[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Medial view of the right hemisphere, depicting the approximate boundaries of the 3 cortical subdivisions of the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe. Precise boundaries are defined histologically on the basis of cytoarchitectonic transition zones. TP indicates temporopolar cortex; PC, perirhinal cortex; and EC, entorhinal cortex.

Medial view of the right hemisphere, depicting the approximate boundaries of the 3 cortical subdivisions of the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe. Precise boundaries are defined histologically on the basis of cytoarchitectonic transition zones. TP indicates temporopolar cortex; PC, perirhinal cortex; and EC, entorhinal cortex.

Figure 2. 
Examples of the regional boundaries of anterior ventromedial temporal lobe regions as operationally defined on coronal magnetic resonance images. Images A through G move progressively in a rostral to caudal direction.

Examples of the regional boundaries of anterior ventromedial temporal lobe regions as operationally defined on coronal magnetic resonance images. Images A through G move progressively in a rostral to caudal direction.

Figure 3. 
Relationship between cortical gray matter volume and total cranial volume for the 3 separate subdivisions of the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe. Regional volume measures are the sum of the left and right hemisphere values. Patient-control differences are significant for perirhinal (P = .006) and entorhinal (P = .004) cortexes but not for the temporopolar cortex.

Relationship between cortical gray matter volume and total cranial volume for the 3 separate subdivisions of the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe. Regional volume measures are the sum of the left and right hemisphere values. Patient-control differences are significant for perirhinal (P = .006) and entorhinal (P = .004) cortexes but not for the temporopolar cortex.

Figure 4. 
Relationship between perirhinal gray matter volume and olfactory threshold sensitivity. This relationship is significantly different for patients and controls. Reduced perirhinal volume results in higher threshold concentrations (ie, decreased olfactory sensitivity) in patients, but not in healthy controls. PEA indicates phenylethyl alcohol.

Relationship between perirhinal gray matter volume and olfactory threshold sensitivity. This relationship is significantly different for patients and controls. Reduced perirhinal volume results in higher threshold concentrations (ie, decreased olfactory sensitivity) in patients, but not in healthy controls. PEA indicates phenylethyl alcohol.

