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation). The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

A strong beneficial effect of STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) was found for motor functions (n = 12): motor performances during finger tapping (A) as well as for the walking test (B) showed marked improvements during STIM ON compared with STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off). Motor functions during STIM ON were comparable with MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation). The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

Figure 4. 
Psychiatric assessments (Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale [BPRS], Hamilton Depression Scale [HAMD]) and self-ratings(n = 12) of symptoms (Well-being Scale [Bf-S]) indicate parallel variations between conditions, with a greater degree of psychopathologic symptoms, of depression, and of impairment in well-being during STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off) compared with STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) (BPRS, HAMD, and Bf-S). Again, STIM ON and MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation) produced no differential effects. The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

Psychiatric assessments (Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale [BPRS], Hamilton Depression Scale [HAMD]) and self-ratings(n = 12) of symptoms (Well-being Scale [Bf-S]) indicate parallel variations between conditions, with a greater degree of psychopathologic symptoms, of depression, and of impairment in well-being during STIM OFF (medication off/stimulation off) compared with STIM ON (medication off/stimulation on) (BPRS, HAMD, and Bf-S). Again, STIM ON and MED (best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation) produced no differential effects. The asterisk indicates significant effects/differences.

Positive and Negative Affective Scores (PANAS) During the Mood Induction Conditions, the Deep Brain Stimulation Conditions, and the Medication Condition*
Positive and Negative Affective Scores (PANAS) During the Mood Induction Conditions, the Deep Brain Stimulation Conditions, and the Medication Condition*
1.
DeLong  MR Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin.  Trends Neurosci. 1990;13281- 285Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Lozano  AMLang  AELevy  RHutchison  WDostrovsky  J Neuronal recordings in Parkinson's disease patients with dyskinesias induced by apomorphine.  Ann Neurol. 2000;47141- 146Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Merello  MCammarota  ANouzeilles  MIBetti  OLeiguarda  R Confirmation of the antidyskinetic effect of posteroventral pallidotomy by means of an intraoperative apomorphine test.  Mov Disord. 1998;13533- 535Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Filion  MTremblay  LBedard  PJ Effects of dopamine agonists on the spontaneous activity of globus pallidus neurons in monkeys with MPTP-induced parkinsonism.  Brain Res. 1991;547152- 161Google Scholar
5.
Benazzouz  AGross  CFeger  JBoraud  TBioulac  B Reversal of rigidity and improvement in motor performance by subthalamic high-frequency stimulation in MPTP-treated monkeys.  Eur J Neurosci. 1993;1382- 389Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Bergmann  HWichmann  TDeLong  MR Reversal of experimental parkinsonism by lesions of the subthalamic nucleus.  Science. 1990;2491436- 1438.Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Boraud  TBezard  EBioulac  BGross  C High frequency stimulation of the internal Globus Pallidus (GPi) simultaneously improves parkinsonian symptoms and reduces the firing frequency of GPi neurons in the MPTP-treated monkey.  Neurosci Lett. 1996;21517- 20Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Limousin  PKrack  PPollak  PBenazzouz  AArdouin  CHoffmann  DBenabid  AL Electrical stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in advanced Parkinson's disease.  N Engl J Med. 1998;3391105- 1111Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Moro  EScerrai  MRomito  LMARoselli  RTonali  PAlbanese  A Chronic subthalamic nucleus stimulation reduces medication requirements in Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1999;5385- 90Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Rodriguez  MCGuridi  OJAlvarez  LMewes  KMacias  RVitek  JDeLong  MRObeso  JA The subthalamic nucleus and tremor in Parkinson's disease.  Mov Disord. 1998;13111- 118Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Kumar  RLozano  AMKim  YJHutchinson  WDSime  EHalket  ELang  AE Double-blind evaluation of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1998;51850- 855Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Bejjani  BPGervais  DArnulf  IPapadopoulos  SDemeret  SBonnet  AMCornu  PDamier  PAgid  Y Axial parkinsonian symptoms can be improved: the role of Levodopa and bilateral subthalamic stimulation.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68595- 600Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Bejjani  BPDamier  PArnulf  IThivard  LBonnet  AMDormont  DCornu  PPidoux  BSamson  YAgid  Y Transient acute depression induced by high-frequency deep-brain stimulation.  N Engl J Med. 1999;3401476- 1480Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Houeto  JLDamier  PBejjani  PBStaedler  CBonnet  AMArnulf  IPidoux  BDormont  DCornu  PAgid  Y Subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson disease: a multidisciplinary approach.  Arch Neurol. 2000;57461- 465Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Volkmann  JAllert  NVoges  JWeiss  PFreund  H-JSturm  V Safety and efficacy of pallidal or subthalamic nucleus stimulation in advanced PD.  Neurology. 2001;56548- 551Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Van Horn  GSchiess  MCSoukup  VM Subthalamic deep brain stimulation: neurobehavioral concerns.  Arch Neurol. 2001;581205- 1206Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Bejjani  BPDamier  PArnulf  IPapadopoulos  SBonnet  AMVidailhet  MAgid  YPidoux  BCornu  PDormont  DMarsault  C Deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease: opposite effects of stimulation in the pallidum.  Mov Disord. 1998;13969- 970Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Kumar  RLozano  AMSime  EHalket  ELang  AE Comparative effects of unilateral and bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation.  Neurology. 1999;53561- 566Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Middleton  FAStrick  PL Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits.  Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;1236- 250Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Amaral  DGInsausti  R Retrograde transport of D-[3H]-aspartate injected into the monkey amygdaloid complex.  Exp Brain Res. 1992;88375- 388Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Benazzouz  AHallett  M Mechanism of action of deep brain stimulation.  Neurology. 2000;5513- 16Google Scholar
22.
Volkmann  JSturm  VKappler  JVoges  JKoulousakis  ALehrke  RHefter  HFreund  H-J Bilateral high-frequency stimulation of the internal globus pallidus in advanced Parkinson's disease.  Ann Neurol. 1998;44953- 961Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Hamilton  M A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;2356- 62Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Overall  JEGorham  DR The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.  Psychol Rep. 1962;10799- 812Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Zerssen  DvKöller  D-M Die Befindlichkeitsskala. Parallelformen Bf-S und Bf-S'. [The well-being scale. Parallel forms Bf-S and Bf S']. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz; 1976.
26.
Schneider  FGur  RCGur  REMuenz  LR Standardized mood induction with happy and sad facial expressions.  Psychiatry Res. 1994;5119- 31Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Watson  DClark  LATellegen  A Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;541063- 1070Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Wechsler  D Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised.  San Antonio, Tex The Psychological Corp1987;
29.
Schürer-Necker  W Gedächtnis und Emotion [Memory and Emotion].  Weinheim, Germany Beltz1994;
30.
Erwin  RJGur  RCGur  RESkolnick  BMawhinney-Hee  MSmailis  J Facial emotion discrimination, I: task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;42231- 240Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Mayes  ARDaum  I How specific are the memory and other cognitive deficits caused by frontal lobe lesions? Rabbitt  Ped Methodology of Frontal and Executive Functions Hove, England Psychology Press1997;Google Scholar
32.
Benton  ABSpreen  O Benton Visual Retention Test. 5th Bern, Switzerland Huber1996;
33.
Brown  RKulik  J Flashbulb memories.  Cognition. 1977;573- 99Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Bower  GH Mood and memory.  Am Psychol. 1981;36129- 148Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Trepanier  LLKumar  RLozano  AMLang  AESaint-Cyr  A Neuropsychological outcome of GPi pallidotomy and GPi or STN deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Brain Cogn. 2000;42324- 347Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Ardouin  CPillon  BPeiffer  EBejjani  PLimousin  PDamier  PArnulf  IBenabid  ALAgid  YPollak  P Bilateral subthalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson's disease affects neither memory nor executive functions: a consecutive series of 62 patients.  Ann Neurol. 1999;46217- 223Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Jahanshahi  MArdouin  CMBrown  RGRothwell  JCObeso  JAlbanese  ARodriguez-Oroz  MCMoro  EBenabid  ALPollak  PLimousin-Dowsey  P The impact of deep brain stimulation on executive function in Parkinson's disease.  Brain. 2000;1231142- 1154Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Alegret  MJunque  CValldeoriola  FVendrell  PPilleri  MRumia  JTolosa  E Effects of bilateral subthalamic stimulation on cognitive function in Parkinson disease.  Arch Neurol. 2001;581223- 1227Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Maricle  RANutt  JGCarter  JH Mood and anxiety fluctuation in Parkinson's disease associated with levodopa infusion: preliminary findings.  Mov Disord. 1995;10329- 332Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Voges  JVolkmann  JAllert  NLehrke  RKoulousakis  A Chronic subthalamic nucleus stimulation: impact of electrode position.  Mov Disord. 2000;1559Google Scholar
41.
Schneider  FHabel  UWagner  MFranke  PSalloum  JBShah  NJToni  ISulzbach  CHönig  KMaier  WGaebel  WZilles  K Subcortical correlates of craving in early abstinent alcoholic patients.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;1581075- 1083Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Cahill  LBabinsky  RMarkowitsch  HJMcGaugh  JL The amygdala and emotional memory.  Nature. 1995;377295- 296Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Cahill  LPrins  BWeber  MMcGaugh  JL Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events.  Nature. 1994;371702- 704Google ScholarCrossref
44.
George  MSKetter  TAGill  DSHaxby  JVUngerleider  LGHerscovitch  PPost  RM Brain regions involved in recognizing facial emotion or identity: an oxygen-15 PET study.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;5384- 394Google Scholar
45.
Sprengelmeyer  RRausch  MEysel  UTPrzuntek  H Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1998;2651927- 1931Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Blair  RJMorris  JSFrith  CDPerrett  DIDolan  RJ Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger.  Brain. 1999;122883- 893Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Schneider  FGur  REMozley  LHSmith  RJMozley  PDCensits  DMAlavi  AGur  RC Mood effects on limbic blood flow correlate with emotional self-rating: a PET study with oxygen-15 labeled water.  Psychiatry Res. 1995;61265- 283Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Schneider  FGur  REAlavi  ASeligman  MEMozley  LHSmith  RJMozley  PDGur  RC Cerebral blood flow changes in limbic regions induced by unsolvable anagram tasks.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153206- 212Google Scholar
49.
Schneider  FHabel  UKessler  CSalloum  JBPosse  S Gender differences in regional cerebral activity during sadness.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;4226- 238[Published correction appears in Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;13:124]Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Jacobs  DHShuren  JBowers  DHeilman  K Emotional facial imagery, perception, and expression in Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1995;451696- 1702Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Smith  MCSmith  MKEllgring  H Spontaneous and posed facial expression in Parkinson's disease.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1962;2383- 391Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Benke  TBösch  SAndree  B A study of emotional processing in Parkinson's disease.  Brain Cogn. 1998;3836- 52Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Funkiewiez  AArdouin  CKrack  PCaputo  Evan Blercom  NFraix  VXie  JMoro  EBenabid  ALPollak  P Acute psychic effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Mov Disord. 2000;1552Google Scholar
54.
Kuhar  MJRitz  MCBoja  JW The dopamine hypothesis of the reinforcing properties of cocaine.  Trends Neurosci. 1991;14299- 302Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Limousin  PGreene  JPollak  PRothwell  JBenabid  ALFrackowiak  R Changes in cerebral activity pattern due to subthalamic nucleus or internal pallidum stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Ann Neurol. 1997;42283- 291Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Bermanzohn  PCSiris  SG Akinesia: a syndrome common to parkinsonism, retarded depression, and negative symptoms of schizophrenia.  Compr Psychiatry. 1992;33221- 232Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Swerdlow  NRKoob  GF Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania and depression: towards a unified hypothesis of cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic function.  Behav Brain Sci. 1987;10197- 245Google ScholarCrossref
Original Article
March 2003

Deep Brain Stimulation of the Subthalamic Nucleus Enhances Emotional Processing in Parkinson Disease

Author Affiliations

From the Departments of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy (Drs Schneider, Habel, and Kornischka and Ms Regel) and Neurology (Dr Freund), University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf; Department of Neurology, University of Kiel, Kiel (Dr Volkmann); and the Department of Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery, University of Cologne, Cologne (Dr Sturm), Germany.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(3):296-302. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.296
Abstract

Background  High-frequency electrical stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus is a new and highly effective therapy for complications of long-term levodopa therapy and motor symptoms in advanced Parkinson disease (PD). Clinical observations indicate additional influence on emotional behavior.

Methods  Electrical stimulation of deep brain nuclei with pulse rates above 100 Hz provokes a reversible, lesioning-like effect. Here, the effect of deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on emotional, cognitive, and motor performance in patients with PD (n = 12) was examined. The results were compared with the effects of a suprathreshold dose of levodopa intended to transiently restore striatal dopamine deficiency. Patients were tested during medication off/stimulation off (STIM OFF), medication off/stimulation on (STIM ON), and during the best motor state after taking levodopa without deep brain stimulation (MED).

Results  More positive self-reported mood and an enhanced mood induction effect as well as improvement in emotional memory during STIM ON were observed, while during STIM OFF, patients revealed reduced emotional performance. Comparable effects were revealed by STIM ON and MED. Cognitive performance was not affected by the different conditions and treatments.

Conclusions  Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus selectively enhanced affective processing and subjective well-being and seemed to be antidepressive. Levodopa and deep brain stimulation had similar effects on emotion. This finding may provide new clues about the neurobiologic bases of emotion and mood disorders, and it illustrates the important role of the basal ganglia and the dopaminergic system in emotional processing in addition to the well-known motor and cognitive functions.

THE PATHOLOGIC hallmark of Parkinson disease (PD) is the degeneration of dopaminergic cells within the substantia nigra and the subsequent dopamine depletion of the striatum. Studies in monkeys with experimental parkinsonism induced by 1-methyl-4-pheny-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine could demonstrate that the striatal dopamine deficiency leads to abnormally high firing rates and altered neuronal discharge patterns of neurons in the internal globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus (STN).1 It is assumed that the overactive internal globus pallidus, an output nucleus of the basal ganglia, causes akinesia and rigidity through excessive inhibition of thalamocortical and brainstem motor circuits. The internal globus pallidus is driven in this scheme by excitatory projections from a disinhibited STN.1 Administration of dopamine agonists alleviates motor symptoms of parkinsonism in parallel with a reduction of neuronal firing rates in the internal globus pallidus and STN.2-4 Inactivation of the STN or internal globus pallidus through lesioning or electrical high-frequency stimulation also reverses parkinsonian symptoms1,5-7 by restoring thalamocortical and brainstem motor activity downstream to the site of dopaminergic action.

These observations and the shortcoming of current medical regimes in preventing long-term adverse effects, such as motor fluctuations and dyskinesias, have led to a resurgence of interest in surgical procedures for treating advanced PD. Permanent bilateral electrical high-frequency stimulation of the STN is one therapeutic option, leading to an average of 50% to 60% improvement in motor function.8-12 Furthermore, deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the STN reduces medication requirements and diminishes motor fluctuations and dyskinesias. While impressive improvement in motor disability may result, several reports mention possible psychiatric adverse effects,13-16 such as transient apathy, depression, or anxiety. Inadvertent stimulation within the adjacent substantia nigra pars reticulata in a patient with PD, with electrodes implanted too deeply for STN stimulation, was associated with reversible symptoms of severe depression.17 In contrast, affective disinhibition with induction of involuntary laughter by stimulation within the STN was also reported.18

The basal ganglia maintain dense interconnections with wide areas of the frontal cortex in the form of anatomically segregated parallel circuits.19 The properties of basal ganglia neurons within these loops closely resemble the properties of the cortical projection neurons, eg, in their relationship to cognitive function in the prefrontal cortex or to parameters of movement in the premotor cortex. The STN has connections to the globus pallidus and substantia nigra pars reticulata, which contain distinct clusters of neurons projecting to the orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the temporal pole.19 These regions are implicated in emotional processing. On the other hand, the amygdala, heavily involved in emotion regulation, has substantial projections to ventral and caudal parts of the striatum, the putamen, and the ventral pallidum.20 Hence, a connectivity between the STN and the limbic system can certainly be inferred and may explain some of the neuropsychiatric effects of subthalamic DBS.

Because DBS mimics lesioning of the target area,21 patients with PD with implanted DBS electrodes offer a unique opportunity to investigate the contribution of the basal ganglia to higher-order behavior in humans by reversible, experimental inactivation. This study aimed at investigating the effects of bilateral STN stimulation on emotional processing, cognition, and motor functions of patients with advanced PD. The stimulation effects were furthermore compared with those of a suprathreshold dose of levodopa as a challenge test for the dopaminergic system. We hypothesized that STN stimulation should influence, besides motor functioning, emotional processing more than cognition. Medication and stimulation, however, should not differentiate in their effects.

Methods
Subjects

Twelve patients with advanced PD (9 women, 3 men; age, mean ± SD, 62.1 ± 6.1 years; duration of illness, 17.0 ± 6.3 years) gave informed consent to participate. The study protocol had been approved by the institutional review board of the Heinrich-Heine-University Medical School (Düsseldorf, Germany) in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The mean ± SD age at illness onset was 44.9 ± 3.9 years, and patients had received medication for 14.4 ± 6.5 years. They had undergone bilateral surgical implantation of stimulating electrodes into the STN for the treatment of severe motor fluctuations and/or dyskinesias a mean ±SD of 9.3 ± 6.9 months (range, 3-24 months) prior to the experiment and were in a stable therapeutic condition. The diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder(DSM-IV) represented an exclusion criterion for surgery as well as for the study. Hence, study patients exhibited no clinically relevant psychiatric symptoms. Patients were clinically assessed before and at regular intervals after surgery according to modified protocol of the core assessment program for intracerebral transplantation, as described elsewhere.8 Part of this assessment is a standardized motor examination using the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) after overnight withdrawal of all antiparkinsonian drugs (MED OFF) and after administration of a suprathreshold dose of levodopa during the best motor "on" state (MED ON). Both states mark the range of motor symptoms that a patient with advanced PD may exhibit during levodopa-induced motor fluctuations. The clinical efficacy of bilateral STN stimulation in our patients was reflected by an average (mean ± SD, 70.5% ± 22.0%) reduction of the UPDRS score in the MED OFF condition compared with the presurgical examination (UPDRS score before surgery, mean ± SD, 51.33 ± 15.32; after surgery, 15.36 ± 9.88). This motor benefit allowed a reduced average (mean ± SD) levodopa equivalent daily dose22 from 817 ± 224 mg before to 330 ± 207 mg after surgery.

Procedures

Following overnight withdrawal from medication, patients were tested during medication off/stimulation off (STIM OFF), a medication off/stimulation on (STIM ON) condition, and as a control for similar effects of stimulation and medication, MED (mean ± SD levodopa dose, 152.1 ± 40.5 mg). Levodopa dose was chosen according to the individual amount necessary to induce an optimal motor response during preoperative clinical testing. The order of conditions was randomized except for MED, which always followed last. During the experimental sessions in the mornings, the stimulator was set to the starting condition according to the randomization protocol. Patients were not informed about the state of stimulation. The first experimental session was initiated after an instruction (15-30 minutes). Following neurologic examination, and taking into account the "best" medication and stimulation response based on prior clinical testing of patients, the next experimental session began after changing the status of stimulation. Usually, a waiting period of 15 to 45 minutes was necessary to achieve a stable treatment response after each change. The average time between conditions was therefore about 2 hours, since the application of all tests, including psychopathologic ratings, took about 1.30 hours per condition. Each investigation was performed within one testing session on the same day. For the conditions, standardized parallel test versions were used for emotion discrimination, emotional memory, the Benton test, and the verbal fluency task. The mood induction and the motor tests did not require parallel test versions.

Psychopathologic assessment

Expert ratings were based on established psychiatric rating scales and a clinical interview using a 2-way communicating system to blind the psychiatrist to the treatment conditions. The psychiatrist and patient were located in different, adjacent rooms, where they had no visual contact. During the interview, they could communicate with an intercom. Ratings had to rely on this acoustic information. Depressive symptoms were measured using the 21-item version of the Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD23). The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS24) and the Well-Being Scale (Bf-S25) were used to evaluate psychosocial functioning and general psychopathologic symptoms.

Tasks
Mood Induction

For happy and sad mood induction, 40 slides of facial expressions were presented (happy or sad).26 The patients were asked to adjust their own emotional state according to the perceived facial expressions. Sex differentiation using the same stimulus material served as a cognitive control condition. Subjects had to determine the sex of the actor on each slide. Immediately following each condition, subjects were asked to rate their emotional state on standardized rating scales (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule27; Emotional Self Rating26). Conditions were administered in counterbalanced order, with the control condition amid the 2 emotional conditions to minimize carry-over effects.

Emotional Memory

This task is similar to a subtest in the Wechsler Memory Scale.28 German emotional and neutral stories were used and have been evaluated previously.29 Subjects were read a short story (about 100 words) that was neutral or emotional (the order was randomized). Immediately thereafter, subjects were required to recall the story. A second free recall of the same story (delayed recall) was obtained after the performance of other tasks that had to be completed during the actual condition (15-minute delay).

Emotion Discrimination

The PENN Facial Discrimination Test26,30 was used to assess performance in emotion and age discrimination. This test contains 10 happy, 10 sad, and 20 neutral facial expressions, representing an emotional task with more cognitive loads.

Cognition

The verbal fluency test, in the version by Mayes and Daum,31 and the Benton Test (recognition version)32 were applied. The verbal fluency test contains 3 categories: word production with a certain letter, word production with a certain category, and alternating 2 categories (cognitive flexibility). Patients were instructed to produce corresponding words during 1 minute. The Benton test includes the recognition of graphic figures of increasing complexity from 6 various alternatives, after each has been presented to the subject for 10 seconds.

Motor

A finger-tapping task and a walking test were used. We counted the number of taps between 2 squares painted on a cardboard with a center to center distance of 30 cm using the right index finger during 1 minute. For the walking test, subjects had to walk a distance of 16 m. The time required and the number of steps taken were noted.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analysis was based on a priori hypotheses and included t tests and analyses of variance (ANOVAs). The level of significance was set at α = .05. Because of our hypotheses, Bonferroni corrections were not applied. Performing corrections for multiple comparisons would be unnecessarily conservative on type I errors since this is a pilot study and the sample size is relatively small (the number of recruitable patients is currently still restricted). Our hypothesis of enhanced emotion processing during STIM ON compared with STIM OFF was derived from our clinical observations of improved mood and more stable affective states in patients following surgery. Furthermore, we hypothesized that STIM ON would be equal to the medication control condition. Hence, statistical comparisons were performed between STIM ON and STIM OFF states. Comparable analyses were performed between STIM ON and MED.

The dependent measure for the subjective ratings was derived from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule scores. These scores were introduced into the ANOVA. The mood induction effect was quantified from ratings of positive and negative scores according to the procedure described earlier.26 Here, MI = MIHappy − MISad, where MI is the mood induction effect, MIHappy is the difference between positive and negative scores for the happy mood induction condition, and MISad is the same difference for the sad condition. Two 3-way ANOVAs with the repeated-measures factors condition (STIM ON, STIM OFF, and MED), task (happiness and sadness), and scale (positive and negative score) were conducted

Two 3-way ANOVAs with the repeated-measures factor condition (STIM ON, STIM OFF, and MED), task (happiness and sadness) and scale (positive and negative score) were conducted. The number of correctly recalled predefined elements of each story, the percentage of correct identifications, and the number of words generated for each category (for the categorical shifting, we calculated correct pairs) served as dependent variables. Separate t tests were used for the number of correctly recognized figures (Benton), the number of taps (tapping test), the steps for the distance divided by seconds (walking test), and global psychopathologic test scores.

Results
Mood induction

Successful mood induction was indicated by a significant interaction between task and scale (F1,11= 5.19, P =.04). Differential effects were indicated by an interaction among condition, task, and scale (F1,11= 4.81, P = .05)(Table 1). The mood induction effect was significant only during STIM ON (t11= 3.31, P = .007) (not during STIM OFF [t11= 1.10, P = .30]). As expected, no differences were found between STIM ON and MED (F1,11= 1.31, P = .27). A significant interaction was found only between task and scale (F1,11= 15.36, P = .002). Hence, the findings indicate that DBS of the STN facilitates emotional experience (Figure 1A) and implies effects equal to those of levodopa.

Emotional memory

In healthy subjects, emotional material is better recalled than neutral information.33,34 Comparing recalled neutral vs emotional material during STIM ON and STIM OFF states, we found a significant interaction between condition and valence (F1,11=6.63, P = .03). This interaction was not significant for the comparison of STIM ON and MED (F1,11= 1.96, P = .19). Post hoc paired t tests were performed, decomposing the significant interaction. During STIM ON, more emotional than neutral elements were recalled (t11= 7.39, P = .001). This effect was reduced, resulting in a nonsignificant difference between recalled elements of emotional and neutral stories during STIM OFF (t11= 1.51, P = .16; Figure 1B). During STIM ON, more emotional items were recalled than during STIM OFF (t11= 2.53, P = .03). For delayed recall, the comparison of recalled neutral vs emotional elements during STIM ON and STIM OFF states yielded no significant interaction between condition and valence (F1,11= 2.67, P = .13). The same applied to the comparison of STIM ON and MED (F1,11= 0.80, P = .39).

Emotion Discrimination

There were no significant stimulation effects, as revealed in the results. The 2-way ANOVAs yielded no significant meaningful interactions (F1,11= 1.40, P = .26) (ON vs MED: F1,11=1.10, P = .32).

Cognition

The ANOVA for the verbal fluency task revealed no meaningful significant interaction between condition and task (F2,20= 1.66, P = .23) (Figure 2A). However, a main effect for task emerged (F2,20= 24.53, P = .001) due to higher word production in a certain category and higher word production with a certain letter, compared with the pairs counted during categorical shifting. The equivalent ANOVA for MED and STIM ON yielded only a comparable main effect for task (F2,22= 43.48, P = .001) and no significant interaction (F2,22= 0.46, P = .56). Also, no significant effects emerged for the number of items correctly recognized in the Benton test during STIM ON and STIM OFF (t11= 1.20, P = .26). Furthermore, no differences were found between STIM ON and MED (t11= 0.53, P =.61).

Motor

Results demonstrated a strong improvement in STIM ON, visible by the walking test (steps per second) (t11=3.61, P = .004) as well as by finger tapping (t11= 6.58, P<.001)(Figure 3). In contrast, STIM ON and MED did not differ in their effects on walking (t11= 1.53, P = .15) or tapping (t11= 1.19, P = .26).

Psychopathology

Increased depressive symptoms (HAMD, t11= −5.59, P<.001) and general psychiatric symptoms (BPRS, t11= −4.53, P<.001) were measured during STIM OFF compared with STIM ON (Figure 4). The same applied to self ratings of well-being (Bf-S, t9= −4.13, P = .003). As expected, MED and STIM ON did not differ in the psychopathologic ratings (HAMD: t11= −1.48, P = .17; BPRS: t11= −1.12, P =.29) or self ratings (Bf-S: t9= −1.58, P = .15).

Comment

Deep brain stimulation of the STN was effective on 2 fronts simultaneously: it demonstrated enhanced emotional processing as well as impressive improvements in motor function. Similar to previous reports, the motor effect of STN-stimulation in the tapping and walking tasks was approximately equivalent to the best levodopa response.8-12,35 No adverse effects of DBS or levodopa on cognitive functions were observed. Our brief test battery, focusing on frontal executive functions, had been chosen according to preceding reports of subtle changes in verbal fluency after DBS of the STN without significant effects in other cognitive domains.36 However, the negative finding in our study is corroborated by Jahanshani et al,37 who subjected STN-stimulated PD patients to a more difficult version of the verbal fluency test that demanded increased cognitive loads on flexibility (categorical shifting); neither found significant stimulation effects. Our results support the general view, that DBS of the STN is relatively safe concerning cognitive adverse effects,38 but does not completely exclude subclinical abnormalities in more detailed cognitive testing.

In our study, STN stimulation appeared to have an antidepressive effect, similar to or even stronger than the previously described thymoleptic effect of levodopa during "motor off" periods.39 This is surprising in the light of several reports describing increased apathy, depression, or anxiety as transient psychiatric adverse events after subthalamic DBS.15,16 These neuropsychiatric symptoms occurred mostly during the first postoperative weeks, when extensive reduction of dopaminergic medication took place. A withdrawal syndrome is therefore an obvious explanation, also because an increase in dopaminergic medication is most effective in alleviating these symptoms.15 We cannot exclude that in some patients with suboptimally placed electrodes in the subthalamic area, neuropsychiatric adverse events result from unintended costimulation of neighboring nuclei or fiber tracts. In this study, patients were selected because of their excellent motor response to subthalamic DBS as a physiologic verification of correct electrode placement. These electrodes are most often located in the dorsal border of the STN, adjacent to the zona incerta.40 The mood-enhancing effect of subthalamic DBS was accompanied by intensified emotional experience, improved processing of emotionally laden information, and enhanced self-reported well-being. It is a well-known phenomenon that the emotional content of material enhances retention and facilitates learning.33,34,41 This effect is lost, however, after selective amygdala damage42 and can be blocked by β-adrenergic antagonists.43 During the parkinsonian "off" state that our patients experienced in the STIM OFF condition, a similar blocking effect was observed.

The emotion discrimination performance, in contrast, was not differentially affected by STN stimulation. Emotion discrimination is, rather, a cognitive demand that does not entail strong emotional participation. During mood induction and emotional memory, emotional processing can be characterized by behavioral consequences (memory advantage of emotional material and mood changes) that are not observed during emotion discrimination. Hence, this categorization task may involve brain networks other than those involved in the sole experience of emotion or emotional memory processes.44 The recognition of different basic emotions may, however, rely on various neural substrates.45,46 Mood experience during mood induction may generally require more subcortical regions to participate.47-49

Impaired emotional processing in nondepressed PD patients compared with healthy subjects has been reported.50 Smith et al51 described spontaneous facial expression to be selectively affected. Corroborating our results in emotion discrimination are the findings of Benke et al.52 Here, patients with normal cognitive function showed intact emotion discrimination and emotion recognition but impaired emotion expression, which is another indication that emotion discrimination may be more linked to cognitive processes than to emotion expression, experience, or memory. Correspondingly, those PD patients who demonstrated mental impairments performed poorly in all 3 tasks.

Funkiewiez et al53 recently reported the acute effects of subthalamic stimulation on mood in comparison with a levodopa challenge in a group of 50 patients with PD. Using the Addiction Research Center Inventory (developed to reveal affective changes after abuse of narcotics), they found the response profile of levodopa exposure or STN-stimulation to closely resemble cocaine- or amphetamine-induced euphoria. Both drugs are known to act through increases in synaptic dopamine level and may, therefore, alter the functional state of the limbic basal ganglia loop.54

Our findings support the hypothesis that STN stimulation may—in a similar manner to the influence on motor circuits—disinhibit nonmotor limbic circuits passing through the basal ganglia and projecting to the prefrontal cortex. An H2O15 positron emission tomography study of patients with STN stimulation showed increases in regional cerebral blood flow not only in the supplementary and primary motor cortex but also in its limbic projection areas, such as the cingulate cortex.55

Several limitations in our study design warrant cautious interpretation of the results. First, since the experimental procedure was time-consuming (approximately 6 hours per patient), medication effects could not be tested in randomized order. The long washout period would have required repeated testing on different days, which could not be realized in an outpatient setting. Second, the small sample size may have reduced the statistical power. Still, DBS is a new therapy, and the number of patients recruitable for such a study is small at this time. Third, blinding of patients to the stimulation state was impossible (in contrast with blinding of the psychiatrist) for the whole testing period since a worsening of motor function became apparent several minutes after switching off the stimulation. A couple of factors suggest, however, that the observed affective changes following levodopa or STN stimulation are unlikely to simply reflect a reaction to improved motor function: (1) patients were highly motivated and readily accepted a transient worsening of motor symptoms at the time they agreed to participate in the experiment;(2) within the short time periods in which the stimulation was turned off, a carry-over effect of prolonged stimulation prevented the full picture of off-period symptoms to develop.

Nevertheless, our results underscore the importance of the STN in the neurobiologic network of emotion processing and affect regulation. They also support the concept of a common pathophysiologic principle in neuropsychiatric and movement disorders.19,56,57 Middleton and Strick19 recently advanced the hypothesis that these disorders may share altered neuronal activity in different combinations of motor, cognitive, and limbic basal ganglia loops.

Corresponding author and reprints: Frank Schneider, MD, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Düsseldorf, Bergische Landstr. 2, 40629 Düsseldorf, Germany (e-mail: frank.schneider@uni-duesseldorf.de).

Submitted for publication April 18, 2001; final revision received November 16, 2001; accepted August 14, 2002.

References
1.
DeLong  MR Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin.  Trends Neurosci. 1990;13281- 285Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Lozano  AMLang  AELevy  RHutchison  WDostrovsky  J Neuronal recordings in Parkinson's disease patients with dyskinesias induced by apomorphine.  Ann Neurol. 2000;47141- 146Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Merello  MCammarota  ANouzeilles  MIBetti  OLeiguarda  R Confirmation of the antidyskinetic effect of posteroventral pallidotomy by means of an intraoperative apomorphine test.  Mov Disord. 1998;13533- 535Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Filion  MTremblay  LBedard  PJ Effects of dopamine agonists on the spontaneous activity of globus pallidus neurons in monkeys with MPTP-induced parkinsonism.  Brain Res. 1991;547152- 161Google Scholar
5.
Benazzouz  AGross  CFeger  JBoraud  TBioulac  B Reversal of rigidity and improvement in motor performance by subthalamic high-frequency stimulation in MPTP-treated monkeys.  Eur J Neurosci. 1993;1382- 389Google ScholarCrossref
6.
Bergmann  HWichmann  TDeLong  MR Reversal of experimental parkinsonism by lesions of the subthalamic nucleus.  Science. 1990;2491436- 1438.Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Boraud  TBezard  EBioulac  BGross  C High frequency stimulation of the internal Globus Pallidus (GPi) simultaneously improves parkinsonian symptoms and reduces the firing frequency of GPi neurons in the MPTP-treated monkey.  Neurosci Lett. 1996;21517- 20Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Limousin  PKrack  PPollak  PBenazzouz  AArdouin  CHoffmann  DBenabid  AL Electrical stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in advanced Parkinson's disease.  N Engl J Med. 1998;3391105- 1111Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Moro  EScerrai  MRomito  LMARoselli  RTonali  PAlbanese  A Chronic subthalamic nucleus stimulation reduces medication requirements in Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1999;5385- 90Google ScholarCrossref
10.
Rodriguez  MCGuridi  OJAlvarez  LMewes  KMacias  RVitek  JDeLong  MRObeso  JA The subthalamic nucleus and tremor in Parkinson's disease.  Mov Disord. 1998;13111- 118Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Kumar  RLozano  AMKim  YJHutchinson  WDSime  EHalket  ELang  AE Double-blind evaluation of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in advanced Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1998;51850- 855Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Bejjani  BPGervais  DArnulf  IPapadopoulos  SDemeret  SBonnet  AMCornu  PDamier  PAgid  Y Axial parkinsonian symptoms can be improved: the role of Levodopa and bilateral subthalamic stimulation.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68595- 600Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Bejjani  BPDamier  PArnulf  IThivard  LBonnet  AMDormont  DCornu  PPidoux  BSamson  YAgid  Y Transient acute depression induced by high-frequency deep-brain stimulation.  N Engl J Med. 1999;3401476- 1480Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Houeto  JLDamier  PBejjani  PBStaedler  CBonnet  AMArnulf  IPidoux  BDormont  DCornu  PAgid  Y Subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson disease: a multidisciplinary approach.  Arch Neurol. 2000;57461- 465Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Volkmann  JAllert  NVoges  JWeiss  PFreund  H-JSturm  V Safety and efficacy of pallidal or subthalamic nucleus stimulation in advanced PD.  Neurology. 2001;56548- 551Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Van Horn  GSchiess  MCSoukup  VM Subthalamic deep brain stimulation: neurobehavioral concerns.  Arch Neurol. 2001;581205- 1206Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Bejjani  BPDamier  PArnulf  IPapadopoulos  SBonnet  AMVidailhet  MAgid  YPidoux  BCornu  PDormont  DMarsault  C Deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease: opposite effects of stimulation in the pallidum.  Mov Disord. 1998;13969- 970Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Kumar  RLozano  AMSime  EHalket  ELang  AE Comparative effects of unilateral and bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation.  Neurology. 1999;53561- 566Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Middleton  FAStrick  PL Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits.  Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;1236- 250Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Amaral  DGInsausti  R Retrograde transport of D-[3H]-aspartate injected into the monkey amygdaloid complex.  Exp Brain Res. 1992;88375- 388Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Benazzouz  AHallett  M Mechanism of action of deep brain stimulation.  Neurology. 2000;5513- 16Google Scholar
22.
Volkmann  JSturm  VKappler  JVoges  JKoulousakis  ALehrke  RHefter  HFreund  H-J Bilateral high-frequency stimulation of the internal globus pallidus in advanced Parkinson's disease.  Ann Neurol. 1998;44953- 961Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Hamilton  M A rating scale for depression.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;2356- 62Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Overall  JEGorham  DR The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.  Psychol Rep. 1962;10799- 812Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Zerssen  DvKöller  D-M Die Befindlichkeitsskala. Parallelformen Bf-S und Bf-S'. [The well-being scale. Parallel forms Bf-S and Bf S']. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz; 1976.
26.
Schneider  FGur  RCGur  REMuenz  LR Standardized mood induction with happy and sad facial expressions.  Psychiatry Res. 1994;5119- 31Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Watson  DClark  LATellegen  A Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales.  J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;541063- 1070Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Wechsler  D Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised.  San Antonio, Tex The Psychological Corp1987;
29.
Schürer-Necker  W Gedächtnis und Emotion [Memory and Emotion].  Weinheim, Germany Beltz1994;
30.
Erwin  RJGur  RCGur  RESkolnick  BMawhinney-Hee  MSmailis  J Facial emotion discrimination, I: task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;42231- 240Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Mayes  ARDaum  I How specific are the memory and other cognitive deficits caused by frontal lobe lesions? Rabbitt  Ped Methodology of Frontal and Executive Functions Hove, England Psychology Press1997;Google Scholar
32.
Benton  ABSpreen  O Benton Visual Retention Test. 5th Bern, Switzerland Huber1996;
33.
Brown  RKulik  J Flashbulb memories.  Cognition. 1977;573- 99Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Bower  GH Mood and memory.  Am Psychol. 1981;36129- 148Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Trepanier  LLKumar  RLozano  AMLang  AESaint-Cyr  A Neuropsychological outcome of GPi pallidotomy and GPi or STN deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Brain Cogn. 2000;42324- 347Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Ardouin  CPillon  BPeiffer  EBejjani  PLimousin  PDamier  PArnulf  IBenabid  ALAgid  YPollak  P Bilateral subthalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson's disease affects neither memory nor executive functions: a consecutive series of 62 patients.  Ann Neurol. 1999;46217- 223Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Jahanshahi  MArdouin  CMBrown  RGRothwell  JCObeso  JAlbanese  ARodriguez-Oroz  MCMoro  EBenabid  ALPollak  PLimousin-Dowsey  P The impact of deep brain stimulation on executive function in Parkinson's disease.  Brain. 2000;1231142- 1154Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Alegret  MJunque  CValldeoriola  FVendrell  PPilleri  MRumia  JTolosa  E Effects of bilateral subthalamic stimulation on cognitive function in Parkinson disease.  Arch Neurol. 2001;581223- 1227Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Maricle  RANutt  JGCarter  JH Mood and anxiety fluctuation in Parkinson's disease associated with levodopa infusion: preliminary findings.  Mov Disord. 1995;10329- 332Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Voges  JVolkmann  JAllert  NLehrke  RKoulousakis  A Chronic subthalamic nucleus stimulation: impact of electrode position.  Mov Disord. 2000;1559Google Scholar
41.
Schneider  FHabel  UWagner  MFranke  PSalloum  JBShah  NJToni  ISulzbach  CHönig  KMaier  WGaebel  WZilles  K Subcortical correlates of craving in early abstinent alcoholic patients.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;1581075- 1083Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Cahill  LBabinsky  RMarkowitsch  HJMcGaugh  JL The amygdala and emotional memory.  Nature. 1995;377295- 296Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Cahill  LPrins  BWeber  MMcGaugh  JL Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events.  Nature. 1994;371702- 704Google ScholarCrossref
44.
George  MSKetter  TAGill  DSHaxby  JVUngerleider  LGHerscovitch  PPost  RM Brain regions involved in recognizing facial emotion or identity: an oxygen-15 PET study.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;5384- 394Google Scholar
45.
Sprengelmeyer  RRausch  MEysel  UTPrzuntek  H Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1998;2651927- 1931Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Blair  RJMorris  JSFrith  CDPerrett  DIDolan  RJ Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger.  Brain. 1999;122883- 893Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Schneider  FGur  REMozley  LHSmith  RJMozley  PDCensits  DMAlavi  AGur  RC Mood effects on limbic blood flow correlate with emotional self-rating: a PET study with oxygen-15 labeled water.  Psychiatry Res. 1995;61265- 283Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Schneider  FGur  REAlavi  ASeligman  MEMozley  LHSmith  RJMozley  PDGur  RC Cerebral blood flow changes in limbic regions induced by unsolvable anagram tasks.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153206- 212Google Scholar
49.
Schneider  FHabel  UKessler  CSalloum  JBPosse  S Gender differences in regional cerebral activity during sadness.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;4226- 238[Published correction appears in Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;13:124]Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Jacobs  DHShuren  JBowers  DHeilman  K Emotional facial imagery, perception, and expression in Parkinson's disease.  Neurology. 1995;451696- 1702Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Smith  MCSmith  MKEllgring  H Spontaneous and posed facial expression in Parkinson's disease.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1962;2383- 391Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Benke  TBösch  SAndree  B A study of emotional processing in Parkinson's disease.  Brain Cogn. 1998;3836- 52Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Funkiewiez  AArdouin  CKrack  PCaputo  Evan Blercom  NFraix  VXie  JMoro  EBenabid  ALPollak  P Acute psychic effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Mov Disord. 2000;1552Google Scholar
54.
Kuhar  MJRitz  MCBoja  JW The dopamine hypothesis of the reinforcing properties of cocaine.  Trends Neurosci. 1991;14299- 302Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Limousin  PGreene  JPollak  PRothwell  JBenabid  ALFrackowiak  R Changes in cerebral activity pattern due to subthalamic nucleus or internal pallidum stimulation in Parkinson's disease.  Ann Neurol. 1997;42283- 291Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Bermanzohn  PCSiris  SG Akinesia: a syndrome common to parkinsonism, retarded depression, and negative symptoms of schizophrenia.  Compr Psychiatry. 1992;33221- 232Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Swerdlow  NRKoob  GF Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania and depression: towards a unified hypothesis of cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic function.  Behav Brain Sci. 1987;10197- 245Google ScholarCrossref
×