Context
There are many published twin studies of schizophrenia. Although these studies have been reviewed previously, to our knowledge, no review has provided quantitative summary estimates of the impact of genes and environment on liability to schizophrenia that also accounted for the different ascertainment strategies used.
Objective
To calculate meta-analytic estimates of heritability in liability and shared and individual-specific environmental effects from the pooled twin data.
Data Sources
We used a structured literature search to identify all published twin studies of schizophrenia, including MEDLINE, dissertation, and books-in-print searches.
Study Selection
Of the 14 identified studies, 12 met the minimal inclusion criteria of systematic ascertainment.
Data Synthesis
By using a multigroup twin model, we found evidence for substantial additive genetic effects—the point estimate of heritability in liability to schizophrenia was 81% (95% confidence interval, 73%-90%). Notably, there was consistent evidence across these studies for common or shared environmental influences on liability to schizophrenia—joint estimate, 11% (95% confidence interval, 3%-19%).
Conclusions
Despite evidence of heterogeneity across studies, these meta-analytic results from 12 published twin studies of schizophrenia are consistent with a view of schizophrenia as a complex trait that results from genetic and environmental etiological influences. These results are broadly informative in that they provide no information about the specific identity of these etiological influences, but they do provide a component of a unifying empirical basis supporting the rationality of searches for underlying genetic and common environmental etiological factors.
GENETIC epidemiological studies of schizophrenia have had a guiding influence on schizophrenia research. In particular, twin and adoption studies that suggested substantial genetic influences on the liability to schizophrenia1 helped create the empirical rationale for numerous ongoing searches to identify the predisposing genetic loci.2,3 Recently, several groups4-6 have presented evidence for genes that may be involved in the etiology of schizophrenia.
Although the primary twin studies of schizophrenia have been reviewed extensively7,8 and have been quantitatively summarized,9-12 we are aware of no meta-analysis of the primary studies that incorporated ascertainment corrections. Failure to correct for ascertainment (the mixtures of singly and doubly ascertained twin pairs found in most of these studies) could bias the results.
The goal of this report was to conduct a quantitative meta-analysis of the published twin studies of schizophrenia. A key advantage of synthetic meta-analytic vs traditional literature reviews is the potential to yield less biased quantitative summaries of the findings of many primary studies.13-16 We were also interested in an additional benefit of meta-analysis: its capacity to detect subtle effects for which individual studies may possess insufficient statistical power. Twin studies of uncommon discrete traits, like those for schizophrenia, possess limited statistical power in many circumstances (unless the sample or effect sizes are large).17 Only when the results of several studies are analyzed jointly in a meta-analysis can subtle effects be assessed with a reasonable degree of confidence.
To identify all relevant primary studies, we performed computerized PubMed searches for an inclusive list of descriptors and searched the reference lists of prior reviews of schizophrenia to identify any reports not retrieved in the PubMed search. We identified 14 published studies18-31 of schizophrenia from independent samples in 6 European countries, Japan, and the United States (Table 1). If there were multiple publications from the same sample, only the most recent was included.
The next step in a meta-analysis is usually the application of a set of inclusion criteria. For twin studies, these include systematic recruitment, blinding to co-twin diagnostic status and zygosity, and use of systematic data collection and diagnostic procedures. Many of these studies were conducted before these criteria were common in psychiatric research (718-24 of the 14 studies were published before 1970). Only 4 studies27-29,31 met all the inclusion criteria, and all were published after 1990. We considered studies that relied solely on hospitalization records as not being blind because the clinicians making the diagnosis may have been influenced by the co-twin history.
We chose to relax our a priori inclusion criteria for 4 reasons. First, the exclusion of most studies is not consistent with our desire to obtain meta-analytic estimates of what might be subtle statistical effects. Second, we retained the capacity to analyze the methodologically superior vs inferior studies. Third, we wanted to avoid a bias of modernity by including older studies. Many of the older studies were performed by prominent and highly respected researchers who conducted the studies in rigorous accordance with the accepted research practices of their era, and contain information pertinent to our research question. Finally, we were influenced by tradition: most prior reviews7,11 of twin studies of schizophrenia have included most of these studies. Therefore, our final exclusion criterion was that these studies ascertain subjects in a systematic manner, which led to the exclusion of 2 studies and the inclusion of 12 studies, as shown in Table 1. Nearly all of these primary studies were believed to support the strong role of genetic factors in the etiology of schizophrenia.