Table 1. 
Patient Clinical Measures*
Patient Clinical Measures*
Table 2. 
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measures
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measures
Table 3. 
Behavioral Measures
Behavioral Measures
Table 4. 
Summary of Findings by Region
Summary of Findings by Region
1.
Moberg  PJAgrin  RGur  REGur  RCTuretsky  BIDoty  RL Olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21325- 340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Kopala  LCClark  CHurwitz  T Olfactory deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1993;8245- 250PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  BJArnold  SADoty  RLGur  RE Low olfactory bulb volume in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160703- 708PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  PJYousem  DMDoty  RLArnold  SEGur  RE Reduced olfactory bulb volume in patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157828- 830PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Insausti  RJuottonen  KSoininen  HInsausti  AMPartanen  KVainio  PLaakso  MPPitkanen  A MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19659- 671PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Saykin  AJShtasel  DLGur  REKester  DBMozley  LHStafiniak  PGur  RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Saykin  AJGur  RCGur  REMozley  PDMozley  LHResnick  SMKester  DBStafiniak  P Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48618- 624PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Kohler  CGBilker  WHagendoorn  MGur  REGur  RC Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition.  Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48127- 136PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Habel  UGur  RCMandal  MKSalloum  JBGur  RESchneider  F Emotional processing in schizophrenia across cultures: standardized measures of discrimination and experience.  Schizophr Res. 2000;4257- 66PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Schneider  FGur  RCGur  REShtasel  DL Emotional processing in schizophrenia: neurobehavioral probes in relation to psychopathology.  Schizophr Res. 1995;1767- 75PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Heimberg  CGur  REErwin  RJShtasel  DLGur  RC Facial emotion discrimination, III: behavioral findings in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;42253- 265PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Shenton  MEDickey  CCFrumin  MMcCarley  RW A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 2001;491- 52PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
DeLisi  LEHoff  ALSchwartz  JEShields  GWHalthore  SNGupta  SMHenn  FAAnand  AK Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29159- 175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Colombo  CAbbruzzese  MLivian  SScotti  GLocatelli  MBonfanti  AScarone  S Memory functions and temporal-limbic morphology in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res. 1993;5045- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Bilder  RMBogerts  BAshtari  MWu  HAlvir  JMJody  DReiter  GBell  LLieberman  JA Anterior hippocampal volume reductions predict frontal lobe dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1995;1747- 58PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Nestor  PGShenton  MEMcCarley  RWHaimson  JSmith  RSO'Donnell  BKimble  MKikinis  RJolesz  FA Neuropsychological correlates of MRI temporal lobe abnormalities in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;1501849- 1855PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Becker  TElmer  KMechela  BSchneider  FTaubert  SSchroth  GGrodd  WBartels  MBeckmann  H MRI findings in medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia.  Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1990;183- 86Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Blackwood  DHYoun  AHMcQueen  JKMartin  MJRoxborough  HMMuir  WJSt Clair  DMKean  DM Magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia: altered brain morphology associated with P300 abnormalities and eye tracking dysfuntion.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;30753- 769Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Dauphinais  IDDeLisi  LECrow  TJAlexandropoulos  KColter  NTuma  IGershon  ES Reduction in temporal lobe size in siblings with schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.  Psychiatry Res. 1990;35137- 147Google ScholarCrossref
20.
DeLisi  LEDauphinais  EDGershon  ES Perinatal complications and reduced size of brain limbic structures in familial schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1988;14185- 191Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Havermans  RHonig  AVuurman  EFKrabbendam  LWilmink  JLamers  TVerheecke  CJJolles  JRomme  MAvan Praag  HM A controlled study of temporal lobe structure volumes and P300 responses in schizophrenic patients with persistent auditory hallucinations.  Schizophr Res. 1999;38151- 158Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Jernigan  TLZisook  SHeaton  RKMoranville  JTHesselink  JRBraff  DL Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48881- 890Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Kawasaki  YMaeda  YUrata  KHigashima  MYamaguchi  NSuzuki  MTakashima  TIde  Y A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of patients with schizophrenia.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;242268- 272Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Ohnuma  TKimura  MTakahashi  TIwamoto  NArai  H A magnetic resonance imaging study in first-episode disorganized-type patients with schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;519- 15Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Razi  KGreene  KPSakuma  MGe  SKushner  MDeLisi  LE Reduction of the parahippocampal gyrus and the hippocampus in patients with chronic schizophrenia.  Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174512- 519Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Sanfilipo  MLafargue  TRusinek  HArena  LLoneragan  CLautin  AFeiner  DRotrosen  JWolkin  A Volumetric measure of the frontal and temporal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relationship to negative symptoms.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57471- 480Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Shenton  MEKikinis  RJolesz  FAPollak  SDLeMay  MWible  CGHokama  HMartin  JMetcalf  DColeman  M  et al.  Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.  N Engl J Med. 1992;327604- 612Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Staal  WGPol  HE HulshoffSchnack  HGHoogendoorn  MLJellma  KKahn  RS Structural brain abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and their healthy siblings.  Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157416- 421Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Pearlson  GDBarta  PEPowers  REMenon  RRRichards  SSAylward  EHFederman  EBChase  GAPetty  RGTien  AY Ziskind-Somerfeld Research Award 1996: medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 1997;411- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Nasrallah  HASharma  SOlson  SC The volume of the entorhinal cortex in schizophrenia: a controlled MRI study.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1997;211317- 1322PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Florencio  PSO'Driscoll  GA The medial temporal lobe and schizophrenia.  McGill Med J. 1999;525- 34Google Scholar
32.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Patient Version (SCID-P).  New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1986;
33.
Shtasel  DLGur  REGallacher  FHeimberg  CCannon  TGur  RC Phenomenology and functioning in first-episode schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1992;18449- 462PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Nonpatient Version (SCID-NP).  New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1987;
35.
Shtasel  DLGur  REMozley  PDRichards  JTaleff  MMHeimberg  CGallacher  FGur  RC Volunteers for biomedical research: recruitment and screening of normal controls.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;481022- 1025PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Overall  JRGorham  DR The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.  J Oper Psychiatry. 1980;1148- 64Google Scholar
37.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS).  Iowa City University of Iowa1983;
38.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS).  Iowa City University of Iowa1984;
39.
Carpenter  WT  JrHeinrichs  DWWagman  AW Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept.  Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145578- 583PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Gur  RECowell  PELatshaw  ATuretsky  BIGrossman  RIArnold  SEBilker  WBGur  RC Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57761- 768PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Gur  RETuretsky  BIBilker  WBGur  RC Reduced gray matter volume in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56905- 911PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Gur  RETuretsky  BICowell  PEFinkelman  CMaany  VGrossman  RIArnold  SEBilker  WBGur  RC Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57769- 775PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Doty  RLShaman  PDann  M Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function.  Physiol Behav. 1984;32489- 502PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Hill  SKRagland  JDGur  RCGur  RE Neuropsychological differences among empirically derived clinical subtypes of schizophrenia.  Neuropsychology. 2001;15492- 501PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Censits  DMRagland  JDGur  RCGur  RE Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study.  Schizophr Res. 1997;24289- 298PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Wechsler  DA Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised (WMS-R), Manual.  Cleveland, Ohio Psychological Corp1981;
47.
Delis  DCKramer  JHKaplan  EOber  BA California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Manual.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1987;
48.
Jones-Gotman  MZatorre  RJCendes  FOlivier  AAndermann  FMcMackin  DStaunton  HSiegel  AMWieser  HG Contribution of medial versus lateral temporal-lobe structures to human odour identification.  Brain. 1997;1201845- 1856PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Nakamura  KKubota  K The primate temporal pole: its putative role in object recognition and memory.  Behav Brain Res. 1996;7753- 77PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Mesulam  MMMufson  EJ Insula of the old world monkey, I: architectonics in the insulo-orbito-temporal component of the paralimbic brain.  J Comp Neurol. 1982;2121- 22PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Gur  RECowell  PTuretsky  BIGallacher  FCannon  TBilker  WGur  RC A follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia: relationship of neuroanatomical changes to clinical and neurobehavioral measures.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55145- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Turetsky  BCowell  PEGur  RCGrossman  RIShtasel  DLGur  RE Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521061- 1070PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Gur  REPetty  RGTuretsky  BIGur  RC Schizophrenia throughout life: sex differences in severity and profile of symptoms.  Schizophr Res. 1996;211- 12PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Brewer  WJPantelis  CAnderson  VVelakoulis  DSingh  BCopolov  DLMcGorry  PD Stability of olfactory identification deficits in neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode psychosis.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158107- 115PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Brewer  WJEdwards  JAnderson  VRobinson  TPantelis  C Neuropsychological, olfactory, and hygiene deficits in men with negative symptom schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;401021- 1031PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Geddes  JHuws  RPratt  P Olfactory acuity in the positive and negative syndromes of schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29774- 778PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Stedman  TJClair  AL Neuropsychological, neurological and symptom correlates of impaired olfactory identification in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1998;3223- 30PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Greer  CA Structural organization of the olfactory system. Getchell  TVDoty  RLBartoshuk  LMSnow  JBeds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease New York, NY Raven Press1991;65- 81Google Scholar
59.
Cotman  CWMonaghan  DTOttersen  OPStorm-Mathisen  J Anatomical organization of excitatory amino acid receptors and their pathways.  Trends Neurosci. 1987;10273- 279Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Olney  JWNewcomer  JWFarber  NB NMDA receptor hypofunction model of schizophrenia.  J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33523- 533PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ellison  G The N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists phencyclidine, ketamine and dizocilpine as both behavioral and anatomical models of the dementias.  Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1995;20250- 267PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Corso  TDSesma  MATenkova  TIDer  TCWozniak  DFFarber  NBOlney  JW Multifocal brain damage induced by phencyclidine is augmented by pilocarpine.  Brain Res. 1997;7521- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Arnold  SEHan  LYMoberg  PJTuretsky  BIGur  RETrojanowski  JQHahn  CG Dysregulation of olfactory receptor neuron lineage in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58829- 835PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Arnold  SERioux  LHan  L-Y Molecular markers of axon guidance and synaptogenesis in the olfactory bulb in schizophrenia [abstract].  Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2001;27454- 455Google Scholar
65.
Sherrard  RMBower  AJ Acute neuronal and vascular changes following unilateral cerebellar pedunculotomy in the neonatal rat.  J Anat. 1997;191177- 189PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Schwerdtfeger  WKBuhl  EHGermroth  P Disynaptic olfactory input to the hippocampus mediated by stellate cells in the entorhinal cortex.  J Comp Neurol. 1990;292163- 177PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  PJOwzar  KJohnson  SADoty  RLGur  RE Physiological impairment of olfactory stimulus processing in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53403- 411PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
December 2003

Decrements in Volume of Anterior Ventromedial Temporal Lobe and Olfactory Dysfunction in Schizophrenia

Author Affiliations

From the Schizophrenia Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(12):1193-1200. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1193
Abstract

Context  Patients with schizophrenia exhibit olfactory deficits, but it is unclear whether these represent a specific abnormality. The link between olfactory impairments and regional brain abnormalities has yet to be established.

Objectives  To determine whether patients with schizophrenia exhibit volumetric deficits in the anterior ventromedial temporal lobe, the target for neuronal inputs from the olfactory bulb, and whether these are related to olfactory performance deficits.

Design  A cohort study of patients and healthy control subjects who underwent both 1-mm spoiled-gradient echo magnetic resonance imaging and behavioral tests of olfaction and memory.

Setting  Schizophrenia Research Center at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.

Participants  Fifty-two patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and 38 healthy control subjects. Individuals were excluded for history of head trauma, significant substance abuse, and medical conditions affecting brain function or olfactory capacity.

Main Outcome Measures  Gray matter volumes of the left and right temporal poles and the perirhinal and entorhinal cortexes; olfactory threshold detection sensitivity and identification test scores; composite indexes of verbal and spatial memory ability.

Results  Patients had reduced volumes, relative to cranial size, in left (P = .003) and right (P = .01) perirhinal and left (P = .002) and right (P = .002) entorhinal cortexes, but not in the temporal pole. Perirhinal, but not entorhinal, cortical volume decrement was associated with decreased olfactory threshold sensitivity. Neither region was associated with impaired memory performance.

Conclusions  Patients with schizophrenia have reduced cortical volumes in brain regions that receive afferents directly from the olfactory bulb. Behavioral olfactory deficits are related to structural brain abnormalities in these regions.

THERE IS increasing evidence that patients with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to detect and identify odors.1 These deficits are present early in the disease course and are unrelated to symptom severity, medication use,2 or smoking. However, it remains unclear whether they represent a specific impairment or merely reflect the global cognitive impairment seen in this disorder. The link between olfactory behavioral impairments and regional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia has yet to be established. A recent finding from our laboratory—that both patients and their healthy first-degree relatives have reduced olfactory bulb volumes—would suggest that structural abnormalities of the olfactory system underlie these impairments.3,4 Whether comparable abnormalities exist in the cortical areas that receive neuronal inputs directly from the olfactory bulbs, and whether these are related to olfactory performance deficits, are questions that remain unanswered.

Olfactory processing is mediated by many of the same medial temporal lobe areas of the brain that have been implicated in schizophrenia. Olfactory afferents travel via the olfactory tract to the ipsilateral anterior ventromedial temporal lobe (AVMT), where they synapse with pyramidal cells. The bulk of these afferents terminate in the piriform cortex, which is located at the rostral uncus and is thought to be responsible for initial olfactory perception. Some fibers terminate posteriorly in the entorhinal cortex (EC), the gateway to the hippocampus. Piriform cortex and EC also connect to the amygdala, thereby providing a neuroanatomic basis for the linkage of olfaction to emotion and memory, 2 processes known to be impaired in schizophrenia. Cytoarchitecturally, the AVMT may be subdivided into temporopolar cortex (TP) (Brodmann area 38), perirhinal cortex (PC) (Brodmann areas 35 and 36), and EC (Brodmann area 28),5 with the piriform cortex included as part of the PC. While the latter 2, which are considered to be part of the limbic cortex, have been implicated in olfaction and memory, the TP is generally considered to be developmentally and functionally distinct. Although it connects to the amygdala, hippocampus, and basal forebrain, it does not receive direct olfactory afferents.

Previous examinations of medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia, using volumetric magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques, have focused on the amygdala-hippocampal complex, with few studies considering the surrounding cortical areas that mediate olfaction. The principal motivation for emphasizing these 2 subcortical structures has been their link to memory and emotion processing. Memory impairments are extremely robust and selective trait abnormalities in schizophrenia6,7; emotion processing disturbances are gaining new prominence.8-11 In a recent review of the literature, Shenton and colleagues12 identified 34 (71%) of 48 studies that reported significant volume reductions in the amygdala and/or hippocampus in patients. Despite the role of the hippocampus in memory processing and the MR imaging findings supporting hippocampal abnormalities in schizophrenia, studies13-16 of structure-function relationships have generally failed to find an association between hippocampal volume reduction and memory impairment. In contrast to the large number of studies examining the hippocampus and amygdala, only 13 studies13,17-28 examined the adjacent parahippocampal gyrus, with 8 (62%) reporting reductions of this cortical gray matter area. Only 2 studies29,30 examined the EC, despite its role as a critical relay between the hippocampus and associational cortexes.31 One of these found bilateral volume reductions in patients,29 while the other reported no difference.30 Hence, it is still unclear to what extent AVMT cortical gray matter is reduced in schizophrenia. No studies, to our knowledge, have looked specifically at the cortical regions responsible for olfactory processing and their relationship to olfactory performance. Perhaps one reason for the relative dearth of such investigations is the problem of selecting appropriate MRI landmarks to guide region-of-interest (ROI) identification in this area. Boundaries between AVMT subregions are based on cytoarchitectural distinctions, rather than gross anatomic features.

In this investigation, we applied volumetric MRI methods to quantify the cortical gray matter volumes of cytoarchitecturally distinct areas of the AVMT in a large sample of patients and healthy control subjects. We used transitional landmarks derived from histologic analysis5 to parse the region into discrete TP, PC, and EC. Participants were assessed on olfactory and memory performance to investigate the relationship between cortical volume abnormalities and behavior.

Methods
Participants

The sample consisted of 52 patients (27 men, 25 women) with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and 38 healthy volunteers(21 men, 17 women). Patient age ranged from 19 to 53 years (mean ± SD, 32.3 ± 9.1 years); healthy control age ranged from 18 to 56 years (mean ± SD, 28.2 ± 9.4 years). This represented a small but significant group difference (t88 = 2.07, P = .04). There was also a difference in smoking habits: 5 of 38 controls and 19 of 52 patients were active smokers (χ21 = 6.14, P<.05). Mean numbers of packs per day were 0.13 ± 0.30 for controls and 0.38 ± 0.51 for patients(t87 = 2.73, P =.008). There was no difference between the groups in sex distribution.

Patients were consecutively referred from both outpatient and inpatient settings and received medical, neurologic, and psychiatric evaluations, including the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R –Patient Version.32,33 To ensure diagnostic accuracy, they were clinically reassessed at 6-month intervals after intake. There was no history of any disorder or event other than schizophrenia that could potentially affect brain function. All patients who met inclusion criteria and were willing and able to provide informed consent were included. Healthy volunteers were recruited by newspaper advertisement and underwent medical, neurologic, and psychiatric (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Nonpatient Version) evaluation.34,35 They were excluded for any history of Axis I psychiatric illness; Axis II diagnosis of schizotypal, schizoid, or paranoid personality disorder; family history of psychosis; or any medical condition or occurrence, including substance abuse, that could compromise brain function. Informed consent was obtained from all participants at the time of enrollment.

Descriptive clinical information and standardized rating scale measures for patients are presented in Table 1. Male patients were younger than female patients (t50 = 3.4, P = .001) and were significantly younger at illness onset, as defined by the earliest evidence of psychotic symptoms in the context of functional decline (t50 = 3.5, P = .001). There were no differences in duration of illness or medicated vs unmedicated status. Fifteen cases were of relatively new onset, with illness duration less than or equal to 2 years. Fifteen patients were not taking antipsychotic medications. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS),36The Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS),37 and The Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS)38 were administered at the time of MRI. Ratings were completed by trained investigators, with interrater reliability greater than 0.90. The BPRS items were summed to form an index of overall symptom severity. The SANS items were combined to form 5 standard subscale scores: Affective Flattening, Alogia, Avolition, Anhedonia, and Attention. A summary Negative Symptom Scale score combined all SANS subscales except Attention. The SAPS items were summed to yield a total Positive Symptom Scale score and 4 subscale measures: Hallucinations, Delusions, Bizarre Behavior, and Formal Thought Disorder. Patients were also subtyped into deficit and nondeficit categories.39 Total BPRS, SANS, and SAPS scores suggest that global symptom severity in this sample was relatively mild. Male patients scored higher on the avolition-apathy subscale of the SANS (t50 = 2.21, P = .03); otherwise, there were no sex differences in symptoms.

Mr image acquisition

Magnetic resonance images were acquired on a 1.5-T system (GE Signa; General Electric Co, Milwaukee, Wis) with the following acquisition parameters: spoiled-gradient echo sequence; flip angle, 35°; repetition time, 35 milliseconds; echo time, 6 milliseconds; field of view, 24 cm; 1-mm slice thickness without gaps; and transaxial images with 0.9375 × 0.9375-mm in-plane resolution. Images were realigned to correct for head tilt and resliced into coronal sections orthogonal to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. Brain volume was extracted semiautomatically and segmented into gray matter and white matter by means of the optimal thresholding and morphologic operations described previously.40-42

Roi identification

The ROIs were traced on coronal images for 3 cortical gray matter areas within the temporal lobe: EC, TP, and PC. The EC encompasses Brodmann area 28. The PC is composed of Brodmann areas 35 and 36 and includes the piriform cortex. The TP represents Brodmann area 38. Figure 1 illustrates the approximate boundaries of these cortical areas on the surface of the AVMT. Precise boundaries are defined histologically on the basis of the identification of cytoarchitectonic transition zones.

Operational criteria for defining ROI boundaries on coronal MRIs were adapted from Insausti and colleagues,5 who validated the MRI landmarks through a histologic analysis of brains that underwent autopsy. All MRI boundaries were traced by individuals familiar with these neuroanatomic landmarks, and interrater reliability exceeded 0.85 for all regions. Specific MRI-guided criteria for each region were as follows (Figure 2).

Temporopolar Cortex

The TP covers the anterior portion (first 6-8 mm) of the temporal lobe. Proceeding in a rostral-caudal direction, the entire temporal lobe parenchyma was included until the appearance of the superior and/or inferior temporal sulci (Figure 2A). Typically, the superior temporal sulcus appears slightly rostral to the inferior temporal sulcus. From this point on, the dorsolateral border was defined as the lateral border of the temporopolar sulcus (delineating the gyrus of Schwalbe) or, if this was absent, by the midpoint of the dorsal aspect of the pole. The ventrolateral border was located at the medial edge of the inferior temporal sulcus or, if not present yet, then along the medial edge of the superior temporal sulcus (Figure 2B). Caudally, the TP terminated with the appearance of the collateral sulcus, usually approximately 2-4 mm rostral to the limen insulae.

Perirhinal Cortex

The PC borders the TP along the medial surface of the rostral temporal lobe. Rostrally, the PC replaces the TP in the dorsomedial aspect of the temporal lobe and caudally in the ventromedial temporal lobe. The rostral tip of the PC was defined as the first slice in which the collateral sulcus could be seen. On the most rostral slices, the edges of the PC were the lateral edge of the collateral sulcus ventromedially and the lateral border of the gyrus of Schwalbe dorsolaterally (Figure 2C). On the first slice in which the limen insulae appeared, the dorsomedial edge was the most medial point of the parahippocampal gyrus (Figure 2D). Caudal to this slice, the PC extended from the middle of the medial bank of the collateral sulcus to the lateral edge of this sulcus(Figure 2E-G). The PC terminated, caudally, with the disappearance of the gyrus intralimbicus.

Entorhinal Cortex

The rostral edge of the EC was one slice caudal to the appearance of the limen insulae. The medial edge of the EC was the most medial point of the parahippocampal gyrus, extending laterally to the midpoint of the medial bank of the collateral sulcus (Figure 2E). Caudally, with appearance of the hippocampal fissure, the medial edge of the EC became the inferomedial edge of the fissure (Figure 2F). The EC terminated caudally one slice after the disappearance of the gyrus intralimbicus.

Olfactory psychophysical testing

Participants underwent standardized psychophysical assessment of their olfactory abilities. A single-staircase, forced-choice odor detection task was used to estimate basal detection sensitivity to phenylethyl alcohol. Participants were asked to smell successive pairs of odorants differing in concentration and to identify which odorant "smells stronger." The geometric mean of the last 4 staircase reversals (out of 7) provided the measure of odor detection threshold sensitivity. Sensitivity data were not obtained from 3 female patients who could not adequately comprehend the task. The ability to identify odors was assessed by the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT),43 a standardized 40-item forced-choice test. The UPSIT data were available for all participants.

Memory assessment

Participants were administered a set of standardized neuropsychological tests. Individual test results were grouped together to form summary z score measures reflecting performance in different cognitive domains.7,44,45 For this correlative analysis, we selected z score measures for verbal and spatial memory. Verbal memory tests included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised,46 logical memory immediate and delayed recall, and the California Verbal Learning Test,47 trials 1 through 5. Spatial memory tests included the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised, design reproduction immediate and delayed recall. Neuropsychological data were unavailable for 1 male patient, 1 male control, and 2 female controls.

Data analysis

To examine patient-control differences in brain volume, a general linear model with separate slope estimates was used. Gray matter volumes in the left and right TP, PC, and EC areas were the dependent measures, diagnosis was a grouping factor, and total cranial volume was a continuous predictor. The modulating effects of age, sex, and smoking (packs per day) were assessed by including these variables in the model. The relationship between regional brain volumes and olfactory functioning was examined by means of a separate-slopes general linear model with threshold detection sensitivity and UPSIT scores as dependent measures, diagnosis as a categorical predictor, and total cranial and regional brain volumes as continuous predictors. The relationship between brain volumes and memory performance was assessed similarly, with the verbal and spatial memory z scores as dependent measures. The relationship to clinical measures within the patient group was also considered. The SANS, SAPS, and BPRS scores, illness duration, medication dosage, age at onset, and deficit-nondeficit status were separate dependent measures, with regional and total brain volumes as continuous predictors. Statistical significance was based on a multivariate probability of P<.05 for all analyses.

Results
Regional brain volumes

The volumetric measures for each region are presented in Table 2. There were no significant main effects of diagnosis for any of the individual brain regions. However, multivariate separate-slope analyses of individual brain regions disclosed significant interactions between diagnosis and total cranial volume for gray matter in the PC (Wilks Λ4,168 = 0.84, P = .006) and EC (Wilks Λ4,168 = 0.83, P = .004), but not the TP (Figure 3). Post hoc univariate analyses showed significant diagnosis × cranial volume effects in both left and right hemispheres for both regions (PC: left, F2,85 = 6.33, P = .003; right, F2,85 = 4.78, P =.01; EC: left, F2,85 = 6.72, P = .002; right, F2,85 = 6.89, P = .002). These group × cranial volume differences remained significant after accounting for age, sex, and smoking. There was a significant interaction between diagnosis and smoking for the EC (Wilks Λ4,162 = 0.79, P<.001), but not for the PC or TP. Patients who smoked showed a slight volume decrement that was not seen among controls who smoked. There were no effects of sex or age on these regional volume measures and no differences between medicated and unmedicated patients.

A diagnosis × cranial volume interaction implies that the relationship between total cranial volume and ROI volume is different in the 2 diagnostic groups. As can be seen in Figure 3, gray matter volumes in the PC and EC were smaller in patients with schizophrenia, but primarily for those with larger cranial volumes. This is the type of relationship that one would expect if there were a relative percentage decrease in regional volume across subjects with different cranial volumes, as opposed to a relative magnitude decrease. The mean decreases observed here were 10.6% for the PC, 7.5% for the EC, and 1.9% for the TP.

Olfactory psychophysical measures

Mean scores on olfactory psychophysical measures are presented in Table 3. As expected, the patient sample showed significant impairments in both odor detection (Wilks Λ2,84 = 0.88, P = .004) and UPSIT identification performance (Wilks Λ2,87 = 0.90, P =.01). These were not explained by sex, age, or smoking differences, and there were no performance differences between medicated and unmedicated patients. However, when the volumetric measures were included in the general linear model as explanatory variables, there were no longer main effects of diagnosis. Rather, for olfactory thresholds, there were significant interactions between diagnosis and left PC (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.87, P = .05), right PC (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.86, P = .03), and left TP (Wilks Λ4,144 = 0.86, P = .02). In all cases, reduced regional gray matter volume was associated, in patients, with poorer ability to detect the presence of an odor (Figure 4). In control subjects, there was either no relationship or a tendency toward the opposite (ie, better detection sensitivity associated with smaller volumes, consistent with the slightly better thresholds observed normally in women compared with men). There were no associations between EC volume and detection threshold sensitivity. For odor identification (UPSIT) performance, there were significant interactions between diagnosis and right TP (Wilks Λ4,150 = 0.86, P = .02) and right EC (Wilks Λ4,150 =0.88, P = .047), with decreased volume associated with poorer UPSIT performance in patients. The PC volumes were unrelated to performance on the odor identification task. These structure-function relationships were unaffected by age, smoking, or sex.

