Background
Dysfunction of frontal-striatal, particularly orbitofrontal-striatal, circuitry has been implicated in the pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), characterized by obsessions, ritualistic behavior, anxiety, and specific cognitive impairments. In addition, neuropsychological studies in OCD have reported impairments in visuospatial tasks and executive functions, such as planning.
Objective
To determine whether dorsal prefrontal-striatal dysfunction mediates planning impairment in patients with OCD.
Design
A parametric self-paced pseudorandomized event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging version of the Tower of London task was used in 22 medication-free patients with OCD and 22 healthy control subjects. This paradigm, allowing flexible responding and post hoc classification of correct responses, was developed to compare groups likely to differ in performance.
Results
Behavioral results showed significant planning impairments in OCD patients compared with control subjects. During planning, decreased frontal-striatal responsiveness was found in OCD patients, mainly in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and caudate nucleus. In addition, OCD patients showed increased, presumably compensatory, involvement of brain areas known to play a role in performance monitoring and short-term memory processing, such as anterior cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal, and parahippocampal cortices.
Conclusions
These findings support the hypothesis that decreased dorsal prefrontal-striatal responsiveness is associated with impaired planning capacity in OCD patients. Because the described frontal-striatal dysfunction in OCD is independent of state anxiety and disease symptom severity, we conclude that executive impairment is a core feature in OCD.
Clinical, neurosurgical, and functional neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that dysfunctional prefrontal cortex (PFC)–basal ganglia circuits are involved in the pathogenesis of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The high co-occurrence between OCD and other basal ganglia disorders strongly supports the so-called frontal-striatal hypothesis.1,2 In addition, recent neuropsychological studies3,4 have shown cognitive impairments in OCD, particularly with regard to visuospatial processing, executive functioning, and motor speed. Other cognitive domains appear to remain intact, indicating a specific, rather than a general, cognitive deficit.
Executive functioning implies different subdomains of higher-order cognitive functioning. Planning, that is, the ability to achieve a goal through a series of intermediate steps, is an essential component of higher-order cognitive processing, such as problem solving. Using a neuronal network model, Dehaene and Changeux5 proposed multiple hierarchical levels coding for specialized subprocesses of planning, such as plan generation, working memory, and internal evaluation and reward. Some subprocesses seem to be relatively independent of task load, ie, increasing planning complexity, while other subprocesses are mainly involved at higher levels of planning behavior.
A frequently used test to probe planning processes is the Tower of London task (ToL), adapted from the Tower of Hanoi task.6,7 The ToL has been used to investigate planning in healthy control subjects using positron emission tomography,8-12 single-photon emission computed tomography,13,14 and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).12,15-20 The results of these imaging studies agree on the involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and parietal-occipital regions during planning. However, activation of other brain regions, such as cingulate9,10,12,16,17,19 and insular9,10,17 cortices, striatum,8,10,12,16,17,20 and rostral PFC,9,10,17 has not been found across all mentioned studies. These inconsistencies are likely to be explained by methodological differences between these studies, such as group size, scanning modality, analysis technique (regions of interest vs whole-brain analysis), and details of task paradigms. For example, baseline conditions have not been uniform (low level vs matched for visual complexity and motor demands). Also, some paradigms require mental execution, whereas in others a touch screen is used. Keith Berg and Byrd21 emphasized the potential effect of such modifications, providing recommendations for constructing, for example, computerized versions of the ToL to increase comparability across studies.
Neuropsychological studies of planning ability, as a measure of executive functioning, in patients with OCD have not provided wholly consistent results. Veale et al,22 using a computerized version of the ToL, found no difference in accuracy between OCD patients and control subjects. However, when OCD patients made a mistake, they spent more time than controls in generating alternative solutions or in checking next responses. Normal accuracy in planning execution in OCD was also found by Schmidtke et al,23 using the Tower of Hanoi task; however, in this study, no response time (RT) data were collected. Results of a study by Purcell et al,24 comparing neuropsychological profiles of patients with OCD, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (MDD), highlight the importance of task implementation. Although motor speed was decreased, OCD patients showed a normal ability to organize and execute a series of goal-directed moves on a planning task when using a touch screen, providing external validation of ongoing performance. In contrast, when the task had to be executed mentally, OCD patients were significantly impaired. Whether impaired executive functioning is a trait feature of OCD, that is, not secondary to present mood or anxiety symptoms, is not yet clear. Impaired performance relative to controls was found in a study25 of subclinical obsessive-compulsive subjects. However, in this study, it was also found that performance was inversely correlated with symptom severity, particularly severity of checking behavior.
Although these neuropsychological studies, together with functional imaging studies in healthy subjects, suggest that the executive impairment found in OCD patients reflects dorsal prefrontal-striatal dysfunction, so far no imaging study has been published in which this hypothesis has been investigated. Functional imaging studies using executive tasks have been performed in schizophrenia26 and MDD,27 whereas in OCD most studies have used symptom provocation paradigms. However, evidence of striatal dysfunction in OCD has been provided in a positron emission tomography study using an implicit learning task.28,29 In this study, OCD patients showed increased activity of posterior (temporal and parietal) cortical regions relative to controls, explained as compensatory mechanisms.
The aim of the present study was to investigate dorsal prefrontal-striatal function during performance of a planning task in OCD patients compared with controls. We used a parametric self-paced pseudorandomized event-related version of the ToL,17 suitable for fMRI. A self-paced parametric design allows flexible responding, as well as comparisons between subjects or groups at each task level, resulting in increased comparability across groups with varying levels of performance. In addition, an event-related design enables post hoc classification of events based on subjects’ responses, so that correct responses and errors can be analyzed separately. Based on previous studies, we expected task performance in OCD subjects to be impaired; in addition, we hypothesized this planning deficit to be reflected in decreased responsiveness of striatal and dorsal prefrontal regions as assessed using fMRI.
Twenty-two OCD patients (mean age, 34.4 years [age range, 21-49 years]; 7 men and 15 women) and 22 healthy control subjects (mean age, 29.9 years [age range, 23-51 years]; 11 men and 11 women) performed the ToL while fMRI data were collected. All subjects were right-handed, as assessed during a medical interview. Exclusion criteria were the presence of major medical illness, co-occurrence of other major psychiatric disorders, and the use of psychotropic medication. Subjects had to be off medication for at least 4 weeks. Diagnoses were established using the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.30 The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale31 and the Padua Inventory–Revised32,33 were used to assess symptom characteristics and severity scores (Table 1). Patients were recruited from the Outpatient Clinic for Anxiety Disorders and Dutch Foundation for Anxiety Disorders, Driebergen, the Netherlands. The ethical review board of the VU University Medical Center approved the study, and all participants provided written informed consent.
A pseudorandomized self-paced version of the ToL was used, discussed in detail previously.17 This version consisted of 6 conditions, including a baseline condition and 5 planning conditions, ranging from 1 to 5 moves. In the planning conditions, subjects saw a starting configuration together with a target configuration, with the instruction to “count the number of steps.” Two possible answers were shown, from which the correct one had to be selected (Figure 1A). In both configurations, 3 colored beads were placed on 3 vertical rods, which could accommodate 1, 2, and 3 beads each. One bead could be moved at a time and only when there was no other bead on top. Subjects were requested to determine the minimum number of moves necessary to reach the target configuration and to press the button corresponding to the side (left or right) of the screen where the correct answer was presented. In the baseline condition, subjects simply had to count the total number of yellow and blue beads (Figure 1B). A pseudorandomized design was adopted to control for any overflow effects (ie, perseverance of task-related cognitive processes after a difficult trial). Therefore, each trial of 3 or more moves was followed by a baseline trial. No feedback regarding the answers was provided during the task. A maximum RT of 30 seconds for each trial was applied. After performing the task, subjects were asked to rate subjective distress using 100-point analogue scales. To ensure that participants were familiar with the procedure, the test was explained and practiced outside the scanner before fMRI was performed.
Imaging was performed on a 1.5-T Sonata system (Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard circularly polarized head coil. Stimuli were generated by a Pentium personal computer (Dell Inc, Round Rock, Tex) and projected on a screen at the end of the scanner table, which was seen through a mirror mounted above the subject’s head. Two magnet-compatible 4-key response boxes were used to record the subject’s performance and RTs. To reduce motion artifacts, the subject’s head was immobilized using foam pads.
Anatomic imaging included a coronal 3-dimensional gradient-echo T1-weighted sequence (matrix, 256×160 pixels; voxel size, 1×1×1.5 mm; 160 sections). For fMRI, an echoplanar imaging sequence (repetition time, 3.045 seconds; echo time, 45 milliseconds; matrix, 64×64 pixels; field of view, 192×192 mm; flip angle, 90°) was used, creating transversal whole-brain acquisitions (35 slices, 3×3-mm in-plane resolution, 2.5-mm slice thickness, 0.5-mm interslice gap). In total, 433 echoplanar imaging volumes per subject were scanned. The distribution frequency of event types was based on RT data of a pilot study, so that a similar number of scans (approximately 70 echoplanar imaging volumes) was acquired per subject for each of the 6 conditions.
Demographic and behavioral data were analyzed using SPSS software (version 11.0; SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill). Imaging data were analyzed using SPM99 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, England). After discarding the first 4 volumes, time series were corrected for differences in slice acquisition times and realigned. Spatial normalization into approximate Talairach and Tournoux space was performed using a standard statistical parametric mapping echoplanar imaging template. Data were resliced to 2×2×2-mm voxels and spatially smoothed using a 6-mm gaussian kernel.
Next, data were analyzed in the context of the general linear model, using delta functions convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function to model responses of varying lengths to each type of stimulus. In addition, error trials were modeled separately as a regressor of no interest. For each subject, weighted contrasts were computed for main effects, that is, all active conditions vs baseline. In addition, specific linear contrasts for task load were computed.34 Contrast images containing parameter estimates for main effects and task load were entered into a second-level (random effects) analysis. Main effects for each group are reported at P<.05 corrected for multiple comparisons; group interaction effects (masked with the appropriate main effect) are reported at an uncorrected threshold of P<.001.
Demographic and behavioral data
The groups did not differ significantly with respect to age and sex ratio. Education level, however, was higher in control subjects. The OCD symptom severity, as measured with the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale and Padua Inventory–Revised questionnaires, was significantly higher in patients (Table 1). Analysis of variance of behavioral data showed significant differences in performance between OCD patients and controls at all task levels, whereas RTs were longer in OCD patients compared with controls only in the 2 easiest planning conditions (Table 2). The mean ± SD subjective distress scores were significantly higher in the OCD group compared with the control group (42.1±25.5 vs 21.2±11.8, F1,42 = 12.4; P<.001). Within groups, performance was not significantly correlated with trait or state measures (P>.10 for all).
In controls, regions showing increased blood oxygenation level–dependent signal during planning compared with baseline (Table 3) were found bilaterally in dorsolateral prefrontal (Brodmann areas [BAs] 9 and 46), motor and premotor (BAs 4, 6, and 8), inferior parietal (BA 40), and insular and superior occipital (BA 19) cortices, as well as in bilateral precuneus (BA 7), right caudate nucleus, and left globus pallidus. The OCD patients showed increased blood oxygenation level–dependent signal in most of these regions as well. However, in contrast to control subjects, no activation was found in the striatum during planning in OCD patients. In addition, activation was found in right cingulate cortex (BA 32). Group-by-task interaction analyses (Table 4) showed increased activation in controls compared with OCD patients in right DLPFC (BAs 9 and 46), right premotor cortex (BA 6), left cingulate cortex (BA 32), bilateral precuneus (BA 7), left inferior parietal cortex (BA 40), right caudate nucleus (Figure 2), and left putamen. No significant group-by-task interactions were found in OCD patients compared with controls.
In controls, increased task load (Table 5) was correlated with increased blood oxygenation level–dependent signal bilaterally in anterior prefrontal (BA 10), dorsolateral prefrontal (BAs 9 and 46), cingulate (BA 32), premotor (BAs 6 and 8), and insular cortices and in the precuneus (BA 7). Furthermore, increases were observed in right inferior parietal cortex (BA 40), left superior occipital cortex (BA 19), and left caudate nucleus. The OCD patients, in contrast, did not show increased activity in the caudate nucleus correlating with task load. Instead, task load was correlated with increased activation bilaterally in supplementary motor area, as well as left posterior globus pallidus, left parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), thalamus and dorsal brainstem, and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) (BA 44). Group-by-task interaction analyses (Table 6, Figure 3, and Figure 4) showed increased activation in controls compared with OCD patients in left DLPFC (BA 46). In contrast, OCD patients, compared with controls, showed increased activation bilaterally in cingulate (BA 32), ventrolateral prefrontal (BAs 45 and 47), and parahippocampal cortices, as well as left anterior temporal cortex and dorsal brainstem.
In OCD subjects, neither main effects for task nor task load effects were associated with symptom severity scores (Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale and Padua Inventory–Revised), except for a small region in left VLPFC (BA 44) (z = 3.24). Additional analyses of covariance were performed to investigate whether the observed between-group differences could be explained by differences in demographic variables or levels of subjective distress. However, the already described group-by-task interaction effects in favor of control subjects persisted after controlling for differences in age, education level, sex ratio, and state anxiety.
In the present study, a parametric self-paced pseudorandomized event-related fMRI version of the ToL was used to investigate the neural substrate of planning in medication-free OCD patients compared with healthy control subjects. This paradigm allowed flexible responding and post hoc selection of correct trials, so that analyses of imaging data were not confounded by performance differences.35 The present results not only confirm previous findings with regard to impaired planning capacity in OCD but also clearly demonstrate decreased responsiveness of dorsal prefrontal-striatal circuits in OCD patients.
Behavioral data showed increased RTs in OCD patients only during the 2 easiest task levels, whereas performance scores were significantly lower compared with those of control subjects across all levels. These behavioral findings are in agreement with some, but not all, previous findings with regard to planning performance in OCD. Although some studies have reported decreased response speed24 or performance scores,25 other studies22,23 found unaffected planning capacity in OCD patients compared with controls. As discussed previously, these differences are likely to reflect methodological differences in task implementation, such as mental performance vs the use of a touch screen or providing feedback vs no feedback.21,24
Imaging results showed increased task-associated activation in controls compared with OCD patients in several regions previously found to be involved in planning, particularly DLPFC, basal ganglia, and parietal cortex. Task load–correlated activity was found in left DLPFC in control subjects compared with OCD patients. In contrast, OCD patients showed increased activation, correlated with increased task load, in bilateral cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal, and parahippocampal cortices, in left anterior temporal cortex, and in dorsal brainstem. Our finding of decreased responsiveness of dorsal prefrontal-striatal circuits during planning in OCD patients is in accord with previous findings with regard to basal ganglia dysfunction in OCD in implicit learning.28,29 However, in the study by Rauch et al,28 OCD subjects showed impaired performance during motor skill acquisition, suggesting basal ganglia–motor cortical rather than dorsal prefrontal-striatal dysfunction, as found in the present study. In addition, several functional imaging studies36-40 using symptom provocation designs have reported abnormalities in orbitofrontal-striatal function in OCD, thought to reflect its role in ritualistic behavior. Most of these studies,36,37,40 however, lacked adequate control groups or failed to find significant group-by-task interactions in subcortical areas. Increased prefrontal-subcortical glucose uptake in OCD patients relative to controls, resolving after successful therapy, has also been reported in several resting-state imaging studies.41-45 Although dorsal prefrontal abnormalities were apparently found in 1 study,43 most of these studies reported increased orbitofrontal-striatal metabolism. Moreover, although dorsal PFC and ventral PFC are known to project to dorsal and ventral parts of the striatum, spatial resolution of positron emission tomography with [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose may have been insufficient to detect differences based on topographical organization within the basal ganglia. Therefore, the evidence for pathologically increased baseline activity in dorsal prefrontal-striatal, as opposed to ventral prefrontal-striatal, loops in OCD is still inconclusive. However, although the present fMRI data do not allow baseline comparisons, our finding of decreased or absent prefrontal-striatal responsiveness is compatible with existing pathophysiological models of OCD hypothesizing basal ganglia disinhibition due to an altered balance between indirect, inhibitory, and direct excitatory cortico-striatal-thalamico-cortical circuits.46,47 Whether the failure of OCD patients to recruit dorsal prefrontal-striatal regions compared with controls, as found in the present study, is specific for planning tasks is also not yet clear. In a recent fMRI study, van der Wee et al48 found decreased performance only at the highest task level in OCD patients compared with controls during performance of a spatial n-back task. Imaging results showed similar activity in bilateral DLPFC and parietal cortex in both groups, from which the authors concluded that (spatial) working memory in OCD was not abnormal. The findings of the present study provide support for this conclusion. Whereas it might be argued that in our paradigm not only planning complexity but also working memory load was (linearly) increased, our results reveal impaired planning at all levels, which is difficult to explain by differences in working memory capacity.
In the present study, OCD patients, compared with control subjects, showed increased activation, correlated with task load, in bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and parahippocampal cortices, in left temporal cortex, and in dorsal brainstem. These interaction effects may reflect compensatory processes and increased arousal. Functional neuroanatomical subdivisions of the lateral PFC (ie, VLPFC and DLPFC) have been proposed based on stimulus type (verbal vs object or spatial)49,50 and process type (maintenance or manipulation), with recent evidence favoring the latter hypothesis.51,52 Therefore, the VLPFC (BAs 44, 45, and 47) supports processes that transfer, maintain, and match information in working memory. In contrast, the DLPFC (BAs 9 and 46) supports complex processes operating on information (spatial and nonspatial) that is maintained in working memory, such as monitoring, manipulation, and higher-level planning. Beyond maintenance in VLPFC and manipulation in DLPFC, the anterior PFC (BAs 8 and 10) is associated with processes of a third level of executive control.53 Increased VLPFC activity may reflect not only working memory load, that is, the number of items kept “online,” but also retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules used for problem solving.54 In addition, it has been shown that VLPFC activity is selectively increased during arithmetic computations, particularly when subjects attempt to resolve a conflict between externally presented answers and internally computed solutions.55 Such a mechanism may explain the increased activity of bilateral VLPFC associated with task load in OCD patients compared with control subjects observed in the present study.
Compared with controls, OCD patients also showed increased activity of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (BA 32) correlated with task load. Anterior cingulate cortex involvement in OCD has been observed at rest, during symptom provocation, and after having committed errors during cognitive tasks.37,40 The ACC has been implicated in performance monitoring, error detection, conflict monitoring, response selection, and reward expectation.56 Anterior cingulate cortex activity has been found before a response during correct trials and immediately following error trials, reflecting its role in error prevention and detection.57 Ursu et al58 reported increased activation of ACC in OCD patients, compared with control subjects, during error and correct trials, indicative of an overactive performance monitoring system in OCD, independent of the actual occurrence of errors. These results were replicated in a subclinical group of obsessive-compulsive undergraduates.59 In addition to ACC, supplementary motor area may have a role in performance monitoring.60 Increased activity in ACC and supplementary motor area was also observed during performance of a spatial n-back task in OCD patients, thought to reflect increased effort to develop an efficient strategy or increased error monitoring.48 A functional neuroanatomical dissociation between these areas has been proposed, with error processing associated with the ACC region and response competition with supplementary motor area.61 Increased performance monitoring during correct task performance, as well as during errors, may be characteristic of the critical self-evaluation of performance in OCD, leading to inappropriate need for correction and, consequently, repetitive behavior.
Increased activity correlated with task load was also observed in bilateral PHG in OCD patients relative to controls. Similar results were obtained by Rauch et al28 in OCD patients during a motor sequence learning paradigm. The authors hypothesized that dysfunction of corticostriatal systems in OCD resulted in compensatory recruitment of “explicit networks” to perform the task. However, data from their study showed that OCD patients did not differ from controls in explicit knowledge with regard to the task. In the present study, PHG involvement may reflect intermediate-term memory for spatial information, in addition to working memory, supported by parietal and lateral PFC as already discussed, and long-term memory (more than about 2 minutes) provided by the hippocampal formation.62 Increased activity of PHG and VLPFC may therefore be secondary to OCD patients’ failure to develop an adequate strategy, so that they need to rely on short- and intermediate-term memory capacity to perform tasks.3 Connections exist between rostral ACC (BA 32) and PHG.63 Faw64 described this system of ACC and adjacent dorsomedial PFC, with extensions to the hippocampal stream, as a key player in spatiotemporal processing and attention, supporting executive functions located in DLPFC.
Finally, we found an area of increased activation in left dorsal brainstem associated with task load in OCD patients compared with control subjects. This region was located within the reticular formation, extending rostrally into the periaqueductal gray, and may therefore reflect increased arousal at higher task loads in our OCD group.65,66 Arousal responses implicate activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the reticular activating system. The reticular activating system is composed of cholinergic neurons from the pedunculopontine nucleus, stimulating the noradrenergic system of the locus ceruleus (LC), which in turn inhibits cholinergic output of the pedunculopontine nucleus.65 Evidence from nonstress experiments suggests that phasic LC activity is associated with increased attention and task-related performance,66-68 whereas increased tonic activity of the LC results in attentional instability, decreased performance, and increased emotional reactivity.66,68 Chronic stress, on the other hand, may lead to LC damage, resulting in reduced output from the LC69 and disinhibition of the pedunculopontine nucleus.70 In the present study, our finding that increased dorsal brainstem activity in OCD was associated with task load suggests increased effort rather than increased subjective distress, although we cannot rule out the latter possibility.
To our knowledge, this study is the first to demonstrate impaired planning associated with decreased dorsal prefrontal-striatal responsiveness in OCD. Strengths of the study are the inclusion of medication-free subjects, the provision of a large group size permitting random-effects analyses, and the use of a parametric self-paced event-related design. The present study is not without limitations, however. First, education level was higher in control subjects, although we controlled for performance differences by selecting correct responses only. In addition, post hoc analyses of covariance with regard to education level, sex ratio, and age were performed. Moreover, it has been shown that planning performance (error rates and RTs) is not correlated with intelligence measures.71 Second, although in the present study, in accord with previous studies,3,48 no clear correlations were found between symptom severity ratings and outcome measures, we did not specifically investigate OCD subgroups, for example, those with prominent washing or checking symptoms. Although little is known with regard to the generalizability of neuropsychological deficits across clinical subtypes, a dimensional model of OCD has been proposed, in which various symptom dimensions are associated with differential patterns of functional neuroanatomical abnormalities.72,73 Recent data showed that activation of VLPFC and caudate nucleus correlated with washing characteristics and activation of dorsal regions (DLPFC, thalamus, putamen, and globus pallidus) correlated with checking symptoms.73 Based on these subtype differences in provocation experiments, one might hypothesize that impaired planning and decreased responsiveness of dorsal prefrontal-striatal circuits as found in the present study mainly concern OCD patients with predominant checking symptoms. Another issue for future research is whether the dorsal prefrontal-striatal dysfunction observed in this study is specific for OCD or extends to other neuropsychiatric disorders. These include not only anxiety disorders but also basal ganglia disorders, such as Tourette syndrome and MDD. Major depressive disorder is characterized by impairments in various executive functions, such as verbal fluency and attentional set shifting.74 However, in MDD, dorsal prefrontal baseline perfusion is decreased rather than increased.75 Moreover, MDD is not associated with striatal pathologic conditions.76 Whereas depressive symptoms frequently co-occur in OCD, cognitive deficits in OCD are not associated with comorbid depression,3 suggesting different pathophysiological mechanisms in OCD compared with MDD. Future studies should also investigate whether the executive deficits in OCD are specific for the present paradigm or can be replicated during other “strategic” tasks, such as set-shifting tasks. Finally, to further elucidate the pathophysiology of OCD, the “state-trait” issue needs to be addressed by using pre-post treatment designs.
Correspondence: Odile A. van den Heuvel, MD, Department of Psychiatry, VU University Medical Center, PO Box 7057, 1007 MB Amsterdam, the Netherlands (oa.vandenheuvel@vumc.nl).
Submitted for Publication: December 22, 2003; final revision received July 22, 2004; accepted September 9, 2004.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant MW 940-37-018 from the Dutch Organization for Scientific Research (Nederlandse organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek), Den Haag.
Acknowledgment: We thank Patricia van Oppen, PhD, for her contribution to patient selection and inclusion.
1.Pauls
DLTowbin
KELeckman
JFZahner
GEPCohen
DJ Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence supporting a genetic relationship.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986;431180- 1182
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Cummings
JL Obsessive-compulsive behavior in basal ganglia disorders.
J Clin Psychiatry 1996;57495- 498
Google Scholar 3.Purcell
RMaruff
PKyrios
MPantelis
C Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder on tests of frontal-striatal function.
Biol Psychiatry 1998;43348- 357
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Greisberg
SMcKay
D Neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review and treatment implications.
Clin Psychol Rev 2003;2395- 117
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Dehaene
SChangeux
JP A hierarchical neuronal network for planning behavior.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;9413293- 13298
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Owen
AMDoyon
JPetrides
MEvans
AC Planning and spatial working memory: a positron emission tomography study in humans.
Eur J Neurosci 1996;8353- 364
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Baker
SCRogers
RDOwen
AMFrith
CDDolan
RJFrackowiak
RSRobbins
TW Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task.
Neuropsychologia 1996;34515- 526
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Dagher
AOwen
AMBoecker
HBrooks
DJ Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task.
Brain 1999;1221973- 1987
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Schall
UJohnston
PLagopoulos
JJuptner
MJentzen
WThienel
RDittmann-Balcar
ABender
SWard
PB Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Neuroimage 2003;201154- 1161
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Morris
RGAhmed
SSyed
GMToone
BK Neural correlates of planning ability: frontal lobe activation during the Tower of London test.
Neuropsychologia 1993;311367- 1378
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Rezai
KAndreasen
NCAlliger
RCohen
GSwayze
VO'Leary
DS The neuropsychology of the prefrontal cortex.
Arch Neurol 1993;50636- 642
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Lazeron
RHRombouts
SAMachielsen
WCScheltens
PWitter
MPUylings
HBBarkhof
F Visualizing brain activation during planning: the Tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2000;211407- 1414
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Fincham
JMCarter
CSvan Veen
VStenger
VAAnderson
JR Neural mechanisms of planning: a computational analysis using event-related fMRI.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002;993346- 3351
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.van den Heuvel
OAGroenewegen
HJBarkhof
FLazeron
RHCvan Dyck
RVeltman
DJ Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: a parametric functional magnetic resonance version of the Tower of London task.
Neuroimage 2003;18367- 374
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Newman
SDCarpenter
PAVarma
SJust
MA Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception.
Neuropsychologia 2003;411668- 1682
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Cazalis
FValabreque
RPelegrini-Issac
MAsloun
SRobbins
TWGranon
S Individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation on the Tower of London planning task: implication for effortful processing.
Eur J Neurosci 2003;172219- 2225
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Beauchamp
MHDagher
AAston
JADDoyon
J Dynamic functional changes associated with cognitive skill learning of an adapted version of the Tower of London task.
Neuroimage 2003;201649- 1660
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Keith Berg
WByrd
D The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research implementation.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24586- 604
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Veale
DMSahakian
BJOwen
AMMarks
IM Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Psychol Med 1996;261261- 1269
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Schmidtke
KSchorb
AWinkelmann
GHohagen
F Cognitive frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1998;43666- 673
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Purcell
RMaruff
PKyrios
MPantelis
C Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55415- 423
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Mataix-Cols
DJunque
CSanchez-Turet
MVallejo
JVerger
KBarrios
M Neuropsychological functioning in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;45898- 904
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Ramsey
NFKoning
HAMWelles
PCahn
Wvan der Linden
JAKahn
RS Excessive recruitment of neural systems subserving logical reasoning in schizophrenia.
Brain 2002;1251793- 1807
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Elliott
RBaker
SCRogers
RDO'Leary
DAPaykel
ESFrith
CDDolan
RJSahakian
BJ Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning task: a study using positron emission tomography.
Psychol Med 1997;27931- 942
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Rauch
SLSavage
CRAlpert
NMDougherty
DKendrick
ADCurran
TBrown
HDManzo
PFischman
AJJenike
MA Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997;9568- 573
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Deckersbach
TSavage
CRCurran
TBohne
AWilhelm
SBaer
LJenike
MARauch
SL A study of parallel implicit and explicit information processing in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;1591780- 1782
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Patient Edition (SCID-1/P, Version 2.0). New York, NY Biometrics Research Department1996;
31.Goodman
WKPrice
LHRasmussen
SAMazure
CFleischmann
RLHill
CLHeninger
GRCharney
DS The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;461006- 1011
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Veltman
DJRombouts
SARBDolan
RJ Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage 2003;18247- 256
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Rauch
SLWhalen
PJCurran
TShin
LMCoffey
BJSavage
CRMcInerney
SCBaer
LJenike
MA Probing striato-thalamic function in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Tourette syndrome using neuroimaging methods. Cohen
DJGoetz
CGJankovic
Jeds
Tourette Syndrome Philadelphia, Pa Lippincott Williams & Wilkins2001;207- 224
Google Scholar 36.McGuire
PKBench
CJFrith
CDMarks
IMFrackowiak
RSDolan
RJ Functional anatomy of obsessive-compulsive phenomena.
Br J Psychiatry 1994;164459- 468
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Rauch
SLJenike
MAAlpert
NMBaer
LBreiter
HCSavage
CRFischman
AJ Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15–labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;5162- 70
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Breiter
HCRauch
SLKwong
KKBaker
JRWeisskoff
RMKennedy
DNKendrick
ADDavis
TLJiang
ACohen
MSStern
CEBelliveau
JWBaer
LO'Sullivan
RLSavage
CRJenike
MARosen
BR Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53595- 606
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Cottraux
JGerard
DCinotti
LFroment
JCDeiber
MPLe Bars
DGaly
GMillet
PLabbe
CLavenne
FBouvard
MMauguiere
F A controlled positron emission tomography study of obsessive and neutral auditory stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder with checking rituals.
Psychiatry Res 1996;60101- 112
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Adler
CMMcDonough-Ryan
PSax
KWHolland
SKArndt
SStrakowski
SM fMRI of neuronal activation with symptom provocation in unmedicated patients with obsessive compulsive disorder.
J Psychiatr Res 2000;34317- 324
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Baxter
LR
JrPhelps
MEMazziotta
JCGuze
BHSchwartz
JMSelin
CE Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with rates in unipolar depression and in normal controls [published correction appears in
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:800].
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44211- 218
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Nordahl
TEBenkelfat
CSemple
WEGross
MKing
ACCohen
RM Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in non-depressed patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology 1989;223- 28
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Swedo
SESchapiro
MBGrady
CLCheslow
DLLeonard
HLKumar
AFriedland
RRapoport
SIRapoport
JL Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46518- 523
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Baxter
LR
JrSchwartz
JMBergman
KSSzuba
MPGuze
BHMazziotta
JCAlazraki
ASelin
CEFerng
HKMunford
PPhelps
ME Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49681- 689
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Baxter
LR
Jr Positron emission tomography studies of cerebral glucose metabolism in obsessive compulsive disorder.
J Clin Psychiatry 1994;55
((suppl))
54- 59
PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.Alexander
GEDeLong
MRStrick
PL Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex.
Annu Rev Neurosci 1986;9357- 381
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Saxena
SBrody
ALSchwartz
JMBaxter
LR Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1998;3526- 37
PubMedGoogle Scholar 48.van der Wee
NJRamsey
NFJansma
JMDenys
DAvan Megen
HJWestenberg
HMKahn
RS Spatial working memory deficits in obsessive compulsive disorder are associated with excessive engagement of the medial frontal cortex.
Neuroimage 2003;202271- 2280
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Petrides
M Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1996;3511455- 1462
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Levy
RGoldman-Rakic
PS Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Exp Brain Res 2000;13323- 32
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Manoach
DSWhite
NSLindgren
KAHeckers
SColeman
MJDubal
SHolzman
PS Hemispheric specialization of the lateral prefrontal cortex for strategic processing during spatial and shape working memory.
Neuroimage 2004;21894- 903
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Burgess
PWVeitch
Ede Lacy
CAShallice
T The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking.
Neuropsychologia 2000;38848- 863
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Bunge
SAKahn
IWallis
JDMiller
EKWagner
AD Neural circuits subserving the retrieval and maintenance of abstract rules.
J Neurophysiol 2003;903419- 3428
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Menon
VMackenzie
KRivera
SMReiss
AL Prefrontal cortex involvement in processing incorrect arithmetic equations: evidence from event-related fMRI.
Hum Brain Mapp 2002;16119- 130
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Shidara
MRichmond
BJ Anterior cingulate: single neuronal signals related to degree of reward expectancy.
Science 2002;2961709- 1711
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Ursu
SStenger
VAShear
MKJones
MRCarter
CS Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Psychol Sci 2003;14347- 353
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Garavan
HRoss
TJMurphy
KRoche
RAPStein
EA Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: inhibition, error detection and correction.
Neuroimage 2002;171820- 1829
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Ullsperger
Mvon Cramon
DY Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs.
Neuroimage 2001;141387- 1401
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Pierrot-Deseilligny
CMuri
RMRivaud-Pechoux
SGaymard
BPloner
CJ Cortical control of spatial memory in humans: the visuooculomotor model.
Ann Neurol 2002;5210- 19
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Lavenex
PSuzuki
WAAmaral
DG Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: projections to the neocortex.
J Comp Neurol 2002;447394- 420
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Faw
B Pre-frontal executive committee for perception, working memory, attention, long-term memory, motor control, and thinking: a tutorial review.
Conscious Cogn 2003;1283- 139
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Reese
NBGarcia-Rill
ESkinner
RD The pedunculopontine nucleus: auditory input, arousal and pathophysiology.
Prog Neurobiol 1995;47105- 133
Google ScholarCrossref 66.Aston-Jones
GRajkowski
JKubiak
PValentino
RJShipley
MT Role of the locus coeruleus in emotional activation. Holstege
GBandler
RSaper
CBeds The Emotional Motor System Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science BV1996;379- 402
67.Usher
MCohen
JDServan-Schreiber
DRajkowski
JAston-Jones
G The role of locus coeruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance.
Science 1999;283549- 554
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Aston-Jones
GRajkowski
JCohen
J Role of locus coeruleus in attention and behavioral flexibility.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;461309- 1320
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Miyazato
HSkinner
RDGarcia-Rill
E Locus coeruleus involvement in the effects of immobilization stress on the p13 midlatency auditory evoked potential in the rat.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2000;241177- 1201
Google ScholarCrossref 71.Robbins
TWJames
MOwen
AMSahakian
BJLawrence
ADMcInnes
LRabbitt
PM A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging: Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1998;4474- 490
Google Scholar 72.Mataix-Cols
DCullen
SLange
KZelaya
FAndrew
CAmaro
EBrammer
MJWilliams
SCSpeckens
APhillips
ML Neural correlates of anxiety associated with obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions in normal volunteers.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53482- 493
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Mataix-Cols
DWooderson
SLawrence
NBrammer
MJSpeckens
APhillips
ML Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61564- 576
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Austin
MPMitchell
PGoodwin
GM Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology.
Br J Psychiatry 2001;178200- 206
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Swaab
DFFliers
EHoogendijk
WJVeltman
DJZhou
JN Interaction of prefrontal cortical and hypothalamic systems in pathogenesis of depression.
Prog Brain Res 2000;126369- 396
PubMedGoogle Scholar 76.Saxena
SBrody
ALHo
MLAlborzian
SHo
MKMaidment
KMHuang
SCWu
HMAu
SCBaxter
LR
Jr Cerebral metabolism in major depression and obsessive-compulsive disorder occurring separately and concurrently.
Biol Psychiatry 2001;50159- 170
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref