Background
Sex differences in the risk of a particular disorder can yield important clues regarding its pathogenesis. The evidence for a sex difference in the risk of schizophrenia is inconclusive. The purpose of this study was to integrate results from the published literature and to provide a quantitative index of the male-female ratio for the incidence of schizophrenia.
Methods
The MEDLINE and PsychLIT databases were searched for English-language publications on "incidence and schizophrenia" that appeared during the period between January 1980 and September 2001. Population-based incidence studies using standard clinical diagnostic criteria were included if they reported sex-specific incidence rates. Sex-specific incidence figures were extracted directly from each study. Categorical analyses were conducted on a subset of studies that met specific methodological criteria (to minimize criterion bias, hospital bias, and age bias). Study categorization and data extraction were performed independently by 2 of us (A. A. and J.-P.S.).
Results
Log risk ratio meta-analysis was conducted using a random-effects model. The incidence risk ratios for men to develop schizophrenia relative to women were 1.42 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.30-1.56) when all studies were included in the analysis (49 effect sizes), 1.31 (95% CI, 1.13-1.51) when studies that minimized selection biases were analyzed separately (23 effect sizes), and 1.39 (95% CI, 1.15-1.68) when only high-quality studies were included(11 effect sizes). The sex difference was significantly smaller in studies with sample years before 1980 than those with sample years after 1980. No significant sex differences were reported in studies from developing countries. A final analysis, limited to studies with an age cutoff of 64 years or older(16 effect sizes), yielded a mean risk ratio of 1.32 (95% CI, 1.13-1.55).
Conclusion
This meta-analysis provides evidence for a sex difference in the risk of developing schizophrenia, as reported in the published literature from the last 2 decades.
The male sex appears in general to suffer somewhat more frequently from dementia praecox than the female.—Emil Kraepelin, 19191
DESPITE KRAEPELIN'S early assertion, it is still unclear whether schizophrenia affects one sex more often than the other. This is unfortunate because sex differences in the risk of a particular disorder can yield important clues about that disorder's pathogenesis. Sex differences allow a unidirectional interpretation; sex cannot be influenced by the disorder.2 In the case of schizophrenia, important differences between men and women have been shown regarding variables such as age of onset, premorbid functioning, symptomatologic characteristics, and course of illness.3 It is also widely acknowledged that in younger age groups, the risk is higher for men and that beyond age 40 years and perhaps even earlier, the risk is higher for women. However, the general view is that there are no sex differences in the lifetime risk of developing the disorder4 or that the evidence is inconclusive.5
It has been argued repeatedly that any findings of a male excess are confounded by several important factors including criterion bias, age bias, and hospital bias.3,6-10 The role of criterion bias is illustrated by the observation that the application of narrow criteria for schizophrenia excludes more women than men.8 The criteria according to the DSM-III,11DSM-III-R,12 and DSM-IV,13 for example, which imply that signs of the disturbance should be present continuously for at least 6 months, are narrower than those according to the International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision (ICD-9)14 or ICD-10.15 A second point of concern is the use of an age cutoff because women in older age groups are at higher risk for developing the disorder than men.8 Thus, to obtain a reliable index of the male-female incidence ratio, studies should ideally include patients of all ages. Third, studies that are limited to inpatients may also promote sampling bias. Because the male sex is associated with a less favorable course of the disorder (eg, violence and aggression are more common in men), men may be overrepresented in samples based on first hospital admissions.10 The inclusion of patients recruited by outpatient services is an important factor in the reduction of methodological artifact.
In this article, we use meta-analytic methods to integrate results from the published literature and to provide a quantitative index of the male-female ratio for schizophrenia incidence. To minimize the selection biases described previously, we selected for separate meta-analysis several studies that met specific criteria. Moreover, we conducted an extra analysis on high-quality studies that administered semistructured diagnostic interviews to large numbers of patients.
MEDLINE and PsychLIT were systematically searched for articles on "incidence and schizophrenia" published between January 1980 and September 2001. Reference lists from identified articles were reviewed. A database of all schizophrenia incidence studies published during the targeted period was also searched.
To be included in the meta-analysis, studies had to fulfill the following criteria: (1) use of standardized clinical diagnostic criteria (eg, DSM-IV or ICD-9); (2) reporting of sex-specific incidence figures; (3) provision or use of a denominator in computing incidence rates; and (4) publication in an English-language, peer-reviewed, indexed scientific journal. Meeting abstracts were not included.
For each study a male-female risk ratio (RR) was calculated. Risk ratios were preferred to odds ratios because they are more intuitive to physicians and more robust to heterogeneity. Given the optimal statistical properties associated with the log RR, we used this method of meta-analysis.16,17 Data extraction and calculation of RRs were performed independently by 2 of us (A. A. and J.-P.S.), who reached a consensus in cases of discrepancy.
From the individual study effect sizes, a variance-weighted mean log RR was computed that could be transformed back into a normal RR. Many studies lacked sufficient information for 2 × 2 contingency tables (numerators and denominators divided by sex) because they did not provide sex-specific numbers of the general population in the investigated area. Moreover, when such numbers were given, there were large differences between studies, with some referring to the total population, others to the age-adjusted population, and still others to person-years at risk. To include the same metric for all studies, we therefore estimated the study variance using the following formula:
V = 1/Nm + 1/Nf,
where Nm is the number of male cases and Nf is the number of female cases (derived from the formula for the variance of odds ratios).18 To prevent studies with very large samples from dominating the analyses, the size of those with more than 500 patients was set at 500, as suggested by Shadish and Haddock.17
A homogeneity statistic, QW, was calculated to test whether the studies within an analysis could be considered to share a common population effect size.17 A significant QW statistic indicates heterogeneity of the individual study effect sizes, which poses a threat to a reliable interpretation of the results. In several analyses, 2 groups of studies were compared using categorical analysis (studies with an age cutoff < 64 years vs those with a cutoff ≥64 years). The QB statistic (a between-category homogeneity statistic)17 was used to test whether such groups differed significantly in their mean effect sizes. This statistic has a χ2 distribution and is analogous to an F test. All analyses were carried out using the random-effects model with MetaWin 2.0 statistical software (Sunderland, Mass).
When the same study reported results based on more than 1 classification system (eg, DSM-III-R and ICD-9), those based on the system that used the broadest schizophrenia definition were selected (in this example, the ICD-9). Studies that used CATEGO criteria19 (based on the CATEGO computer program) were included. However, when these studies also reported rates based on the ICD-9, the latter were preferred because CATEGO is not technically a system of diagnostic criteria. When studies published results based on DSM-III, DSM-III-R, or DSM-IV criteria, the categories "schizophreniform disorder" and "schizoaffective disorder" were included when possible.
Seven different analyses were carried out. First, an initial analysis was performed in which all studies were included (analysis 1). Second, a separate analysis (analysis 2) was conducted on a subset of studies that met the following requirements: (1) age cutoff of 54 years or older; (2) case findings included both inpatient and outpatient services; and (3) ICD classification of schizophrenia, all subtypes (section 295.x), or CATEGO "broad schizophrenia," which includes the CATEGO classes S (schizophrenic psychosis), P (paranoid psychosis), and O(+) (other psychosis). The choice of requirement 3 was motivated by the observation that most studies used these criteria. The selection of these studies, which are relatively homogeneous in terms of diagnostic criteria, could contribute to the reduction of methodological variance. This subset of relatively unbiased studies was contrasted with those that did not fulfill these criteria ("biased" studies). Analysis 3 concerned high-quality studies that met all of the criteria from analysis 2 but also fulfilled the following requirements: (1) use of semistructured diagnostic interviews and (2) inclusion of at least 50 cases of schizophrenia.
To examine any effect of the inclusion of older subjects, an analysis was performed on all studies with an age cutoff of 64 years or older (analysis 4). In the next analysis, we evaluated the studies that used DSM-III-R and DSM-IV criteria separately (analysis 5). Studies based on DSM-III criteria were not included because these criteria demand that symptoms manifest before age 45 years. One study20 included patients with an ICD-8 or DSM-III-R diagnosis but reported sex-specific incidence rates only for the (mixed) group as a whole. Therefore, this study was omitted from analysis 5. Furthermore, we explored possible time trends for the male-female RR by comparing studies with sample years before 1980 with studies that included only sample years after 1980(analysis 6). When the range of sample years within 1 study included the years before and after 1980, we included the study in the first group (very few studies had sample years that were exclusively before 1980). When a study included multiple samples21-23 with sample years before 1980 and independent groups with sample years after 1980, data from these samples were included separately in the corresponding analysis. Finally, findings from developed and developing countries were contrasted (analysis 7). The World Health Organization human development index of countries24 was used to classify the studies according to this variable. Specifically, countries with a high development index (eg, England or Sweden) were compared with countries that had a medium development index (eg, India or Brazil).
Several articles concerned incidence studies from the same region. In cases of a complete overlap of samples, only 1 study was included in the analysis. Most samples from the same region, however, did not overlap or overlapped only to a small extent. For example, in the case of Finland, one study25 overlapped briefly with another,21 but in contrast to the latter study, the former included outpatient services and did not include an age cutoff. In such cases, both studies were included in the first, broad analysis but not in the second, more stringent analysis to avoid the problem of findings from 1 sample dominating the outcome. In the latter analysis, the study that met most of our criteria was included (in this example, the one by Salokangas.)25 To categorize studies according to the factors mentioned previously, study characteristics were independently abstracted by 2 of us (A. A. and J.-P.S.). In cases of discrepancy, a consensus was reached by means of discussion.
Thirty-eight articles8,20-23,25-57 met our criteria for inclusion in the initial analysis, yielding 49 effect sizes (several studies reported on multiple independent groups). These included register studies, prospective first-contact studies (for a defined period and location), and cohort studies.
Results of the meta-analyses are detailed in Table 1. Analysis 1, which concerned all studies (49 effect sizes), yielded a mean RR of 1.42 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.30-1.56). Analysis 2 compared studies that minimized sex-related sampling bias with those that did not. The results are listed in Table 1. The magnitude of the mean RR is somewhat smaller for the studies that minimized sampling bias than for studies that did not control for sampling bias (1.35 vs 1.48), but this difference was not significant (QB =1.0; P = .32). The studies that minimized sampling bias still show a considerable sex difference in schizophrenia incidence. Analysis 3, with the methodologically more rigorous studies (11 effect sizes), yielded a mean RR of 1.39 (95% CI, 1.15-1.68). Analysis 4, which involved studies with an age cutoff of 64 years or older, yielded a mean RR of 1.32(16 effect sizes; 95% CI, 1.13-1.55). The difference with the other studies, which had an age cutoff of younger than 64 years (32 effect sizes; mean RR, 1.50), was not significant (QB = 1.7; P =.19). When studies using DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria were analyzed separately (9 studies; analysis 5), a mean RR of 1.58 was obtained. The difference from the mean effect size of the studies using ICD criteria (30 effect sizes; mean RR, 1.45) was not statistically significant.
Table 1 also shows the results of the analysis (analysis 6) in which studies including sample years before 1980 were compared with studies that had only sample years beyond 1980. The difference in the mean RR (1.27 and 1.54, respectively) was significant (QB = 7.3; P = .007). To examine the possibility that this result was due to the application of more restrictive criteria after 1980, we carried out an additional analysis that was confined to studies using ICD criteria. The difference (mean RR, 1.32; 95% CI, 1.18-1.48; 16 effect sizes; vs mean RR, 1.55; 95% CI, 1.37-1.74; 16 effect sizes) became somewhat smaller but remained statistically significant (QB= 4.1; P = .04). Thus, the sex difference was found to be smaller in studies that included samples diagnosed before 1980.
Finally, studies from countries with a high World Health Organization human development index were compared with those that had a medium development index (analysis 7). The difference in mean RR was significant (QB =4.9; P = .03); there appeared to be no significant sex difference in schizophrenia incidence in the countries with a medium development index. The CI for these countries included 1.0 (mean RR, 1.09; 95% CI, 0.79-1.49), whereas this was not the case for countries with a high development index (mean RR, 1.48; 95% CI, 1.34-1.63).
Figure 1 plots the log RRs associated with the studies that were entered into either analysis 1, 2, or 3. Although analysis 3 yielded more negative effect sizes (ie, a female excess) for individual studies, the general pattern of a male excess is essentially the same, implying that the sex difference in incidence may be relatively insensitive to methodological factors.
The results from this systematic review of studies published during the period between January 1980 and September 2001 point to a sex difference in the risk for schizophrenia. The risk remained significantly higher in men after controlling for potentially confounding factors (ie, age bias, criterion bias, and hospital bias). A separate meta-analysis of studies that minimized selection bias used semistructured diagnostic interviews, and included 50 cases or more also revealed a significantly higher risk of schizophrenia in men. The magnitude of the male excess reported in this article is somewhat smaller than the female preponderance in Alzheimer disease, for which the mean odds ratio in a recent meta-analysis58 was estimated at 1.56.
Do these findings indicate that men are definitively at higher risk for developing schizophrenia? To enter an incidence study of schizophrenia, the patient has to contact a psychiatric service (stage 1), the treating physician has to recognize the symptoms of psychosis (stage 2), and the physician has to recognize a certain pattern indicative of schizophrenia (stage 3). Errors inducing sex bias could occur at each of these stages.
As far as stage 1 is concerned, it is uncertain whether women are more or less likely than men to seek treatment for psychotic symptoms. Ideally, large door-to-door surveys to detect all cases of schizophrenia in a given area could solve the question of whether the higher treated incidence rates for men are compensated for by large numbers of untreated women. However, the major surveys of the past 2 decades relied on structured diagnostic interviews, and the results are of questionable validity.59,60 We found no evidence that the male excess in treated incidence studies could be explained by biases occurring at stage 2 or 3. The analysis in which studies with an age cutoff of 64 years were included did not reveal a significantly smaller sex difference. The male excess remained significant when broad diagnostic criteria were applied and when the case-finding methods included inpatient and outpatient services. The lack of evidence for hospital bias as an explanation for the male excess is consistent with the results of a study from England.61 This study examined factors associated with hospital admission in 484 patients who made a first contact for nonaffective psychosis; it failed to find evidence of a sex bias.
It has been argued that schizophrenia is underdiagnosed at first hospital admission, that this bias is most pronounced in women, and that it can be reduced by the use of semistructured diagnostic interviews.6 The results of our separate meta-analysis on the subset of studies that used such interviews still showed a male preponderance in the incidence of schizophrenia. It is possible that men are more frequently misdiagnosed as having schizophrenia in the first stage of the illness and that the diagnosis is adjusted at a later stage (to bipolar disorder, for example). However, this is at odds with the findings of a large and meticulous study that did not observe sex differences in changes of diagnosis after 2 years.62
Nonetheless, biases may have occurred at stage 2 or 3. The possibility remains that some physicians do not recognize psychotic symptoms and do not refer the patient for a semistructured diagnostic interview. Physicians may also recognize these phenomena but fail to draw the conclusion that the patient has a psychotic disorder and erroneously label these symptoms as dissociative phenomena or (transient) psychotic phenomena within the framework of a borderline personality disorder. The only way to counteract this kind of bias is to examine all patients who make a first contact for any psychiatric disorder by means of diagnostic interviews.
Authors who comment on methodological pitfalls of research concerning the sex difference in incidence of schizophrenia are almost exclusively concerned with bias that favors male predominance. However, bias might obscure a higher male risk as well. Specifically, mortality due to schizophrenia is more than twice as high in men as in women,63 and it is possible that this male excess also applies to subjects who commit suicide before they contact a psychiatric service. Finally, to enhance methodological homogeneity, we focused on studies using ICD criteria. Notably, there is no evidence that ICD criteria are more valid than DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria.
When summarizing the evidence in light of potential methodological limitations, we are left with 2 possibilities, which are illustrated graphically in Figure 2. The first possibility is that the real incidence of schizophrenia is identical for men and women but that it is more readily detected in men because of greater severity of the disorder(Figure 2A). Additional factors might be that women with psychosis are less likely to seek treatment and that their symptoms go unrecognized by physicians. The second possibility is that the real incidence is indeed larger for men (Figure 2B). As indicated in Figure 2, the detection threshold, which is influenced by severity of the disorder, is an important variable that complicates the interpretation of epidemiological studies.
Evidence shows that the proportion of men with deficit schizophrenia is even higher than that of men with schizophrenia in general. A recent meta-analysis of studies of patients with deficit schizophrenia64 found a mean odds ratio of 1.75, although this review was based on experimental rather than epidemiological studies. The Roscommon Family Study,65 a population-based study on the treated prevalence of schizophrenia, found that the proportion of men in the deficit group was 91% compared with 63% in the nondeficit group.
If there is a higher incidence of schizophrenia in men, has this always been the case? Ødegaard66 estimated the lifetime risk for hospitalized schizophrenia on the basis of first admissions to Norwegian hospitals during the period from 1926 to 1935 and found a male excess of only 7%. It could be argued, therefore, that there was a slight excess during the first three quarters of the 20th century and that this excess has increased significantly since then. The results of our categorical analysis support this interpretation because RRs were higher in study samples from after 1980. Possible factors that could lead to an increase in male preponderance are the use of illicit drugs, which could precipitate schizophrenia in those who are genetically at risk,67 or the introduction of oral contraceptives because estrogens may have a protective effect in women.68 It has also been suggested that the incidence of schizophrenia is declining and that this decline is more prominent in women,69 but the evidence is inconclusive.21,23
The fact that the sex difference was not apparent in countries with a medium development index may also point to effects of illicit drugs or oral contraceptives; the use of both is less widespread in developing countries. It may imply, however, that factors associated with industrialization or Western culture play a role. Alternatively, it could be argued that psychoses other than schizophrenia may act as a confounding factor in this finding. The incidence of nonaffective acute remitting psychosis in developing countries has been shown to be about 10-fold the incidence in industrialized countries.70 In addition, the incidence of this form of psychosis is twice as great in women as in men. Given the use of ICD classification in most studies of developing countries, this form of psychosis might have been diagnosed as schizophrenia.
Several limitations have already been mentioned. Another limitation of our meta-analysis is that it did not include prevalence studies. Most studies of the treated prevalence repeat the findings of the treated incidence studies and find a male excess; the Roscommon Family Study, based on semistructured diagnostic interviews, found that the treated prevalence rate of DSM-III-R schizophrenia was 0.54% for men and 0.28% for women.71 Finally, our study may not have found a significant effect for diagnostic criteria because comparisons between criteria are more powerful when made within studies as opposed to between them (eg, Castle et al8).
This meta-analysis documents an apparent higher risk for men of developing schizophrenia, as reported in the published literature during the last 2 decades. However, given the methodological concerns, the existence of a true sex difference remains controversial. The possibility that schizophrenia may go unrecognized to a larger extent in women than in men has not been completely ruled out. We conclude that the male sex is a major risk factor for a more severe and therefore more easily recognizable form of schizophrenia.
Corresponding author and reprints: Jean-Paul Selten, MD, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, A00.241, University Medical Center, Heidelberglaan 100, NL-3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands (e-mail: j.p.selten@azu.nl).
Submitted for publication March 20, 2002; final revision received November 8, 2002; accepted November 15, 2002.
Dr Aleman was supported by Vernieuwingsimpuls Grant 016-026-027 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, The Hague.
We thank J. J. Hox, PhD, for statistical advice, and H. Schnack, PhD, for his help with Figure 2. We also thank El Saadi, MD, of the Schizophrenia Research Centre at Brisbane, Queensland, Australia, for providing the database of schizophrenia incidence studies.
1.Kraepelin
E Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Barclay
RMtrans. New York, NY Krieger1971;
2.Lewine
RRJ Gender and schizophrenia. Nasrallah
HAed
Handbook of Schizophrenia. 3 Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier1988;379- 397
Google Scholar 3.Leung
AChue
P Sex differences in schizophrenia: a review of the literature.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2000;4013- 38
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Hales
REYudofsky
SCTalbott
JA The American Psychiatric Press Textbook of Psychiatry. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1999;
5.Sadock
BJSadock
VAeds
Kaplan & Sadock's Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott Williams & Wilkins2000;
6.Hambrecht
MRiecher-Rössler
AFätkenheuer
BLouzä
MRHäfner
H Higher morbidity risk for schizophrenia in males: fact or fiction?
Compr Psychiatry. 1994;3539- 49
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Lewine
RJBurbach
DMeltzer
HY Effect of diagnostic criteria on the ratio of male to female schizophrenic patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1984;14184- 87
Google Scholar 8.Castle
DJWessely
SMurray
RM Sex and schizophrenia: effects of diagnostic stringency, and associations with premorbid variables.
Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162658- 664
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Jablensky
A The epidemiological horizon. Hirsch
SRWeinberger
DReds
Schizophrenia. Oxford, England Blackwell Publishing1995;206- 252
Google Scholar 10.Walker
EFLewine
RRJ Sampling biases in studies of gender and schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1993;191- 7
Google ScholarCrossref 11.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Third Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1980;
12.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Revised Third Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
13.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
14.World Health Organization, International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision (ICD-9). Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1977;
15.World Health Organization, International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD-10). Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1992;
16.Fleiss
JL Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York, NY Wiley1981;
17.Shadish
WRHaddock
CK Combining estimates of effect size. Cooper
HHedges
LVeds
The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York, NY Russell Sage1994;261- 280
Google Scholar 18.Lipsey
MWWilson
DB Practical Meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, Calif Sage Publications2001;
19.Wing
JKCooper
JESartorius
N Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press1974;
20.Vázquez-Barquero
JLCuesta Nuñez
MJde la Varga
MHerrera Castanedo
SGaite
LArenal
A The Cantabria first episode schizophrenia study: a summary of general findings.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1995;91156- 162
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Suvisaari
JMHaukka
JKTanskanen
AJLönnqvist
JK Decline in the incidence of schizophrenia in Finnish cohorts born from 1954 to 1965.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56733- 740
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Folnegovic
ZFolnegovic-Šmalc
VKulcar
Z The incidence of schizophrenia in Croatia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156363- 365
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Takei
NLewis
GSham
PCMurray
RM Age-period-cohort analysis of the incidence of schizophrenia in Scotland.
Psychol Med. 1996;26963- 973
Google ScholarCrossref 24.United Nations, Human Development Report 2001. New York, NY United Nations2001;
25.Salokangas
RKR First-contact rate for schizophrenia in community psychiatric care: consideration of the oestrogen hypothesis.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;242337- 346
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Iacono
WGBeiser
M Are males more likely than females to develop schizophrenia?
Am J Psychiatry. 1992;1491070- 1074
Google Scholar 27.Mahy
GEMallett
RLeff
JBhugra
D First-contact incidence rate of schizophrenia on Barbados.
Br J Psychiatry. 1999;17528- 33
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Wahlbeck
KForsén
TOsmond
CBarker
DJPEriksson
JG Association of schizophrenia with low maternal body mass index, small size at birth, and thinness during childhood.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;5848- 52
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Räsänen
SVeijola
JHakko
HJoukamaa
MIsohanni
M Gender differences in incidence and age at onset of DSM-III-R schizophrenia: preliminary results of the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort study.
Schizophr Res. 1999;37197- 198
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Van Os
JDriessen
GGunther
NDelespaul
P Neighborhood variation in incidence of schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 2000;176243- 248
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Selten
JPSlaets
JPJKahn
RS Schizophrenia in Surinamese and Dutch Antillean immigrants to the Netherlands: evidence of an increased incidence.
Psychol Med. 1997;27807- 811
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Beratis
SGabriel
JHoidas
S Gender differences in the frequency of schizophrenic subtypes in unselected hospitalized patients.
Schizophr Res. 1997;23239- 244
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Bhugra
DHilwig
MHossein
BMarceau
HNeehall
JLeff
JMallett
RDer
G First-contact incidence rates of schizophrenia in Trinidad and one-year follow-up.
Br J Psychiatry. 1996;169587- 592
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Bhugra
DLeff
JMallett
RDer
GCorridan
BRudge
S Incidence and outcome of schizophrenia in whites, African-Caribbeans and Asians in London.
Psychol Med. 1997;27791- 798
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Bresnahan
MABrown
ASSchaefer
CABegg
MDWyatt
RJSusser
ES Incidence and cumulative risk of treated schizophrenia in the prenatal determinants of schizophrenia study.
Schizophr Bull. 2000;26297- 308
Google ScholarCrossref 36.Brennan
PAMednick
SAHodgins
S Major mental disorders and criminal violence in a Danish birth cohort.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57494- 500
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Brewin
JCantwell
RDalkin
TFox
RMedley
IGlazebrook
CKwiecinski
RHarrison
G Incidence of schizophrenia in Nottingham: a comparison of two cohorts, 1978-1980 and 1992-1994.
Br J Psychiatry. 1997;171140- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 38.DeSalvia
DBarbato
ASalvo
PZadro
F Prevalence and incidence of schizophrenic disorders in Portogruaro: an Italian case register study.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1993;181275- 282
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Goldacre
MShiwach
RYeates
D Estimating incidence and prevalence of treated psychiatric disorders from routine statistics: the example of schizophrenia in Oxfordshire.
J Epidemiol Community Health. 1994;48318- 322
Google ScholarCrossref 40.Häfner
HMaurer
KLöffler
WRiecher-Rössler
A The influence of age and sex on the onset and early course of schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1993;16280- 86
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Harrison
GOwens
DHolton
ANeilson
DBoot
D A prospective study of severe mental disorder in Afro-Caribbean patients.
Psychol Med. 1988;18643- 657
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Hickling
FWRodgers-Johnson
P The incidence of first contact schizophrenia in Jamaica.
Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167193- 196
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Jablensky
ASartorius
NErnberg
GAnker
MKorten
ACooper
JEDay
RBertelsen
A Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures: a World Health Organization ten-country study.
Psychol Med Monogr Suppl. 1992;201- 97
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Jones
IHKirkby
KCHay
DADaniels
BALongmore
LM Decline in diagnoses of schizophrenia in Tasmania during the period 1965-1990.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1997;9513- 18
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Jones
PBRodgers
BMurray
RMMarmot
MG Child developmental risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort.
Lancet. 1994;3441398- 1402
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Messias
ESampaio
JJMessias
N CordeiroKirkpatrick
B Epidemiology of schizophrenia in Northeast Brazil.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 2000;188118- 120
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Mortensen
PBPedersen
CBWestergaard
TWohlfahrt
JEwald
HMors
OAndersen
PKMelbye
M Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia.
N Engl J Med. 1999;340603- 608
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Morgan
VCastle
DPage
AFazio
SGurrin
LBurton
PMontgomery
PJablensky
A Influenza epidemics and incidence of schizophrenia, affective disorders and mental retardation in Western Australia: no evidence of a major effect.
Schizophr Res. 1997;2625- 39
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Munk-Jørgensen
PMortensen
PB Incidence and other aspects of the epidemiology of schizophrenia in Denmark, 1971-87.
Br J Psychiatry. 1992;161489- 495
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Nicole
LLedage
ALalonde
P Lower incidence and increased male:female ratio in schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1992;161556- 557
Google ScholarCrossref 52.Ösby
UHammar
NBrandt
LWicks
LThinsz
ZEkbom
ASparén
P Time trends in first admissions for schizophrenia and paranoid psychosis in Stockholm County, Sweden.
Schizophr Res. 2001;47247- 254
Google ScholarCrossref 53.Rajkumar
SPadmavati
RThara
RMenon
M Sarada Incidence of schizophrenia in an urban community in Madras.
Indian J Psychiatry. 1993;3518- 21
Google Scholar 54.Ring
NTantam
DMontague
LNewby
DBlack
DMorris
J Gender differences in the incidence of definite schizophrenia and atypical psychosis: focus on negative symptoms of schizophrenia.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1991;84489- 496
Google ScholarCrossref 55.Selten
JPVeen
NFeller
WBlom
JDSchols
DCamoenie
WOolders
Jvan der Velden
MHoek
HWRivero
VMvan der Graaf
YKahn
R Incidence of psychotic disorders in immigrant groups to The Netherlands.
Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178367- 372
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Susser
ESNeugebauer
RHoek
HWBrown
ASLin
SLabovitz
DGorman
JM Schizophrenia in rural Ireland over a 50-year period.
Br J Psychiatry. 1994;164171- 176
Google ScholarCrossref 57.Waddington
JLYoussef
HA Evidence for a gender-specific decline in the rate of schizophrenia in rural Ireland over a 50-year period.
Br J Psychiatry. 1994;164171- 176
Google ScholarCrossref 58.Gao
SHendrie
HCHall
KSHui
S The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55809- 815
Google ScholarCrossref 59.Kendler
KSGallagher
TJAbelson
JMKessler
RC Lifetime prevalence, demographic risk factors, and diagnostic validity of nonaffective psychosis as assessed in a US community sample: the National Comorbidity Survey.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;531022- 1031
Google ScholarCrossref 60.Brugha
TSBebbington
PEJenkins
R A difference that matters: comparisons of structured and semi-structured psychiatric diagnostic interviews in the general population.
Psychol Med. 1999;291013- 1020
Google ScholarCrossref 61.Castle
DJPhelan
MWessely
SMurray
RM Which patients with non-affective functional psychoses are not admitted at first contact?
Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165101- 106
Google ScholarCrossref 62.Schwartz
JEFennig
SWanenberg-Karant
MCarlson
GCraig
TGalambos
NLavelle
JBromet
EJ Congruence of diagnoses 2 years after a first-admission diagnosis of psychosis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57593- 600
Google ScholarCrossref 64.Roy
MAMaziade
MLabbé
AMérette
C Male gender is associated with deficit schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.
Schizophr Res. 2001;47141- 147
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Kirkpatrick
BRoss
DEWalsh
DKarkowski
LKendler
KS Family characteristics of deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia in the Roscommon Family Study.
Schizophr Res. 2000;4557- 64
Google ScholarCrossref 66.Ødegaard
Ø A statistical investigation of the incidence of mental disorder in Norway.
Psychiatr Q. 1946;20381- 399
Google ScholarCrossref 67.Andreasson
SAllebeck
PEngstrom
ARydberg
U. Cannabis and schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts.
Lancet. 1987;21483- 1486
Google ScholarCrossref 68.Häfner
HRiecher-Rössler
AAn Der Heiden
WMaurer
KFätkenheuer
BLöffler
W Generating and testing a causal explanation of the gender difference in age at first onset of schizophrenia.
Psychol Med. 1993;23925- 940
Google ScholarCrossref 69.Scully
PJQuinn
JFMorgan
MGKinsella
AO'Callaghan
EOwens
JMWaddington
JL First-episode schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and other psychoses in a rural Irish catchment area: incidence and gender in the Cavan-Monaghan study at 5 years.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2002;43s3- s9
Google ScholarCrossref 70.Susser
EWanderling
J Epidemiology of nonaffective acute remitting psychosis vs schizophrenia: sex and sociocultural setting.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51294- 301
Google ScholarCrossref 71.Kendler
KSWalsh
D Gender and schizophrenia: results of an epidemiologically-based family study.
Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167184- 192
Google ScholarCrossref