A model for the patterns of familial resemblance was used to predict the observed concordant and discordant pair frequencies. The model included parameters for additive genetic (a2), common environmental (c2), and individual-specific environmental (e2) components of variance.32 Additive genetic influences are shared completely by monozygotic (MZ) twins and correlate 0.5 between dizygotic (DZ) twins. Common environmental influences are shared completely by the members of a twin pair regardless of zygosity, and account for DZ correlations that are greater than half of the MZ correlation. Individual-specific environmental components contribute separately to each twin and, therefore, account for less than a perfect resemblance between MZ twins. These parameters were estimated by maximum likelihood.
The likelihood of the observed pairs of twins was computed using a bivariate normal threshold model of liability.33 This model specifies that individuals in the population have a frequency distribution of liability to schizophrenia that is described by a normal distribution. On this liability continuum, there exists a threshold t such that individuals above the threshold have schizophrenia and those below do not. The distribution of twin pair liabilities is bivariate normal, with unit variances and correlations as predicted by the following statistical models: rMZ = a2 + c2 and rDZ = 0.5a2 + c2. Although it is usual to fit submodels that eliminate specific sources of variance, this practice is inappropriate in a meta-analysis, and a common approach to selecting parsimonious models (via the Akaike information content) can select incorrect models.34
Four different types of subject ascertainment were used in the 12 twin studies in Table 1. Each type of ascertainment is associated with different types of data. First, random population ascertainment yields a full 2 × 2 contingency table (diagnosis of twin 1 × diagnosis of twin 2). In this instance, all 4 types of twin pairs are observed directly (ie, concordant unaffected twin pairs, the 2 discordant cells, and concordant affected twin pairs), as in 2 twin studies26,30 of schizophrenia. Second, complete ascertainment is when concordant unaffected twin pairs are the only pairs not observed, so that concordant and discordant affected twins are ascertained, as in 4 twin studies23-25,27 of schizophrenia. Third, 2 twin studies29,35 of schizophrenia used single ascertainment, in which pairs with 2 probands are not observed and only 1 of the 2 possible discordant cells is observed together with concordant affected pairs. Finally, incomplete ascertainment is intermediate between complete and single ascertainment. The key quantity is π, the probability of being ascertained given that one is affected. The π value is 1 for complete ascertainment, approaches 0 for single ascertainment, and 0<π<1 for incomplete ascertainment. The π value can be estimated as 2D/(2D + S), where D is the number of doubly ascertained pairs and S is the number of singly ascertained pairs. Four twin studies20,21,28,31 of schizophrenia had incomplete ascertainment.
Each type of ascertainment requires a different ascertainment correction. For random population ascertainment, no correction for ascertainment is required because pairs are thought to be representative of the population and the likelihood is as follows:
where ϕ is the bivariate normal probability density function; x1 and x2 are the liabilities to schizophrenia for twin 1 and twin 2, respectively; and ai and bi are a0 = −∞, a1 = t, b0 = t, and b1 = ∞ (where the subscript 0 denotes unaffected; and 1, affected).
Under nonrandom ascertainment, the likelihood of pairs may be written as equation 1 multiplied by an ascertainment correction. Under complete ascertainment(π = 1), the ascertainment correction is as follows:
Under single ascertainment (π→0), the correction is as follows:
Under incomplete ascertainment (0<π<1), the correction for single-proband pairs is as follows:
For double-proband pairs, the equation is as follows:
A script that uses the appropriate correction for ascertainment and the population threshold estimate for simultaneous analysis of the 12 studies is available on the Mx Examples Web site (http://www.vcu.edu/mx/examples.html).36 To test for heterogeneity, the parameters of the model (a2 and c2) were set equal across the studies, and the fit of this model was compared via the likelihood-ratio test to the fit of the model in which the a2, c2, and e2 parameters were allowed to differ between studies. Because the variance of the latent distribution cannot be estimated separately from the thresholds, we used the common practice of standardizing to unit variance by imposing the following nonlinear constraint: a2 + c2 + e2 = 1. Effectively, there are only 2 free parameters per sample (a2 and c2), so the comparison tests for heterogeneity have a df = 2 for each sample beyond the first.
Figure 1 depicts the MZ and DZ twin correlations from 12 twin studies of schizophrenia and the meta-analytic summary estimates. Seven of the 12 studies and the meta-analytic summary estimate(rMZ = 0.92; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.91-0.94; and rDZ = 0.52; 95% CI, 0.48-0.56) lie between lines depicting extreme cases in which a trait is entirely due to additive genetic effects (100% a2) or entirely due to common environmental effects (100% c2), suggesting the presence of both additive genetic and common environmental effects in the etiology of schizophrenia.
Figure 2 presents the variance component estimates from Mx for 12 twin studies of schizophrenia and the meta-analytic summary estimates. Figure 2A depicts additive genetic effects, and Figure 2B depicts common environmental effects.
For additive genetic effects, the point estimates from all but 2 studies23,24 are in excess of 50%. The 95% CIs for the estimates are often large because of the relatively small sample sizes of the individual studies. The meta-analytic summary for additive genetic variance in liability to schizophrenia was estimated at 81% (95% CI, 73%-90%). The 95% CI for the joint estimate overlapped with the 95% CIs for 1019-21,23,26-31 of the 12 studies.
For common environmental effects, the point estimates for 719-21,23-25,29 of the 12 studies were nonzero, with 6 greater than 15%. The 95% CIs tended to be large owing to the limited statistical power to detect this effect in the individual studies. The meta-analytic summary for common environmental effects for the liability to schizophrenia was estimated at 11% and, notably, the 95% CI did not include 0 (95% CI, 3%-19%). The 95% CI for this estimate overlapped with the CIs of all but 223,24 of the 12 studies.
An inspection of Figure 1 and Figure 2 suggests that there is heterogeneity across the 12 individual twin studies. A formal test for homogeneity was strongly rejected (χ220 = 113.9, P<.001). Critically, when we compared the methodologically superior studies27-29,31 with the methodologically inferior studies,19-21,23-26,30 we found similar point estimates for additive genetic effects (77% vs 78%) and common environmental effects (17% vs 14%).
In addition, 623-26,30,31 of the 12 twin studies used unique population prevalence estimates for schizophrenia. Because prevalence can influence the variance component estimates, we performed the analyses again, with all studies forced to have population prevalences for schizophrenia of 0.5%, 0.75%, and 1%. The pattern of results was similar to that described previously, with high estimates of additive genetic effects and estimates of common environmental effects whose 95% CIs did not include 0. Estimates of common environmental effects were larger with decreasing prevalence.
One principal result of this quantitative meta-analysis of 12 published twin studies of schizophrenia was expected, and the other was quite surprising. Consistent with prior summaries of the twin literature on schizophrenia, the meta-analytic summary estimate of its heritability is high (point estimate, 81%; 95% CI, 73%-90%), and this result may provide a useful summary of a diverse literature. However, we also determined that there are small but significant common environmental effects on liability to schizophrenia (point estimate, 11%; 95% CI, 3%-19%). This latter estimate is unexpected and of considerable interest.
Before discussing these findings further, it is imperative that we consider 2 key limitations. First, the methodological quality of the published twin studies of schizophrenia was not uniformly high compared with, for example, that for major depression37 or various smoking behaviors.38 Most studies in this work did not include several critical features (blinding and a standardized diagnostic approach) that are generally viewed as central to the interpretability of twin studies of medical disorders. In fairness, most of these twin studies of schizophrenia were conducted before the importance of these methodological features was widely recognized and viewed as essential. In fact, many of the earlier twin studies represented monumental and even heroic efforts by individual psychiatrists who devoted years of personal effort despite limited resources to accumulate the samples quantitatively summarized herein. For the reasons described in the "Primary Studies" subsection of the "Methods" section, we chose to include 12 of 14 studies that met post hoc modifications of our a priori criteria.
Second, the 12 studies included in this meta-analysis of schizophrenia were statistically heterogeneous. The presence of heterogeneity of the variance component estimates raises 2 questions—What is the source of heterogeneity? and Does it limit the validity of the meta-analytic estimates? It is likely that differences in methods across studies are a source of heterogeneity given that the 4 studies27-29,31 that met our a priori inclusion criteria had greater evidence for the homogeneity of the components of variance. This is unsurprising, and is consistent with the increased reliability generally found with more rigorous methodological approaches to psychiatric diagnosis. In addition, heterogeneity could have resulted from other sample-specific characteristics, like the male-female or MZ/DZ ratios. However, it is also possible that there exists true variation in the etiology of schizophrenia during the decades spanned by these studies or across the different countries and ethnic ancestries of the individuals in these studies. There are reports of associations of schizophrenia with potential etiological factors that would be expected to vary across populations and over time, such as discrete periods of famine,39 season of birth,40 or prenatal exposure to influenza or other viral infections.41-43
The presence of heterogeneity across studies does not necessarily invalidate the meta-analytic approach we used to summarize these studies. Rather, there is a set of advantages and disadvantages. The critical advantage of including heterogeneous studies is the capacity to summarize the variance component estimates when there is true etiological variation. It is unlikely that schizophrenia has a single etiology, but instead is an end result of heterogeneous processes that result in a similar clinical portrait. Therefore, the inclusion of heterogeneous studies is consistent with this belief and offers the potential of a more accurate and less sample-specific summary of the fundamental nature of this complex illness. On the other hand, the critical disadvantage is if heterogeneity is an index of a shared methodological flaw (eg, bias in recruitment or diagnosis). In this instance, heterogeneity reflects the presence of individual studies that are flawed and could result in imprecise and inaccurate estimates.
On balance, we believe that it is more advantageous than disadvantageous to include heterogeneous studies. This contention is supported by the similarity of parameter estimates from the methodologically superior vs inferior studies. In fact, the point estimate for common environmental effects was higher in the superior studies (17%) than in the inferior studies (14%).
Common environment and schizophrenia
The most notable finding from this meta-analysis was that variance in liability to schizophrenia was estimated to have a nonzero contribution of environmental influences shared by members of a twin pair. This finding is ironic because it is unusual to find a behavioral trait or disorder with significant common environmental influences44,45 and schizophrenia is often described as one of the more "genetic" psychiatric disorders. The magnitude of the finding suggests that these influences, while significant, have a modest impact on liability to schizophrenia.
When considering this surprising finding further, we discovered that significant common environmental effects for schizophrenia were reported previously by Rao et al (20%)9 and McGue et al (19%).10 When these 2 articles were published in the early 1980s, there was a sharp division within psychiatry as to whether schizophrenia resulted from biological/genetic factors or environmental factors, such as adverse maternal-child relationships. These perspectives were often framed as mutually exclusive (nature or nurture). The stronger genetic component to schizophrenia was clearly the more influential result from these articles. Our rediscovery of subtle, but nontrivial, common environmental effects for schizophrenia is likely to be interpreted differently than in the 1980s.
The traditional phrases, common environment and shared environment, are misnomers in that they generally evoke psychiatric risk factors like parental rearing behavior and traumatic life events. In the context of the assumptions and definitions of twin analyses, however, common environment refers to any process that makes members of a twin pair similar regardless of zygosity. These processes include the classic environmental factors previously noted, but also encompass profoundly biological processes such as exposure to infectious agents, macronutrient or micronutrient dietary characteristics, and exposure to environmental toxins, teratogens, and other intrauterine factors. In addition, it is possible that some portions of the significant common environmental effects are artifactual (eg, due to assortative mating or a substantially biased zygosity assignment).
Moreover, the environments of members of twin pairs tend to diverge over time. The environments of twins are most similar in utero and in the immediate postnatal period, with increasing divergence over infancy, childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Therefore, the presence of significant common environmental effects on liability to schizophrenia suggests that these effects would be most likely to occur early in life. This prediction is consistent with a neurodevelopmental etiology of schizophrenia46-48 and with the literature on early risk factors for schizophrenia. For example, several reviews49,50—including a recent meta-analysis51 of large, prospective, and population-based studies—found consistent evidence to support the status of pregnancy complications, abnormal fetal development, and delivery complications as risk factors for schizophrenia. An additional report52 strongly suggests the importance of maternal-fetal Rh blood group incompatibility as a specific risk factor for schizophrenia.
In conclusion, these meta-analytic results from 12 published twin studies of schizophrenia support a view of schizophrenia as a complex trait that results from both genetic and shared environmental etiological influences. These results are broadly informative in that they provide no information about the specific number or identity of these etiological influences, but provide a component of a unifying empirical basis supporting the rationality of searches for underlying genetic and common environmental etiological factors.
Corresponding author: Patrick F. Sullivan, MD, FRANZCP, Department of Genetics, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Campus Box 7264, Chapel Hill, NC 27599 (e-mail: pfsulliv@med.unc.edu).
Submitted for publication December 2, 2002; final revision received April 2, 2003; accepted April 11, 2003.
This study was supported by grant MH-01458 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Neale).
We thank Irving I. Gottesman, PhD, for critical comments on earlier drafts of this article.
1.Kendler
KSDiehl
SR Schizophrenia: genetics. Kaplan
HISadock
BJeds.
Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. 6th Baltimore, Md Williams & Wilkins1995;942- 957
Google Scholar 2.Altmüller
JPalmer
LJFischer
GScherb
HWjst
M Genomewide scans of complex human diseases: true linkage is hard to find.
Am J Hum Genet. 2001;69936- 950
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Levinson
DFHolmans
PALaurent
CRiley
BPulver
AEGejman
PVSchwab
SGWilliams
NMOwen
MJWildenauer
DBSanders
ARNestadt
GMowry
BJWormley
BBauche
SSoubigou
SRibble
RNertney
DALiang
KYMartinolich
LMaier
WNorton
NWilliams
HAlbus
MCarpenter
EBDeMarchi
NEwen-White
KRWalsh
DJay
MDeleuze
JFO'Neill
FAPapadimitriou
GWeilbaecher
ALerer
BO'Donovan
MCDikeos
DSilverman
JMKendler
KSMallet
JCrowe
RRWalters
M No major schizophrenia locus detected on chromosome 1q in a large multicenter sample.
Science. 2002;296739- 741
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Stefansson
HSigurdsson
ESteinthorsdottir
VBjornsdottir
SSigmundsson
TGhosh
SBrynjolfsson
JGunnarsdottir
SIvarsson
OChou
TTHjaltason
OBirgisdottir
BJonsson
HGudnadottir
VGGudmundsdottir
EBjornsson
AIngvarsson
BIngason
ASigfusson
SHadardottir
HHarvey
RPLai
DZhou
MBrunner
DMutel
VGonzalo
ALemke
GSainz
JJohannesson
GAndresson
JGudbjartsson
DManolescu
AFrigge
MLGurney
MEKong
AGulcher
JRPetursson
HStefansson
K Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to schizophrenia.
Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71877- 892
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Straub
REJiang
YMacLean
CJMa
YWebb
BTMyakishev
MVHarris-Kerr
CWormley
BSadek
HKadambi
BCesare
AJGibberman
AWang
XO'Neill
FAWalsh
DKendler
KS Genetic variation in the 6p22.3 gene DTNBP1, the human ortholog of the mouse dysbindin gene, is associated with schizophrenia.
Am J Hum Genet. 2002;71337- 348
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.van den Oord
EJSullivan
PFJiang
YWalsh
DO'Neill
FAKendler
KSRiley
BP Identification of a high-risk haplotype for the dystrobrevin binding protein 1 (
DTNBP1) gene in the Irish study of high-density schizophrenia families.
Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8499- 510
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Gottesman
IIShields
J Contributions of twin studies to perspectives on schizophrenia. Maher
BAed.
Contributions to the Psychopathology of Schizophrenia. New York, NY Academic Press1977;169- 266
Google Scholar 9.Rao
DCMorton
NEGottesman
IILew
R Path analysis of qualitative data on pairs of relatives: application to schizophrenia.
Hum Hered. 1981;31325- 333
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.McGue
MGottesman
IIRao
DC The transmission of schizophrenia under a multifactorial threshold model.
Am J Hum Genet. 1983;351161- 1178
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Kendler
KS Overview: a current perspective on twin studies of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1983;1401413- 1425
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Cardno
AGGottesman
II Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to Star Wars Mx and functional genomics.
Am J Med Genet. 2000;9712- 17
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Glass
GVMcGaw
BSmith
ML Meta-analysis in Social Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif Sage Publications1981;
14.Smith
MLGlass
GVMiller
TI The Benefits of Psychotherapy. Baltimore, Md Johns Hopkins University Press1980;
16.Petitti
DB Meta-analysis, Decision Analysis, and Cost-effectiveness Analysis. New York, NY Oxford University Press Inc1994;
17.Neale
MCEaves
LJKendler
KS The power of the classical twin study to resolve variation in threshold traits.
Behav Genet. 1994;24239- 258
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Rosanoff
AJHandy
LMPlesset
IRBrush
S The etiology of so-called schizophrenic psychoses with special reference to their occurrence in twins.
Am J Psychiatry. 1934;91247- 286
Google Scholar 19.Essen-Møller
E Psychiatrische Untersuchungen an einer Serie von Zwillingen.
Acta Psychiatr Neurol Scand. 1941;231- 200
Google Scholar 20.Kallmann
FJ The genetic theory of schizophrenia: an analysis of 691 schizophrenic twin index families.
Am J Psychiatry. 1946;103309- 322
Google Scholar 21.Slater
EShields
J Psychotic and Neurotic Illnesses in Twins. London, England Her Majesty's Stationery Office1953;
22.Inouye
E Similarity and Dissimilarity of Schizophrenia in Twins: Proceedings of the Third World Congress of Psychiatry. Toronto, Ontario University of Toronto Press1961;
23.Kringlen
F Heredity and Environment in the Functional Psychoses: An Epidemiological Twin Study. Oslo, Norway Universitetsforlaget1967;
25.Tienari
P Schizophrenia in Finnish male twins. Lader
MHed.
Studies of Schizophrenia. Kent, England Headley Brothers1975;29- 35
Google Scholar 26.Kendler
KSRobinette
CD Schizophrenia in the National Academy of Sciences–National Research Council Twin Registry: a 16-year update.
Am J Psychiatry. 1983;1401551- 1563
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Kläning
U Schizophrenia in Twins. Aarhus, Denmark University of Aarhus1996;
29.Franzek
EBeckman
H Psychoses of the Schizophrenic Spectrum in Twins. Vienna, Austria Springer-Verlag1999;
30.Cannon
TDKaprio
JLonnqvist
JHuttunen
MKoskenvuo
M The genetic epidemiology of schizophrenia in a Finnish twin cohort: a population-based modeling study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;5567- 74
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Cardno
AGMarshall
EJCoid
BMacdonald
AMRibchester
TRDavies
NJVenturi
PJones
LALewis
SWSham
PCGottesman
IIFarmer
AEMcGuffin
PReveley
AMMurray
RM Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley twin psychosis series.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56162- 168
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Neale
MCCardon
LR Methodology for the Study of Twins and Families. Dordrecht, the Netherlands Kluwer Academic Publisher Group1992;
33.Falconer
DS The inheritance of liability to certain diseases, estimated from the incidence among relatives.
Ann Hum Genet. 1965;2951- 76
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Essen-Møller
E Twenty-one psychiatric cases and their MZ co-twins: a thirty years' follow-up.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma). 1970;19315- 317
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Neale
MCBoker
SXie
GMaes
H Mx: Statistical Modeling. 5th Richmond, VA Virginia Commonwealth University, Department of Psychiatry2001;
37.Sullivan
PFNeale
MCKendler
KS The genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;1571552- 1562
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Hoek
HWBrown
ASSusser
E The Dutch famine and schizophrenia spectrum disorders.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1998;33373- 379
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Suvisaari
JMHaukka
JKLonnqvist
JK Season of birth among patients with schizophrenia and their siblings: evidence for the procreational habits hypothesis.
Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158754- 757
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Rantakallio
PJones
PMoring
JVon Wendt
L Association between central nervous system infections during childhood and adult onset schizophrenia and other psychoses: a 28-year follow-up.
Int J Epidemiol. 1997;26837- 843
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Westergaard
TMortensen
PBPedersen
CBWohlfahrt
JMelbye
M Exposure to prenatal and childhood infections and the risk of schizophrenia: suggestions from a study of sibship characteristics and influenza prevalence.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56993- 998
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Munk-Jorgensen
PEwald
H Epidemiology in neurobiological research: exemplified by the influenza-schizophrenia theory.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 2001;40s30- s32
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Plomin
RDaniels
D Why are children in the same family so different from one another?
Behav Brain Sci. 1987;101- 16
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Marenco
SWeinberger
DR The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia: following a trail of evidence from cradle to grave.
Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12501- 527
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Cannon
MJones
PBMurray
RM Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: historical and meta-analytic review.
Am J Psychiatry. 2002;1591080- 1092
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Palmer
CGTurunen
JASinsheimer
JSMinassian
SPaunio
TLonnqvist
JPeltonen
LWoodward
JA RHD maternal-fetal genotype incompatibility increases schizophrenia susceptibility.
Am J Hum Genet. 2002;711312- 1319
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref