Context
Patients with schizophrenia exhibit olfactory deficits, but it is unclear whether these represent a specific abnormality. The link between olfactory impairments and regional brain abnormalities has yet to be established.
Objectives
To determine whether patients with schizophrenia exhibit volumetric deficits in the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe, the target for neuronal inputs from the olfactory bulb, and whether these are related to olfactory performance deficits.
Design
A cohort study of patients and healthy control subjects who underwent both 1-mm spoiled-gradient echo magnetic resonance imaging and behavioral tests of olfaction and memory.
Setting
Schizophrenia Research Center at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
Participants
Fifty-two patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and 38 healthy control subjects. Individuals were excluded for history of head trauma, significant substance abuse, and medical conditions affecting brain function or olfactory capacity.
Main Outcome Measures
Gray matter volumes of the left and right temporal poles and the perirhinal and entorhinal cortexes; olfactory threshold detection sensitivity and identification test scores; composite indexes of verbal and spatial memory ability.
Results
Patients had reduced volumes, relative to cranial size, in left (P = .003) and right (P = .01) perirhinal and left (P = .002) and right (P = .002) entorhinal cortexes, but not in the temporal pole. Perirhinal, but not entorhinal, cortical volume decrement was associated with decreased olfactory threshold sensitivity. Neither region was associated with impaired memory performance.
Conclusions
Patients with schizophrenia have reduced cortical volumes in brain regions that receive afferents directly from the olfactory bulb. Behavioral olfactory deficits are related to structural brain abnormalities in these regions.
THERE IS increasing evidence that patients with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to detect and identify odors.1 These deficits are present early in the disease course and are unrelated to symptom severity, medication use,2 or smoking. However, it remains unclear whether they represent a specific impairment or merely reflect the global cognitive impairment seen in this disorder. The link between olfactory behavioral impairments and regional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia has yet to be established. A recent finding from our laboratory—that both patients and their healthy first-degree relatives have reduced olfactory bulb volumes—would suggest that structural abnormalities of the olfactory system underlie these impairments.3,4 Whether comparable abnormalities exist in the cortical areas that receive neuronal inputs directly from the olfactory bulbs, and whether these are related to olfactory performance deficits, are questions that remain unanswered.
Olfactory processing is mediated by many of the same medial temporal lobe areas of the brain that have been implicated in schizophrenia. Olfactory afferents travel via the olfactory tract to the ipsilateral anterior ventromedial temporal lobe (AVMT), where they synapse with pyramidal cells. The bulk of these afferents terminate in the piriform cortex, which is located at the rostral uncus and is thought to be responsible for initial olfactory perception. Some fibers terminate posteriorly in the entorhinal cortex (EC), the gateway to the hippocampus. Piriform cortex and EC also connect to the amygdala, thereby providing a neuroanatomic basis for the linkage of olfaction to emotion and memory, 2 processes known to be impaired in schizophrenia. Cytoarchitecturally, the AVMT may be subdivided into temporopolar cortex (TP) (Brodmann area 38), perirhinal cortex (PC) (Brodmann areas 35 and 36), and EC (Brodmann area 28),5 with the piriform cortex included as part of the PC. While the latter 2, which are considered to be part of the limbic cortex, have been implicated in olfaction and memory, the TP is generally considered to be developmentally and functionally distinct. Although it connects to the amygdala, hippocampus, and basal forebrain, it does not receive direct olfactory afferents.
Previous examinations of medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia, using volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques, have focused on the amygdala-hippocampal complex, with few studies considering the surrounding cortical areas that mediate olfaction. The principal motivation for emphasizing these 2 subcortical structures has been their link to memory and emotion processing. Memory impairments are extremely robust and selective trait abnormalities in schizophrenia6,7; emotion processing disturbances are gaining new prominence.8-11 In a recent review of the literature, Shenton and colleagues12 identified 34 (71%) of 48 studies that reported significant volume reductions in the amygdala and/or hippocampus in patients. Despite the role of the hippocampus in memory processing and the MR imaging findings supporting hippocampal abnormalities in schizophrenia, studies13-16 of structure-function relationships have generally failed to find an association between hippocampal volume reduction and memory impairment. In contrast to the large number of studies examining the hippocampus and amygdala, only 13 studies13,17-28 examined the adjacent parahippocampal gyrus, with 8 (62%) reporting reductions of this cortical gray matter area. Only 2 studies29,30 examined the EC, despite its role as a critical relay between the hippocampus and associational cortexes.31 One of these found bilateral volume reductions in patients,29 while the other reported no difference.30 Hence, it is still unclear to what extent AVMT cortical gray matter is reduced in schizophrenia. No studies, to our knowledge, have looked specifically at the cortical regions responsible for olfactory processing and their relationship to olfactory performance. Perhaps one reason for the relative dearth of such investigations is the problem of selecting appropriate MRI landmarks to guide region-of-interest (ROI) identification in this area. Boundaries between AVMT subregions are based on cytoarchitectural distinctions, rather than gross anatomic features.
In this investigation, we applied volumetric MRI methods to quantify the cortical gray matter volumes of cytoarchitecturally distinct areas of the AVMT in a large sample of patients and healthy control subjects. We used transitional landmarks derived from histologic analysis5 to parse the region into discrete TP, PC, and EC. Participants were assessed on olfactory and memory performance to investigate the relationship between cortical volume abnormalities and behavior.
The sample consisted of 52 patients (27 men, 25 women) with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and 38 healthy volunteers(21 men, 17 women). Patient age ranged from 19 to 53 years (mean ± SD, 32.3 ± 9.1 years); healthy control age ranged from 18 to 56 years (mean ± SD, 28.2 ± 9.4 years). This represented a small but significant group difference (t88 = 2.07, P = .04). There was also a difference in smoking habits: 5 of 38 controls and 19 of 52 patients were active smokers (χ21 = 6.14, P<.05). Mean numbers of packs per day were 0.13 ± 0.30 for controls and 0.38 ± 0.51 for patients(t87 = 2.73, P =.008). There was no difference between the groups in sex distribution.
Patients were consecutively referred from both outpatient and inpatient settings and received medical, neurologic, and psychiatric evaluations, including the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R –Patient Version.32,33 To ensure diagnostic accuracy, they were clinically reassessed at 6-month intervals after intake. There was no history of any disorder or event other than schizophrenia that could potentially affect brain function. All patients who met inclusion criteria and were willing and able to provide informed consent were included. Healthy volunteers were recruited by newspaper advertisement and underwent medical, neurologic, and psychiatric (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Nonpatient Version) evaluation.34,35 They were excluded for any history of Axis I psychiatric illness; Axis II diagnosis of schizotypal, schizoid, or paranoid personality disorder; family history of psychosis; or any medical condition or occurrence, including substance abuse, that could compromise brain function. Informed consent was obtained from all participants at the time of enrollment.
Descriptive clinical information and standardized rating scale measures for patients are presented in Table 1. Male patients were younger than female patients (t50 = 3.4, P = .001) and were significantly younger at illness onset, as defined by the earliest evidence of psychotic symptoms in the context of functional decline (t50 = 3.5, P = .001). There were no differences in duration of illness or medicated vs unmedicated status. Fifteen cases were of relatively new onset, with illness duration less than or equal to 2 years. Fifteen patients were not taking antipsychotic medications. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS),36The Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS),37 and The Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS)38 were administered at the time of MRI. Ratings were completed by trained investigators, with interrater reliability greater than 0.90. The BPRS items were summed to form an index of overall symptom severity. The SANS items were combined to form 5 standard subscale scores: Affective Flattening, Alogia, Avolition, Anhedonia, and Attention. A summary Negative Symptom Scale score combined all SANS subscales except Attention. The SAPS items were summed to yield a total Positive Symptom Scale score and 4 subscale measures: Hallucinations, Delusions, Bizarre Behavior, and Formal Thought Disorder. Patients were also subtyped into deficit and nondeficit categories.39 Total BPRS, SANS, and SAPS scores suggest that global symptom severity in this sample was relatively mild. Male patients scored higher on the avolition-apathy subscale of the SANS (t50 = 2.21, P = .03); otherwise, there were no sex differences in symptoms.
Magnetic resonance images were acquired on a 1.5-T system (GE Signa; General Electric Co, Milwaukee, Wis) with the following acquisition parameters: spoiled-gradient echo sequence; flip angle, 35°; repetition time, 35 milliseconds; echo time, 6 milliseconds; field of view, 24 cm; 1-mm slice thickness without gaps; and transaxial images with 0.9375 × 0.9375-mm in-plane resolution. Images were realigned to correct for head tilt and resliced into coronal sections orthogonal to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. Brain volume was extracted semiautomatically and segmented into gray matter and white matter by means of the optimal thresholding and morphologic operations described previously.40-42
The ROIs were traced on coronal images for 3 cortical gray matter areas within the temporal lobe: EC, TP, and PC. The EC encompasses Brodmann area 28. The PC is composed of Brodmann areas 35 and 36 and includes the piriform cortex. The TP represents Brodmann area 38. Figure 1 illustrates the approximate boundaries of these cortical areas on the surface of the AVMT. Precise boundaries are defined histologically on the basis of the identification of cytoarchitectonic transition zones.
Operational criteria for defining ROI boundaries on coronal MRIs were adapted from Insausti and colleagues,5 who validated the MRI landmarks through a histologic analysis of brains that underwent autopsy. All MRI boundaries were traced by individuals familiar with these neuroanatomic landmarks, and interrater reliability exceeded 0.85 for all regions. Specific MRI-guided criteria for each region were as follows (Figure 2).
The TP covers the anterior portion (first 6-8 mm) of the temporal lobe. Proceeding in a rostral-caudal direction, the entire temporal lobe parenchyma was included until the appearance of the superior and/or inferior temporal sulci (Figure 2A). Typically, the superior temporal sulcus appears slightly rostral to the inferior temporal sulcus. From this point on, the dorsolateral border was defined as the lateral border of the temporopolar sulcus (delineating the gyrus of Schwalbe) or, if this was absent, by the midpoint of the dorsal aspect of the pole. The ventrolateral border was located at the medial edge of the inferior temporal sulcus or, if not present yet, then along the medial edge of the superior temporal sulcus (Figure 2B). Caudally, the TP terminated with the appearance of the collateral sulcus, usually approximately 2-4 mm rostral to the limen insulae.
The PC borders the TP along the medial surface of the rostral temporal lobe. Rostrally, the PC replaces the TP in the dorsomedial aspect of the temporal lobe and caudally in the ventromedial temporal lobe. The rostral tip of the PC was defined as the first slice in which the collateral sulcus could be seen. On the most rostral slices, the edges of the PC were the lateral edge of the collateral sulcus ventromedially and the lateral border of the gyrus of Schwalbe dorsolaterally (Figure 2C). On the first slice in which the limen insulae appeared, the dorsomedial edge was the most medial point of the parahippocampal gyrus (Figure 2D). Caudal to this slice, the PC extended from the middle of the medial bank of the collateral sulcus to the lateral edge of this sulcus(Figure 2E-G). The PC terminated, caudally, with the disappearance of the gyrus intralimbicus.
The rostral edge of the EC was one slice caudal to the appearance of the limen insulae. The medial edge of the EC was the most medial point of the parahippocampal gyrus, extending laterally to the midpoint of the medial bank of the collateral sulcus (Figure 2E). Caudally, with appearance of the hippocampal fissure, the medial edge of the EC became the inferomedial edge of the fissure (Figure 2F). The EC terminated caudally one slice after the disappearance of the gyrus intralimbicus.
Olfactory psychophysical testing
Participants underwent standardized psychophysical assessment of their olfactory abilities. A single-staircase, forced-choice odor detection task was used to estimate basal detection sensitivity to phenylethyl alcohol. Participants were asked to smell successive pairs of odorants differing in concentration and to identify which odorant "smells stronger." The geometric mean of the last 4 staircase reversals (out of 7) provided the measure of odor detection threshold sensitivity. Sensitivity data were not obtained from 3 female patients who could not adequately comprehend the task. The ability to identify odors was assessed by the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT),43 a standardized 40-item forced-choice test. The UPSIT data were available for all participants.
Participants were administered a set of standardized neuropsychological tests. Individual test results were grouped together to form summary z score measures reflecting performance in different cognitive domains.7,44,45 For this correlative analysis, we selected z score measures for verbal and spatial memory. Verbal memory tests included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised,46 logical memory immediate and delayed recall, and the California Verbal Learning Test,47 trials 1 through 5. Spatial memory tests included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised, design reproduction immediate and delayed recall. Neuropsychological data were unavailable for 1 male patient, 1 male control, and 2 female controls.
To examine patient-control differences in brain volume, a general linear model with separate slope estimates was used. Gray matter volumes in the left and right TP, PC, and EC areas were the dependent measures, diagnosis was a grouping factor, and total cranial volume was a continuous predictor. The modulating effects of age, sex, and smoking (packs per day) were assessed by including these variables in the model. The relationship between regional brain volumes and olfactory functioning was examined by means of a separate-slopes general linear model with threshold detection sensitivity and UPSIT scores as dependent measures, diagnosis as a categorical predictor, and total cranial and regional brain volumes as continuous predictors. The relationship between brain volumes and memory performance was assessed similarly, with the verbal and spatial memory z scores as dependent measures. The relationship to clinical measures within the patient group was also considered. The SANS, SAPS, and BPRS scores, illness duration, medication dosage, age at onset, and deficit-nondeficit status were separate dependent measures, with regional and total brain volumes as continuous predictors. Statistical significance was based on a multivariate probability of P<.05 for all analyses.
The volumetric measures for each region are presented in Table 2. There were no significant main effects of diagnosis for any of the individual brain regions. However, multivariate separate-slope analyses of individual brain regions disclosed significant interactions between diagnosis and total cranial volume for gray matter in the PC (Wilks Λ4,168 = 0.84, P = .006) and EC (Wilks Λ4,168 = 0.83, P = .004), but not the TP (Figure 3). Post hoc univariate analyses showed significant diagnosis × cranial volume effects in both left and right hemispheres for both regions (PC: left, F2,85 = 6.33, P = .003; right, F2,85 = 4.78, P =.01; EC: left, F2,85 = 6.72, P = .002; right, F2,85 = 6.89, P = .002). These group × cranial volume differences remained significant after accounting for age, sex, and smoking. There was a significant interaction between diagnosis and smoking for the EC (Wilks Λ4,162 = 0.79, P<.001), but not for the PC or TP. Patients who smoked showed a slight volume decrement that was not seen among controls who smoked. There were no effects of sex or age on these regional volume measures and no differences between medicated and unmedicated patients.
A diagnosis × cranial volume interaction implies that the relationship between total cranial volume and ROI volume is different in the 2 diagnostic groups. As can be seen in Figure 3, gray matter volumes in the PC and EC were smaller in patients with schizophrenia, but primarily for those with larger cranial volumes. This is the type of relationship that one would expect if there were a relative percentage decrease in regional volume across subjects with different cranial volumes, as opposed to a relative magnitude decrease. The mean decreases observed here were 10.6% for the PC, 7.5% for the EC, and 1.9% for the TP.
Olfactory psychophysical measures
Mean scores on olfactory psychophysical measures are presented in Table 3. As expected, the patient sample showed significant impairments in both odor detection (Wilks Λ2,84 = 0.88, P = .004) and UPSIT identification performance (Wilks Λ2,87 = 0.90, P =.01). These were not explained by sex, age, or smoking differences, and there were no performance differences between medicated and unmedicated patients. However, when the volumetric measures were included in the general linear model as explanatory variables, there were no longer main effects of diagnosis. Rather, for olfactory thresholds, there were significant interactions between diagnosis and left PC (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.87, P = .05), right PC (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.86, P = .03), and left TP (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.86, P = .02). In all cases, reduced regional gray matter volume was associated, in patients, with poorer ability to detect the presence of an odor (Figure 4). In control subjects, there was either no relationship or a tendency toward the opposite (ie, better detection sensitivity associated with smaller volumes, consistent with the slightly better thresholds observed normally in women compared with men). There were no associations between EC volume and detection threshold sensitivity. For odor identification (UPSIT) performance, there were significant interactions between diagnosis and right TP (Wilks Λ4,150 = 0.86, P = .02) and right EC (Wilks Λ4,150 =0.88, P = .047), with decreased volume associated with poorer UPSIT performance in patients. The PC volumes were unrelated to performance on the odor identification task. These structure-function relationships were unaffected by age, smoking, or sex.
Memory performance measures
The pattern of results for the memory performance measures differed from those observed for the olfaction measures. Patients were impaired on both verbal (F1,82 = 16.50, P<.001) and spatial (F1,82 = 6.75, P = .01) memory. There was also a significant diagnosis × age interaction for verbal memory (F1,82 = 5.35, P = .02), with healthy individuals showing a decline with aging that was not observed in patients. However, neither of the memory performance measures was related to any of the regional brain volumes, either directly or interacting with diagnosis. Therefore, although memory performance was impaired in patients, this was not related to the extent of gray matter volume reduction in the AVMT. This was notwithstanding that these cortical regions have been implicated in memory processes.
There were no associations between MRI measures and clinical measures of illness onset, illness duration, or negative symptom rating scores. There were also no differences between deficit and nondeficit, new-onset and chronic illness, or medicated and unmedicated patients. There was an isolated relationship between left PC volume and ratings on the Bizarre Behavior subscale of the SAPS (F1,43 = 8.00, P =.007), with smaller volumes being associated with increasingly bizarre behavior.
Table 4 summarizes the basic findings of this investigation. These findings support the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia have reduced cortical volumes in some, but not all, subdivisions of the AVMT. We observed bilateral reductions in both the PC and EC, both of which receive direct afferent inputs from the olfactory bulb, with the latter also being integrally involved with hippocampal-mediated memory processes. Not surprisingly, the magnitude of the volume loss increased with increasing head size. In contrast, the TP—which is not considered to be part of either the limbic or olfactory cortex and does not receive inputs from the olfactory bulb—did not exhibit volume reductions in patients.
The data also support the hypothesis that olfactory deficits reflect specific structural brain abnormalities in cortical regions underlying different aspects of olfactory processing. We found an association between threshold detection sensitivity and bilateral PC volumes. This region includes the piriform cortex, the area that receives the bulk of the afferent inputs from the olfactory bulb and is primarily responsible for early odor perception.48 No such relationship was found between these same PC volume measures and olfactory identification performance. This suggests that the link between PC volume reduction and impaired olfactory threshold detection sensitivity represents a specific structure-function relationship that is disturbed in schizophrenia, rather than a nonspecific correlation reflecting global impairments. It is significant, in this regard, that EC volume was not similarly associated with olfactory threshold detection sensitivity. Although this region had comparable bilateral volume reductions in the patients, it is not as extensively innervated by olfactory bulb afferents and receives a much more widely distributed array of inputs. This further supports the specificity of the relationship between olfactory threshold sensitivity and PC volume.
Findings for the TP were more complex. This region was linked both to threshold sensitivity on the left and, with the EC, to odor identification on the right. There is an emerging understanding of the functional role of the TP as mediating recognition of familiar objects.49 As part of a so-called paralimbic system, the temporopolar region is important in integrating the perceptual experience of external sensory stimuli with one's subjective internal state.50 It might therefore participate in aspects of both odor perception and odor identification. However, without direct afferents from the bulb, this would appear to be an indirect association to more complex cognitive processes involving the integration of perceived odors, rather than to odor perception per se.
The specificity of the link between regional MRI measurements and olfaction is reinforced by the absence of any association between the volumes of these regions and memory performance. Memory abnormalities are relatively selective and severe deficits in patients.7 If reduced brain volume were a global indicator of disease severity and/or chronicity, we might expect it to correlate with memory performance more than with any other neuropsychological or psychophysical measure. The fact that we find memory impairments in this patient sample, but do not see an association with AVMT volumes, suggests that the associations we do observe (eg, between olfactory threshold sensitivity and PC volume) are functionally and anatomically specific.
The absence of associations between brain volumes and clinical measures is not too surprising. Structural brain changes, which are already evident at the initial presentation of illness,42,51,52 are relatively stable abnormalities, while clinical symptoms fluctuate over the course of illness.53 It is particularly noteworthy that there was no relationship of MRI volumes to either negative symptoms or the interaction of negative symptoms and age. Some studies,54-56 although not all,57 have reported associations of olfactory dysfunction with negative symptoms. These symptoms have also been shown to increase with increasing age of patients,53 although this is not prominent until much later in life. In this sample, with all but one patient younger than 50 years, negative symptoms were unrelated to either olfactory performance or age.
Two possible mechanisms may underlie the loss of AVMT gray matter and the associated disturbance in olfactory function. Glutamate is likely the predominant excitatory neurotransmitter acting at the synapse between afferent neurons originating in the mitral cell layer of the olfactory bulb and pyramidal cells in the piriform cortex.58 High levels of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamatergic receptors are found in this region.59 Dysfunction of NMDA receptors is the cornerstone of the "glutamate hypothesis" of schizophrenia.60 According to this model, hypofunction at the postsynaptic NMDA receptor results in excess release of glutamate, which, in turn, leads to both psychotic symptoms61 and irreversible neuronal degeneration in corticolimbic regions.62 If this is correct, then areas rich in NMDA receptors, such as the piriform cortex and EC, would be especially prone to structural damage and associated functional loss.
There is also evidence that abnormalities in the olfactory system extend to the most peripheral afferent neurons. Postmortem studies in our laboratory have demonstrated that there is abnormal development of olfactory receptor neurons in the nasal epithelium of patients with schizophrenia.63 We have also noted reductions in olfactory bulb volume on MRIs of patients.4 These findings are consistent with a model of underlying synaptic dysregulation within the olfactory bulb. Direct examination of the olfactory bulb has now provided further support for this hypothesis. Significant alterations have been found in the densities of presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins, as well as molecules important for trophic support in the bulb.64 These peripheral abnormalities are likely to result in secondary disruptions of the afferent inputs into primary olfactory cortical regions. Such a relative deafferentation not only would result in functional olfactory impairments but also could cause anterograde degeneration of the primary sensory areas of the cortex.65,66
The validity of behavioral measures of olfactory dysfunction is limited by nonspecific factors such as patient motivation and global cognitive impairment. The multiple replications in the literature suggest that these factors do not, in themselves, account for the olfactory deficits seen in patients. Nevertheless, they may be influencing some of the relationships we observe. Newer methods, using olfactory event-related potentials, are now available to directly assess the physiologic responses of olfactory cortical areas. We have recently shown that patients with schizophrenia also demonstrate impairments with the use of these nonbehavioral measures.67 It will be important to next determine whether these functional measures similarly correlate with brain volume measures. If they do, this will provide even stronger evidence of a specific structural-functional impairment of olfaction in schizophrenia.
1.Moberg
PJAgrin
RGur
REGur
RCTuretsky
BIDoty
RL Olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21325- 340
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Kopala
LCClark
CHurwitz
T Olfactory deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 1993;8245- 250
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Turetsky
BIMoberg
BJArnold
SADoty
RLGur
RE Low olfactory bulb volume in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160703- 708
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Turetsky
BIMoberg
PJYousem
DMDoty
RLArnold
SEGur
RE Reduced olfactory bulb volume in patients with schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157828- 830
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Insausti
RJuottonen
KSoininen
HInsausti
AMPartanen
KVainio
PLaakso
MPPitkanen
A MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19659- 671
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Saykin
AJShtasel
DLGur
REKester
DBMozley
LHStafiniak
PGur
RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Saykin
AJGur
RCGur
REMozley
PDMozley
LHResnick
SMKester
DBStafiniak
P Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48618- 624
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Kohler
CGBilker
WHagendoorn
MGur
REGur
RC Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition.
Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48127- 136
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Habel
UGur
RCMandal
MKSalloum
JBGur
RESchneider
F Emotional processing in schizophrenia across cultures: standardized measures of discrimination and experience.
Schizophr Res. 2000;4257- 66
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Schneider
FGur
RCGur
REShtasel
DL Emotional processing in schizophrenia: neurobehavioral probes in relation to psychopathology.
Schizophr Res. 1995;1767- 75
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Heimberg
CGur
REErwin
RJShtasel
DLGur
RC Facial emotion discrimination, III: behavioral findings in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 1992;42253- 265
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.DeLisi
LEHoff
ALSchwartz
JEShields
GWHalthore
SNGupta
SMHenn
FAAnand
AK Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29159- 175
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Colombo
CAbbruzzese
MLivian
SScotti
GLocatelli
MBonfanti
AScarone
S Memory functions and temporal-limbic morphology in schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 1993;5045- 56
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Bilder
RMBogerts
BAshtari
MWu
HAlvir
JMJody
DReiter
GBell
LLieberman
JA Anterior hippocampal volume reductions predict frontal lobe dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 1995;1747- 58
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Nestor
PGShenton
MEMcCarley
RWHaimson
JSmith
RSO'Donnell
BKimble
MKikinis
RJolesz
FA Neuropsychological correlates of MRI temporal lobe abnormalities in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1993;1501849- 1855
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Becker
TElmer
KMechela
BSchneider
FTaubert
SSchroth
GGrodd
WBartels
MBeckmann
H MRI findings in medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1990;183- 86
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Blackwood
DHYoun
AHMcQueen
JKMartin
MJRoxborough
HMMuir
WJSt Clair
DMKean
DM Magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia: altered brain morphology associated with P300 abnormalities and eye tracking dysfuntion.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;30753- 769
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Dauphinais
IDDeLisi
LECrow
TJAlexandropoulos
KColter
NTuma
IGershon
ES Reduction in temporal lobe size in siblings with schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
Psychiatry Res. 1990;35137- 147
Google ScholarCrossref 20.DeLisi
LEDauphinais
EDGershon
ES Perinatal complications and reduced size of brain limbic structures in familial schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1988;14185- 191
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Havermans
RHonig
AVuurman
EFKrabbendam
LWilmink
JLamers
TVerheecke
CJJolles
JRomme
MAvan Praag
HM A controlled study of temporal lobe structure volumes and P300 responses in schizophrenic patients with persistent auditory hallucinations.
Schizophr Res. 1999;38151- 158
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Jernigan
TLZisook
SHeaton
RKMoranville
JTHesselink
JRBraff
DL Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48881- 890
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Kawasaki
YMaeda
YUrata
KHigashima
MYamaguchi
NSuzuki
MTakashima
TIde
Y A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of patients with schizophrenia.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;242268- 272
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Ohnuma
TKimura
MTakahashi
TIwamoto
NArai
H A magnetic resonance imaging study in first-episode disorganized-type patients with schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;519- 15
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Razi
KGreene
KPSakuma
MGe
SKushner
MDeLisi
LE Reduction of the parahippocampal gyrus and the hippocampus in patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174512- 519
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Sanfilipo
MLafargue
TRusinek
HArena
LLoneragan
CLautin
AFeiner
DRotrosen
JWolkin
A Volumetric measure of the frontal and temporal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relationship to negative symptoms.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57471- 480
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Shenton
MEKikinis
RJolesz
FAPollak
SDLeMay
MWible
CGHokama
HMartin
JMetcalf
DColeman
M
et al. Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.
N Engl J Med. 1992;327604- 612
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Staal
WGPol
HE HulshoffSchnack
HGHoogendoorn
MLJellma
KKahn
RS Structural brain abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and their healthy siblings.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157416- 421
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Pearlson
GDBarta
PEPowers
REMenon
RRRichards
SSAylward
EHFederman
EBChase
GAPetty
RGTien
AY Ziskind-Somerfeld Research Award 1996: medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;411- 14
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Nasrallah
HASharma
SOlson
SC The volume of the entorhinal cortex in schizophrenia: a controlled MRI study.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1997;211317- 1322
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Florencio
PSO'Driscoll
GA The medial temporal lobe and schizophrenia.
McGill Med J. 1999;525- 34
Google Scholar 32.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBWGibbon
M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Patient Version (SCID-P). New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1986;
33.Shtasel
DLGur
REGallacher
FHeimberg
CCannon
TGur
RC Phenomenology and functioning in first-episode schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1992;18449- 462
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBWGibbon
M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Nonpatient Version (SCID-NP). New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1987;
35.Shtasel
DLGur
REMozley
PDRichards
JTaleff
MMHeimberg
CGallacher
FGur
RC Volunteers for biomedical research: recruitment and screening of normal controls.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;481022- 1025
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Overall
JRGorham
DR The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.
J Oper Psychiatry. 1980;1148- 64
Google Scholar 37.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City University of Iowa1983;
38.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City University of Iowa1984;
39.Carpenter
WT
JrHeinrichs
DWWagman
AW Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept.
Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145578- 583
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Gur
RECowell
PELatshaw
ATuretsky
BIGrossman
RIArnold
SEBilker
WBGur
RC Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57761- 768
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Gur
RETuretsky
BICowell
PEFinkelman
CMaany
VGrossman
RIArnold
SEBilker
WBGur
RC Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57769- 775
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Doty
RLShaman
PDann
M Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function.
Physiol Behav. 1984;32489- 502
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Hill
SKRagland
JDGur
RCGur
RE Neuropsychological differences among empirically derived clinical subtypes of schizophrenia.
Neuropsychology. 2001;15492- 501
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Censits
DMRagland
JDGur
RCGur
RE Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study.
Schizophr Res. 1997;24289- 298
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Wechsler
DA Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised (WMS-R), Manual. Cleveland, Ohio Psychological Corp1981;
47.Delis
DCKramer
JHKaplan
EOber
BA California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Manual. New York, NY Psychological Corp1987;
48.Jones-Gotman
MZatorre
RJCendes
FOlivier
AAndermann
FMcMackin
DStaunton
HSiegel
AMWieser
HG Contribution of medial versus lateral temporal-lobe structures to human odour identification.
Brain. 1997;1201845- 1856
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Nakamura
KKubota
K The primate temporal pole: its putative role in object recognition and memory.
Behav Brain Res. 1996;7753- 77
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Mesulam
MMMufson
EJ Insula of the old world monkey, I: architectonics in the insulo-orbito-temporal component of the paralimbic brain.
J Comp Neurol. 1982;2121- 22
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Gur
RECowell
PTuretsky
BIGallacher
FCannon
TBilker
WGur
RC A follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia: relationship of neuroanatomical changes to clinical and neurobehavioral measures.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55145- 152
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Turetsky
BCowell
PEGur
RCGrossman
RIShtasel
DLGur
RE Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521061- 1070
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Gur
REPetty
RGTuretsky
BIGur
RC Schizophrenia throughout life: sex differences in severity and profile of symptoms.
Schizophr Res. 1996;211- 12
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Brewer
WJPantelis
CAnderson
VVelakoulis
DSingh
BCopolov
DLMcGorry
PD Stability of olfactory identification deficits in neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode psychosis.
Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158107- 115
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Brewer
WJEdwards
JAnderson
VRobinson
TPantelis
C Neuropsychological, olfactory, and hygiene deficits in men with negative symptom schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1996;401021- 1031
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Geddes
JHuws
RPratt
P Olfactory acuity in the positive and negative syndromes of schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29774- 778
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Stedman
TJClair
AL Neuropsychological, neurological and symptom correlates of impaired olfactory identification in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 1998;3223- 30
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Greer
CA Structural organization of the olfactory system. Getchell
TVDoty
RLBartoshuk
LMSnow
JBeds.
Smell and Taste in Health and Disease New York, NY Raven Press1991;65- 81
Google Scholar 59.Cotman
CWMonaghan
DTOttersen
OPStorm-Mathisen
J Anatomical organization of excitatory amino acid receptors and their pathways.
Trends Neurosci. 1987;10273- 279
Google ScholarCrossref 61.Ellison
G The N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists phencyclidine, ketamine and dizocilpine as both behavioral and anatomical models of the dementias.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1995;20250- 267
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Corso
TDSesma
MATenkova
TIDer
TCWozniak
DFFarber
NBOlney
JW Multifocal brain damage induced by phencyclidine is augmented by pilocarpine.
Brain Res. 1997;7521- 14
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Arnold
SEHan
LYMoberg
PJTuretsky
BIGur
RETrojanowski
JQHahn
CG Dysregulation of olfactory receptor neuron lineage in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58829- 835
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Arnold
SERioux
LHan
L-Y Molecular markers of axon guidance and synaptogenesis in the olfactory bulb in schizophrenia [abstract].
Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2001;27454- 455
Google Scholar 65.Sherrard
RMBower
AJ Acute neuronal and vascular changes following unilateral cerebellar pedunculotomy in the neonatal rat.
J Anat. 1997;191177- 189
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Schwerdtfeger
WKBuhl
EHGermroth
P Disynaptic olfactory input to the hippocampus mediated by stellate cells in the entorhinal cortex.
J Comp Neurol. 1990;292163- 177
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Turetsky
BIMoberg
PJOwzar
KJohnson
SADoty
RLGur
RE Physiological impairment of olfactory stimulus processing in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53403- 411
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Corresponding author and reprints: Bruce I. Turetsky, MD, Neuropsychiatry Program, Department of Psychiatry, 10th Floor, Gates Bldg, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104 (e-mail: turetsky@bbl.med.upenn.edu).
Submitted for publication July 18, 2002; final revision received May 16, 2003; accepted May 19, 2003.
This study was supported by grant MH59852 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.
We thank Laurel Evans and Sarah C. Johnson, BA, for their assistance in the difficult tasks of MRI processing and cortical ROI analysis, and Warren Bilker, PhD, for statistical consultation.