Memory performance measures

The pattern of results for the memory performance measures differed from those observed for the olfaction measures. Patients were impaired on both verbal (F1,82 = 16.50, P<.001) and spatial (F1,82 = 6.75, P = .01) memory. There was also a significant diagnosis × age interaction for verbal memory (F1,82 = 5.35, P = .02), with healthy individuals showing a decline with aging that was not observed in patients. However, neither of the memory performance measures was related to any of the regional brain volumes, either directly or interacting with diagnosis. Therefore, although memory performance was impaired in patients, this was not related to the extent of gray matter volume reduction in the AVMT. This was notwithstanding that these cortical regions have been implicated in memory processes.

Clinical measures

There were no associations between MRI measures and clinical measures of illness onset, illness duration, or negative symptom rating scores. There were also no differences between deficit and nondeficit, new-onset and chronic illness, or medicated and unmedicated patients. There was an isolated relationship between left PC volume and ratings on the Bizarre Behavior subscale of the SAPS (F1,43 = 8.00, P =.007), with smaller volumes being associated with increasingly bizarre behavior.

Comment

Table 4 summarizes the basic findings of this investigation. These findings support the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia have reduced cortical volumes in some, but not all, subdivisions of the AVMT. We observed bilateral reductions in both the PC and EC, both of which receive direct afferent inputs from the olfactory bulb, with the latter also being integrally involved with hippocampal-mediated memory processes. Not surprisingly, the magnitude of the volume loss increased with increasing head size. In contrast, the TP—which is not considered to be part of either the limbic or olfactory cortex and does not receive inputs from the olfactory bulb—did not exhibit volume reductions in patients.

The data also support the hypothesis that olfactory deficits reflect specific structural brain abnormalities in cortical regions underlying different aspects of olfactory processing. We found an association between threshold detection sensitivity and bilateral PC volumes. This region includes the piriform cortex, the area that receives the bulk of the afferent inputs from the olfactory bulb and is primarily responsible for early odor perception.48 No such relationship was found between these same PC volume measures and olfactory identification performance. This suggests that the link between PC volume reduction and impaired olfactory threshold detection sensitivity represents a specific structure-function relationship that is disturbed in schizophrenia, rather than a nonspecific correlation reflecting global impairments. It is significant, in this regard, that EC volume was not similarly associated with olfactory threshold detection sensitivity. Although this region had comparable bilateral volume reductions in the patients, it is not as extensively innervated by olfactory bulb afferents and receives a much more widely distributed array of inputs. This further supports the specificity of the relationship between olfactory threshold sensitivity and PC volume.

Findings for the TP were more complex. This region was linked both to threshold sensitivity on the left and, with the EC, to odor identification on the right. There is an emerging understanding of the functional role of the TP as mediating recognition of familiar objects.49 As part of a so-called paralimbic system, the temporopolar region is important in integrating the perceptual experience of external sensory stimuli with one's subjective internal state.50 It might therefore participate in aspects of both odor perception and odor identification. However, without direct afferents from the bulb, this would appear to be an indirect association to more complex cognitive processes involving the integration of perceived odors, rather than to odor perception per se.

The specificity of the link between regional MRI measurements and olfaction is reinforced by the absence of any association between the volumes of these regions and memory performance. Memory abnormalities are relatively selective and severe deficits in patients.7 If reduced brain volume were a global indicator of disease severity and/or chronicity, we might expect it to correlate with memory performance more than with any other neuropsychological or psychophysical measure. The fact that we find memory impairments in this patient sample, but do not see an association with AVMT volumes, suggests that the associations we do observe (eg, between olfactory threshold sensitivity and PC volume) are functionally and anatomically specific.

The absence of associations between brain volumes and clinical measures is not too surprising. Structural brain changes, which are already evident at the initial presentation of illness,42,51,52 are relatively stable abnormalities, while clinical symptoms fluctuate over the course of illness.53 It is particularly noteworthy that there was no relationship of MRI volumes to either negative symptoms or the interaction of negative symptoms and age. Some studies,54-56 although not all,57 have reported associations of olfactory dysfunction with negative symptoms. These symptoms have also been shown to increase with increasing age of patients,53 although this is not prominent until much later in life. In this sample, with all but one patient younger than 50 years, negative symptoms were unrelated to either olfactory performance or age.

Two possible mechanisms may underlie the loss of AVMT gray matter and the associated disturbance in olfactory function. Glutamate is likely the predominant excitatory neurotransmitter acting at the synapse between afferent neurons originating in the mitral cell layer of the olfactory bulb and pyramidal cells in the piriform cortex.58 High levels of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamatergic receptors are found in this region.59 Dysfunction of NMDA receptors is the cornerstone of the "glutamate hypothesis" of schizophrenia.60 According to this model, hypofunction at the postsynaptic NMDA receptor results in excess release of glutamate, which, in turn, leads to both psychotic symptoms61 and irreversible neuronal degeneration in corticolimbic regions.62 If this is correct, then areas rich in NMDA receptors, such as the piriform cortex and EC, would be especially prone to structural damage and associated functional loss.

There is also evidence that abnormalities in the olfactory system extend to the most peripheral afferent neurons. Postmortem studies in our laboratory have demonstrated that there is abnormal development of olfactory receptor neurons in the nasal epithelium of patients with schizophrenia.63 We have also noted reductions in olfactory bulb volume on MRIs of patients.4 These findings are consistent with a model of underlying synaptic dysregulation within the olfactory bulb. Direct examination of the olfactory bulb has now provided further support for this hypothesis. Significant alterations have been found in the densities of presynaptic and postsynaptic proteins, as well as molecules important for trophic support in the bulb.64 These peripheral abnormalities are likely to result in secondary disruptions of the afferent inputs into primary olfactory cortical regions. Such a relative deafferentation not only would result in functional olfactory impairments but also could cause anterograde degeneration of the primary sensory areas of the cortex.65,66

The validity of behavioral measures of olfactory dysfunction is limited by nonspecific factors such as patient motivation and global cognitive impairment. The multiple replications in the literature suggest that these factors do not, in themselves, account for the olfactory deficits seen in patients. Nevertheless, they may be influencing some of the relationships we observe. Newer methods, using olfactory event-related potentials, are now available to directly assess the physiologic responses of olfactory cortical areas. We have recently shown that patients with schizophrenia also demonstrate impairments with the use of these nonbehavioral measures.67 It will be important to next determine whether these functional measures similarly correlate with brain volume measures. If they do, this will provide even stronger evidence of a specific structural-functional impairment of olfaction in schizophrenia.

References
1.
Moberg  PJAgrin  RGur  REGur  RCTuretsky  BIDoty  RL Olfactory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21325- 340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Kopala  LCClark  CHurwitz  T Olfactory deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1993;8245- 250PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  BJArnold  SADoty  RLGur  RE Low olfactory bulb volume in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160703- 708PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  PJYousem  DMDoty  RLArnold  SEGur  RE Reduced olfactory bulb volume in patients with schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157828- 830PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Insausti  RJuottonen  KSoininen  HInsausti  AMPartanen  KVainio  PLaakso  MPPitkanen  A MR volumetric analysis of the human entorhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19659- 671PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Saykin  AJShtasel  DLGur  REKester  DBMozley  LHStafiniak  PGur  RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Saykin  AJGur  RCGur  REMozley  PDMozley  LHResnick  SMKester  DBStafiniak  P Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48618- 624PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Kohler  CGBilker  WHagendoorn  MGur  REGur  RC Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition.  Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48127- 136PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Habel  UGur  RCMandal  MKSalloum  JBGur  RESchneider  F Emotional processing in schizophrenia across cultures: standardized measures of discrimination and experience.  Schizophr Res. 2000;4257- 66PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Schneider  FGur  RCGur  REShtasel  DL Emotional processing in schizophrenia: neurobehavioral probes in relation to psychopathology.  Schizophr Res. 1995;1767- 75PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Heimberg  CGur  REErwin  RJShtasel  DLGur  RC Facial emotion discrimination, III: behavioral findings in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;42253- 265PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Shenton  MEDickey  CCFrumin  MMcCarley  RW A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 2001;491- 52PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
DeLisi  LEHoff  ALSchwartz  JEShields  GWHalthore  SNGupta  SMHenn  FAAnand  AK Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29159- 175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Colombo  CAbbruzzese  MLivian  SScotti  GLocatelli  MBonfanti  AScarone  S Memory functions and temporal-limbic morphology in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res. 1993;5045- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Bilder  RMBogerts  BAshtari  MWu  HAlvir  JMJody  DReiter  GBell  LLieberman  JA Anterior hippocampal volume reductions predict frontal lobe dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1995;1747- 58PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Nestor  PGShenton  MEMcCarley  RWHaimson  JSmith  RSO'Donnell  BKimble  MKikinis  RJolesz  FA Neuropsychological correlates of MRI temporal lobe abnormalities in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry. 1993;1501849- 1855PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Becker  TElmer  KMechela  BSchneider  FTaubert  SSchroth  GGrodd  WBartels  MBeckmann  H MRI findings in medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia.  Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1990;183- 86Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Blackwood  DHYoun  AHMcQueen  JKMartin  MJRoxborough  HMMuir  WJSt Clair  DMKean  DM Magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia: altered brain morphology associated with P300 abnormalities and eye tracking dysfuntion.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;30753- 769Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Dauphinais  IDDeLisi  LECrow  TJAlexandropoulos  KColter  NTuma  IGershon  ES Reduction in temporal lobe size in siblings with schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.  Psychiatry Res. 1990;35137- 147Google ScholarCrossref
20.
DeLisi  LEDauphinais  EDGershon  ES Perinatal complications and reduced size of brain limbic structures in familial schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1988;14185- 191Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Havermans  RHonig  AVuurman  EFKrabbendam  LWilmink  JLamers  TVerheecke  CJJolles  JRomme  MAvan Praag  HM A controlled study of temporal lobe structure volumes and P300 responses in schizophrenic patients with persistent auditory hallucinations.  Schizophr Res. 1999;38151- 158Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Jernigan  TLZisook  SHeaton  RKMoranville  JTHesselink  JRBraff  DL Magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in lenticular nuclei and cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48881- 890Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Kawasaki  YMaeda  YUrata  KHigashima  MYamaguchi  NSuzuki  MTakashima  TIde  Y A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of patients with schizophrenia.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1993;242268- 272Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Ohnuma  TKimura  MTakahashi  TIwamoto  NArai  H A magnetic resonance imaging study in first-episode disorganized-type patients with schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;519- 15Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Razi  KGreene  KPSakuma  MGe  SKushner  MDeLisi  LE Reduction of the parahippocampal gyrus and the hippocampus in patients with chronic schizophrenia.  Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174512- 519Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Sanfilipo  MLafargue  TRusinek  HArena  LLoneragan  CLautin  AFeiner  DRotrosen  JWolkin  A Volumetric measure of the frontal and temporal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relationship to negative symptoms.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57471- 480Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Shenton  MEKikinis  RJolesz  FAPollak  SDLeMay  MWible  CGHokama  HMartin  JMetcalf  DColeman  M  et al.  Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.  N Engl J Med. 1992;327604- 612Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Staal  WGPol  HE HulshoffSchnack  HGHoogendoorn  MLJellma  KKahn  RS Structural brain abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and their healthy siblings.  Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157416- 421Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Pearlson  GDBarta  PEPowers  REMenon  RRRichards  SSAylward  EHFederman  EBChase  GAPetty  RGTien  AY Ziskind-Somerfeld Research Award 1996: medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 1997;411- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Nasrallah  HASharma  SOlson  SC The volume of the entorhinal cortex in schizophrenia: a controlled MRI study.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1997;211317- 1322PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Florencio  PSO'Driscoll  GA The medial temporal lobe and schizophrenia.  McGill Med J. 1999;525- 34Google Scholar
32.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Patient Version (SCID-P).  New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1986;
33.
Shtasel  DLGur  REGallacher  FHeimberg  CCannon  TGur  RC Phenomenology and functioning in first-episode schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull. 1992;18449- 462PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  M Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Nonpatient Version (SCID-NP).  New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1987;
35.
Shtasel  DLGur  REMozley  PDRichards  JTaleff  MMHeimberg  CGallacher  FGur  RC Volunteers for biomedical research: recruitment and screening of normal controls.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;481022- 1025PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Overall  JRGorham  DR The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.  J Oper Psychiatry. 1980;1148- 64Google Scholar
37.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS).  Iowa City University of Iowa1983;
38.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS).  Iowa City University of Iowa1984;
39.
Carpenter  WT  JrHeinrichs  DWWagman  AW Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept.  Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145578- 583PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Gur  RECowell  PELatshaw  ATuretsky  BIGrossman  RIArnold  SEBilker  WBGur  RC Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57761- 768PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Gur  RETuretsky  BIBilker  WBGur  RC Reduced gray matter volume in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56905- 911PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Gur  RETuretsky  BICowell  PEFinkelman  CMaany  VGrossman  RIArnold  SEBilker  WBGur  RC Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57769- 775PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Doty  RLShaman  PDann  M Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function.  Physiol Behav. 1984;32489- 502PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Hill  SKRagland  JDGur  RCGur  RE Neuropsychological differences among empirically derived clinical subtypes of schizophrenia.  Neuropsychology. 2001;15492- 501PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Censits  DMRagland  JDGur  RCGur  RE Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study.  Schizophr Res. 1997;24289- 298PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Wechsler  DA Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Revised (WMS-R), Manual.  Cleveland, Ohio Psychological Corp1981;
47.
Delis  DCKramer  JHKaplan  EOber  BA California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Manual.  New York, NY Psychological Corp1987;
48.
Jones-Gotman  MZatorre  RJCendes  FOlivier  AAndermann  FMcMackin  DStaunton  HSiegel  AMWieser  HG Contribution of medial versus lateral temporal-lobe structures to human odour identification.  Brain. 1997;1201845- 1856PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Nakamura  KKubota  K The primate temporal pole: its putative role in object recognition and memory.  Behav Brain Res. 1996;7753- 77PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Mesulam  MMMufson  EJ Insula of the old world monkey, I: architectonics in the insulo-orbito-temporal component of the paralimbic brain.  J Comp Neurol. 1982;2121- 22PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Gur  RECowell  PTuretsky  BIGallacher  FCannon  TBilker  WGur  RC A follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia: relationship of neuroanatomical changes to clinical and neurobehavioral measures.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55145- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Turetsky  BCowell  PEGur  RCGrossman  RIShtasel  DLGur  RE Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521061- 1070PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Gur  REPetty  RGTuretsky  BIGur  RC Schizophrenia throughout life: sex differences in severity and profile of symptoms.  Schizophr Res. 1996;211- 12PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Brewer  WJPantelis  CAnderson  VVelakoulis  DSingh  BCopolov  DLMcGorry  PD Stability of olfactory identification deficits in neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode psychosis.  Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158107- 115PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Brewer  WJEdwards  JAnderson  VRobinson  TPantelis  C Neuropsychological, olfactory, and hygiene deficits in men with negative symptom schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;401021- 1031PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Geddes  JHuws  RPratt  P Olfactory acuity in the positive and negative syndromes of schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29774- 778PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Stedman  TJClair  AL Neuropsychological, neurological and symptom correlates of impaired olfactory identification in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res. 1998;3223- 30PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Greer  CA Structural organization of the olfactory system. Getchell  TVDoty  RLBartoshuk  LMSnow  JBeds. Smell and Taste in Health and Disease New York, NY Raven Press1991;65- 81Google Scholar
59.
Cotman  CWMonaghan  DTOttersen  OPStorm-Mathisen  J Anatomical organization of excitatory amino acid receptors and their pathways.  Trends Neurosci. 1987;10273- 279Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Olney  JWNewcomer  JWFarber  NB NMDA receptor hypofunction model of schizophrenia.  J Psychiatr Res. 1999;33523- 533PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ellison  G The N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists phencyclidine, ketamine and dizocilpine as both behavioral and anatomical models of the dementias.  Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1995;20250- 267PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Corso  TDSesma  MATenkova  TIDer  TCWozniak  DFFarber  NBOlney  JW Multifocal brain damage induced by phencyclidine is augmented by pilocarpine.  Brain Res. 1997;7521- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Arnold  SEHan  LYMoberg  PJTuretsky  BIGur  RETrojanowski  JQHahn  CG Dysregulation of olfactory receptor neuron lineage in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58829- 835PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Arnold  SERioux  LHan  L-Y Molecular markers of axon guidance and synaptogenesis in the olfactory bulb in schizophrenia [abstract].  Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2001;27454- 455Google Scholar
65.
Sherrard  RMBower  AJ Acute neuronal and vascular changes following unilateral cerebellar pedunculotomy in the neonatal rat.  J Anat. 1997;191177- 189PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Schwerdtfeger  WKBuhl  EHGermroth  P Disynaptic olfactory input to the hippocampus mediated by stellate cells in the entorhinal cortex.  J Comp Neurol. 1990;292163- 177PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Turetsky  BIMoberg  PJOwzar  KJohnson  SADoty  RLGur  RE Physiological impairment of olfactory stimulus processing in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53403- 411PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

Corresponding author and reprints: Bruce I. Turetsky, MD, Neuropsychiatry Program, Department of Psychiatry, 10th Floor, Gates Bldg, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104 (e-mail: turetsky@bbl.med.upenn.edu).

Submitted for publication July 18, 2002; final revision received May 16, 2003; accepted May 19, 2003.

This study was supported by grant MH59852 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.

We thank Laurel Evans and Sarah C. Johnson, BA, for their assistance in the difficult tasks of MRI processing and cortical ROI analysis, and Warren Bilker, PhD, for statistical consultation.

×