Background
Functional neuroimaging may elucidate the pathophysiologic features of anxiety disorders and the site of action of anxiolytic drugs. A large body of evidence suggests that the amygdala and associated limbic structures play a critical role in the expression of anxiety and may be treatment targets for anxiolytic drugs.
Objective
To determine whether lorazepam dose-dependently attenuates blood oxygenation level–dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD fMRI) activation in the amygdala and associated limbic structures during an emotion face assessment task.
Participants and Design
Fifteen healthy volunteers participated in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized dose-response study. Subjects underwent imaging 3 times (at least a week apart) and were given either a single-dose placebo or 0.25 mg or 1.0 mg of lorazepam 1 hour prior to an MRI session. During fMRI, subjects completed an emotion face assessment task, which has been shown to elicit amygdala activation.
Main Outcome Measures
The BOLD-fMRI activation in amygdala, insula, and medial prefrontal cortex during the emotion face assessment task.
Results
Lorazepam significantly attenuated the BOLD-fMRI signal in a dose-dependent manner in bilateral amygdala and insula but not in the medial prefrontal cortex. Lorazepam did not affect the BOLD-fMRI signal in the primary visual cortex.
Conclusions
The current finding provides the first neuroimaging evidence of a dose-dependent change induced by an established therapeutic agent in brain regions known to be critical for the mediation of anxiety. This investigation may help to support the use of BOLD-fMRI with pharmacological probes to investigate the neural circuits underlying anxiety and the use of fMRI as a tool in the development of new anxiolytic agents.
Anxiety disorders are among the most common psychiatric disorders but comprise a heterogeneous group of conditions. Delineating the basic neurocircuitry underlying normal and pathologic anxiety1 may help to better define anxiety disorders. The amygdala plays a critical role in normal fear conditioning and is implicated in the pathophysiologic mechanism of anxiety disorders.2,3 However, this structure is also important for other emotional information processing and behavior.4 Functional neuroimaging studies have shown amygdala activation in fear conditioning,5 reward-related processing,6 encoding of emotionally salient information,7 risk taking,8 processing positively valenced stimuli,9 and appetitive or aversive olfactory learning.10
In addition to the amygdala, a network of structures that includes the insula, anterior cingulate gyrus, and medial prefrontal cortex is important to identify the emotional significance of a stimulus, generate an affective response, and regulate the affective state.11 The insula has afferent and efferent connections to the medial and orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, and several nuclei of the amygdala.12 Although insula activation has frequently been associated with disgust,13 there is increasing evidence of a broader role for this brain structure in emotion processing.14 Insula activation is thought to be involved in differential positive vs negative emotion processing,5 in particular fearful face processing,15 pain perception,16,17 and when individuals are asked to make judgments about emotions.18
Benzodiazepine derivatives are the most commonly prescribed antianxiety agents in clinical practice. Lorazepam, a 3-hydroxy-1,4-benzodiazepine, is rapidly and readily absorbed, reaching peak concentrations in the blood proportional to the dose approximately 1 to 2 hours after oral administration.19 Previous neuroimaging investigation using 1 mg of intravenous lorazepam during a memory task found significant decreases in activation within the hippocampal, fusiform, and inferior prefrontal regions but no significant alterations in activation in the striate cortex.20
The aim of this study was to use blood oxygenation level–dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD fMRI) to test the hypothesis that lorazepam dose-dependently attenuates activation during an emotion face paradigm in the amygdala, insula, and medial prefrontal cortex. A modified emotion face assessment task was used to determine whether this attenuation would occur with negative emotion faces (angry and fear) but not with positive emotion faces (happy). Support for this hypothesis would provide a link between the clinical efficacy of lorazepam as an anxiolytic and the biological basis of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic modulation of the amygdala and limbic structures as key targets for anxiety circuitry.
Pharmacological fMRI is an emerging discipline with the potential to address a variety of neural systems and drug development questions.21 One important consideration with pharmacological fMRI is the differentiation between the drug’s effect on neural tissue or on hemodynamic modulation. Some investigators have argued that pharmacological stimulation results in heterogeneous fMRI effects as opposed to global, homogeneous changes in BOLD signal. This argument is consistent with an action on specific neuronal receptor–based mechanisms.22 Moreover, focusing on a priori hypothesized areas may prevent false-positive findings.23 Thus, the current study used primarily a region-of-interest approach.
The University of California, San Diego, institutional review board approved the study procedures. All participants provided written informed consent and were paid for their participation. We studied 15 healthy, nonsmoking individuals (6 women and 9 men) aged 18 to 39 years (mean ± SD, 27.6 ± 1.4 years) with 12 to 18 years of education (mean ± SD, 15.6 ± 0.3 years). Participants did not have medical or psychiatric disorders as determined by medical history and diagnoses according to the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Fourth Edition. Each subject was required to consume less than 150 mg/d of caffeine and less than 14 caffeinated beverages per week. Subjects had no history of drug or alcohol abuse and did not report a prior use of benzodiazepines. All participants passed a urine drug screen. Subjects were instructed to maintain their regular bedtimes and wake times for 1 week before and throughout the study period. Individuals presented to the MRI facility between 8 AM and 4 PM with at least 1 week between studies.
The study was performed in a double-blind manner. Participants underwent each of 3 conditions in randomized order between 1 and 3 weeks apart. Lorazepam was chosen as the anxiolytic because of its well-documented behavioral effects and because it has a short metabolic half-life, which peaks at about 60 to 120 minutes after ingestion,24 and no active metabolite, which eliminates the possible confounding effects of drug accumulation.25 We selected doses that minimally disrupt performance of behavioral tasks and memory (≤1.0 mg) but are used to treat anxiety. Subjects arrived at the MRI facility 60 to 90 minutes prior to the MRI study. At arrival, subjects’ vital signs were assessed and subjects orally received either placebo or 0.25 mg or 1.0 mg of lorazepam suspension mixed in diet decaffeinated cola.
ASSESSMENTS AND fMRI PARADIGM
Subjects completed the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory26 (STAI) and the visual analog scales for anxiety, tension, alertness, and trembling before and after the MRI study. During fMRI, each subject was tested using a slightly modified version of the emotion face assessment task.27 During each 5-second trial, a subject was presented with a target face (on the top of the computer screen) and 2 probe faces (on the bottom of the screen) and was instructed to match the probe with the same emotional expression to the target by pressing the left or right key on a button box (Figure 1). A block consisted of 6 consecutive trials in which the target face was either angry, fearful, or happy.28 During the sensorimotor control task, subjects were presented with 5-second trials of ovals or circles in an analogous configuration and were instructed to match the shape of the probe to the target. Each block of faces and of the sensorimotor control task was presented 3 times in a pseudorandomized order. A fixation cross was interspersed between each block. For each trial, response accuracy and reaction time data were obtained.
During the task, one BOLD-fMRI run was collected for each subject using a 1.5-T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) (T2-weighted echoplanar imaging, time to repeat = 2000 milliseconds, echo time = 40 milliseconds, 64 × 64 matrix, twenty 4-mm axial slices, 256 repetitions). During the same experimental session, a T1-weighted image (magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo, time to repeat = 11.4 milliseconds, echo time = 4.4 milliseconds, flip angle = 10°, field of view = 256 × 256, 1-mm3 voxels) was obtained for anatomical reference. For preprocessing, voxel time series were interpolated to correct for nonsimultaneous slice acquisition within each volume and were corrected for 3-dimensional motion.
All structural and functional image processing were done with the Analysis of Functional Neuroimages software package.29 Echoplanar images were coregistered to the 128th image using a 3-dimensional coregistration algorithm. The time series of motion parameters was used to obtain a mean for these 6 parameters for each subject. Three motion parameters (d-roll, d-pitch, and d-yaw, indicating change in roll, pitch, and yaw directions) were used as nuisance regressors to account for motion artifacts. The 4 orthogonal regressors of interest were (1) happy, (2) angry, (3) fearful, and (4) circle/oval sensorimotor condition. These regressors were convolved with a modified gamma variate function to account for the delay and the dispersion brain response of the BOLD-fMRI signal due to hemodynamics.30 Additional regressors were used to model residual motion in the roll, pitch, and yaw directions as well as baseline and linear trends. The Analysis of Functional Neuroimages program 3dDeconvolve29 was used to calculate the estimated voxelwise response amplitude. A gaussian filter with 6 mm full width at half maximum was applied to the voxelwise percent signal change data to account for individual variations of the anatomical landmarks.
Data for each subject were normalized to Talairach coordinates. A priori regions of interest (defined by the Talairach Daemon atlas31) in the bilateral amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, primary visual cortex, and insula were used as masks. Based on these areas of interest, it was determined via simulations that a voxelwise a priori probability of .05 would result in a corrected clusterwise activation probability of .05 if a minimum volume of 128 μL and 2 connected voxels (in the amygdala) or 512 μL and 8 connected voxels (in all other regions of interest) was considered. The areas of interest were superimposed on each individual’s voxelwise percent signal change brain image. Only activations within the areas of interest that also satisfied the volume and voxel connection criteria were extracted and used for further analysis. No clusters of voxels survived the cluster threshold parameters in the medial prefrontal cortex. Thus, planned comparisons using contrasts for repeated-measures designs wereperformed for only bilateral amygdala and insula when significant omnibus F values were found. The voxelwise percent signal change data were entered into a mixed-model analysis of variance with task contrast (face type vs circle/oval comparison condition) and dose (placebo or 0.25 or 1.0 mg of lorazepam) as fixed factors and subjects as a random factor. In addition, a whole-brain analysis was carried out to determine whether lorazepam affected nonhypothesized areas. Activations clusters were extracted and corrected for multiple comparisons using an a priori P < .05 threshold and a volume mask of greater than 1000 μL.
All behavioral analyses were carried out with SPSS version 10.0.32 A repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance, with dose (placebo or 0.25 or 1.0 mg of lorazepam) as the within-subjects factor, was used to analyze the behavioral measures and neural activation patterns. Behavioral measures are reported as an interaction between dose and task type. Self-rating measures are reported as interactions between the drug and ratings before and after the MRI study. Correlational analyses were conducted for placebo administration by examining the relationship between self-rating scales or performance measures and activation in the bilateral insula and amygdala during viewing of angry, fearful, and happy faces.
Subjects matched the probe face to the target face with nearly perfect accuracy (mean ± SD, 97.0% ± 0.7%), which was not affected by either the 0.25-mg or 1.0-mg dose of lorazepam (F2,28 = 0.40, P = .60, η2 = 0.03). As shown in Figure 2, although latency was affected by face type (ie, longer for matching angry or fearful faces when compared with happy faces and circles or squares [F3,12 = 48.8, P<.001, η2 = 0.92]), neither dose of lorazepam affected response latency (F2,28 = 1.47, P = .25, η2 = 0.09).
As shown in Figure 3, lorazepam did not affect the level of anxiety, either measured by visual analog scales (F2,22 = 1.71, P = .19, η2 = 0.14) or by the STAI (F2,28 = 0.31, P = .73, η2 = 0.02). Moreover, visual analog ratings of tension (F2,22 = 0.57, P = .57, η2 = 0.05) or trembling (F2,22 = 1.82, P = .18, η2 = 0.14) did not significantly change across drug administration. However, individuals reported increased sleepiness after 1.0 mg of lorazepam, as evidenced by a dose by pre-post interaction (F2,22 = 7.2, P = .004, η2 = 0.40) (Figure 2).
Functional neuroimaging results
Irrespective of face type, subjects showed bilateral activation of the amygdala and anterior insula during the emotion face assessment task relative to the sensorimotor control task (see Figure 4 and the Table for statistics). Lorazepam attenuated the activation in a dose-dependent fashion in the amygdala and insula (Table, Figure 5A). Specifically, activation in bilateral amygdala after administration of 1.0 mg of lorazepam was significantly lower than after 0.25 mg (t14 = 4.83, P<.001) and significantly lower than after placebo administration (t14 = 2.85, P = .01). There was no significant difference between placebo and 0.25 mg of lorazepam (t14 = 0.62, P = .54). Similarly, activation in the bilateral insula after 1.0 mg of lorazepam was significantly lower than after 0.25 mg (t14 = 3.41, P = .004) and significantly lower than after placebo administration (t14 = 3.63, P = .003). There was no significant difference between placebo and 0.25 mg of lorazepam (t14 = 0.70, P = .50). There was no significant effect of lorazepam on activation in the bilateral visual cortex for placebo or the 2 doses of lorazepam (Table, Figure 5B). A whole-brain analysis revealed significant effects of lorazepam in the right inferior temporal gyrus (coordinates: 43,−63,0), left insula (−27,7,−6), amygdala (−9,−11,−10), and fusiform gyrus (−29,−93,−13). In all areas, there was a significant attenuation of the signal in the 1.0-mg lorazepam condition relative to both placebo and 0.25 mg of lorazepam.
Correlations between behavioral and functional neuroimaging results
There were no significant correlations between visual analog scales before administration or after imaging with the degree of activation in the bilateral insula or amygdala during task performance. Moreover, behavioral measures such as latency of response or accuracy of response during the emotion face assessment task did not correlate with the degree of activation in the bilateral insula or amygdala.
The results of this investigation show for the first time that a known anxiolytic, the benzodiazepine lorazepam, dose-dependently attenuates task-induced activation in bilateral amygdala and insula but has no effect on the visual cortex. These effects were observed at doses of lorazepam that did not significantly affect behavior on this task and did not change levels of anxiety in these healthy (nonanxious) volunteers. In animals, a large body of evidence suggests that benzodiazepine agonists attenuate brain activity in the amygdala consistent with their anxiolytic effects.33 In humans, however, evidence of benzodiazepine effects on reducing amygdala activity had been missing.
A major focus in the field of anxiety research is the delineation of the basic neurocircuitry underlying normal and pathologic anxiety.1 Amygdala activation occurs during fear conditioning, and altered amygdala activation has been implicated in the pathophysiologic mechanism of anxiety disorders.2,3 For example, individuals with social anxiety disorder34 or posttraumatic stress disorder35 show amygdala hyperresponsivity to fearful or angry faces. Moreover, patients with panic disorder have decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in the left hippocampus and precuneus36 as well as in the right orbitofrontal cortex and right insula.37 The insular cortex appears to be important for subjective feeling states and interoceptive awareness.38,39 To our knowledge, this is the first study to show that an anxiolytic drug can dose-dependently reduce activation in the amygdala and insula, areas that are important for emotional processing in general and anxiety in particular. Thus, pharmacological fMRI with specific behavioral probes may prove especially useful for delineating how these brain structures are altered in anxiety disorders.
Lorazepam also attenuated activation in the fusiform gyrus. Although primarily recognized as a face-processing area,40 this structure’s activation patterns are related to the degree of conscious perception,41 anticipation,42 and identification of emotionally important visual clues.43 Moreover, activation in the fusiform gyrus is altered in individuals with anxiety disorders,44 and increased activation in the fusiform gyrus to fearful faces has been observed after cholinergic enhancement.45 The amygdala can modulate early visual processing of emotional faces in the extrastriate cortex.15 Thus, lorazepam-induced attenuation of fusiform activity may be indirectly elicited by its down-regulation of the amygdala.
The behavioral effects of benzodiazepines in healthy, nonanxious volunteers have been mixed; for example, diazepam had no effect on fear-potentiated startle response,46 whereas it impaired the recognition of anger and fear but not other emotional expressions.47 Single administration vs repeated administration of lorazepam can induce loss of appetite, dizziness, and physical and mentaltiredness.48,49 Benzodiazepines dose-dependently disrupt learning and performance on acute vigilance tasks50 and on a variety of memory tasks, presumably by reducing the normally facilitative effect of emotion on memory.51,52 Thus, behavioral effects may not provide robust indicators of potential anxiolytic efficacy in healthy nonanxious subjects, whereas changes in neural activation patterns induced by these substances may be informative. A low level of state anxiety may account for the lack of correlation between attenuation of the amygdala by lorazepam and changes in anxiety levels in our study. Future studies may include individuals with high trait anxiety (and ultimately anxiety disorders) to elucidate the relationship between anxiolytic effect and amygdala attenuation.
New drugs with novel anxiolytic mechanisms have been developed, including GABA A subreceptors,53 metabotropic glutamate receptor agents,54 and neuropeptide modulators.55 The current results provide a strong rationale for using BOLD fMRI imaging and amygdala- and/or insula-sensitive behavioral paradigms to determine whether these substances exert effects similar to those seen with standard anxiolytics. Furthermore, evaluation of the effects of other known classes of anxiolytics, such as long-term administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, should be undertaken to further validate this paradigm. The effect sizes (η2) observed in this study provide proof in principle that small-scale (ie, sample sizes in the range of 15 to 30 subjects) BOLD fMRI studies can provide reasonably robust data to determine the site of action of putative anxiolytics. In summary, the current finding provides the first evidence of a dose-dependent change induced by an established therapeutic agent in brain regions known to be critical for the mediation of anxiety.
Correspondence: Martin P. Paulus, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 8950 La Jolla Village Dr, Suite C213, La Jolla, CA 92037-0985 (mpaulus@ucsd.edu).
Submitted for Publication: March 29, 2004; final revision received July 27, 2004; accepted August 20, 2004.
Funding/Support: This work was supported by grants DA13186 and MH65413 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.
Acknowledgment: We acknowledge the invaluable help of Kelly Winternheimer, BS, and Thuy Le, BS.
2.Rauch
SLShin
LMWright
CI Neuroimaging studies of amygdala function in anxiety disorders.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;985389- 410
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Charney
DS Neuroanatomical circuits modulating fear and anxiety behaviors.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 2003;38- 50
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Buchel
CMorris
JDolan
RJFriston
KJ Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study.
Neuron 1998;20947- 957
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Breiter
HCRosen
BR Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain reward circuitry in the human.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 1999;877523- 547
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Canli
TZhao
ZBrewer
JGabrieli
JDCahill
L Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience.
J Neurosci 2000;20RC99
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Ernst
MBolla
KMouratidis
MContoreggi
CMatochik
JAKurian
VCadet
JLKimes
ASLondon
ED Decision-making in a risk-taking task: a PET study.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;26682- 691
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Garavan
HPendergrass
JCRoss
TJStein
EARisinger
RC Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli.
Neuroreport 2001;122779- 2783
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Gottfried
JAO'Doherty
JDolan
RJ Appetitive and aversive olfactory learning in humans studied using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging.
J Neurosci 2002;2210829- 10837
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Phillips
MLDrevets
WCRauch
SLLane
R Neurobiology of emotion perception, I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;54504- 514
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Augustine
JR Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1996;22229- 244
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Phillips
MLYoung
AWScott
SKCalder
AJAndrew
CGiampietro
VWilliams
SCBullmore
ETBrammer
MGray
JA Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust.
Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1998;2651809- 1817
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Phan
KLWager
TTaylor
SFLiberzon
I Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI.
Neuroimage 2002;16331- 348
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Morris
JSFriston
KJBèuchel
CFrith
CDYoung
AWCalder
AJDolan
RJ A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions.
Brain 1998;12147- 57
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Gelnar
PAKrauss
BRSheehe
PRSzeverenyi
NMApkarian
AV A comparative fMRI study of cortical representations for thermal painful, vibrotactile, and motor performance tasks.
Neuroimage 1999;10460- 482
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Peyron
RLaurent
BGarcia-Larrea
L Functional imaging of brain responses to pain: a review and meta-analysis (2000).
Neurophysiol Clin 2000;30263- 288
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Gorno-Tempini
MLPradelli
SSerafini
MPagnoni
GBaraldi
PPorro
CNicoletti
RUmita
CNichelli
P Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage 2001;14465- 473
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Kyriakopoulos
AAGreenblatt
DJShader
RI Clinical pharmacokinetics of lorazepam: a review.
J Clin Psychiatry 1978;3916- 23
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Sperling
RGreve
DDale
AKilliany
RHolmes
JRosas
HDCocchiarella
AFirth
PRosen
BLake
SLange
NRoutledge
CAlbert
M Functional MRI detection of pharmacologically induced memory impairment.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002;99455- 460
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Salmeron
BJStein
EA Pharmacological applications of magnetic resonance imaging.
Psychopharmacol Bull 2002;36102- 129
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Stein
EAPankiewicz
JHarsch
HHCho
JKFuller
SAHoffmann
RGHawkins
MRao
SMBandettini
PABloom
AS Nicotine-induced limbic cortical activation in the human brain: a functional MRI study.
Am J Psychiatry 1998;1551009- 1015
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Busto
UEKaplan
HLWright
CEGomez-Mancilla
BZawertailo
LGreenblatt
DJSellers
EM A comparative pharmacokinetic and dynamic evaluation of alprazolam sustained-release, bromazepam, and lorazepam.
J Clin Psychopharmacol 2000;20628- 635
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Rush
CRHiggins
STBickel
WKHughes
JR Acute effects of triazolam and lorazepam on human learning, performance and subject ratings.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1993;2641218- 1226
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Spielberger
CD Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Palo Alto, Calif Consulting Psychologists Press1983;
27.Hariri
ARMattay
VSTessitore
AKolachana
BFera
FGoldman
DEgan
MFWeinberger
DR Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.
Science 2002;297400- 403
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Cox
RW AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.
Comput Biomed Res 1996;29162- 173
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Boynton
GMEngel
SAGlover
GHHeeger
DJ Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1.
J Neurosci 1996;164207- 4221
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Lancaster
JLWoldorff
MGParsons
LMLiotti
MFreitas
CSRainey
LKochunov
PVNickerson
DMikiten
SAFox
PT Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping.
Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10120- 131
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Norusis
MJ SPSS Base System User's Guide. Chicago, Ill SPSS Inc1990;
33.Menard
JTreit
D Effects of centrally administered anxiolytic compounds in animal models of anxiety.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1999;23591- 613
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Stein
MBGoldin
PRSareen
JZorrilla
LTBrown
GG Increased amygdala activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;591027- 1034
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Rauch
SLWhalen
PJShin
LMMcInerney
SCMacklin
MLLasko
NBOrr
SPPitman
RK Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;47769- 776
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Bremner
JDInnis
RBWhite
TFujita
MSilbersweig
DGoddard
AWStaib
LStern
ECappiello
AWoods
SBaldwin
RCharney
DS SPECT [I-123]iomazenil measurement of the benzodiazepine receptor in panic disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;4796- 106
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Malizia
ALCunningham
VJBell
CJLiddle
PFJones
TNutt
DJ Decreased brain GABA(A)-benzodiazepine receptor binding in panic disorder: preliminary results from a quantitative PET study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55715- 720
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Craig
AD How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3655- 666
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Critchley
HDWiens
SRotshtein
POhman
ADolan
RJ Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness.
Nat Neurosci 2004;7189- 195
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Kanwisher
NMcDermott
JChun
MM The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception.
J Neurosci 1997;174302- 4311
PubMedGoogle Scholar 41.Andrews
TJSchluppeck
DHomfray
DMatthews
PBlakemore
C Activity in the fusiform gyrus predicts conscious perception of Rubin's vase-face illusion.
Neuroimage 2002;17890- 901
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Geday
JGjedde
ABoldsen
ASKupers
R Emotional valence modulates activity in the posterior fusiform gyrus and inferior medial prefrontal cortex in social perception.
Neuroimage 2003;18675- 684
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Bremner
JDVythilingam
MVermetten
ESouthwick
SMMcGlashan
TStaib
LHSoufer
RCharney
DS Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53879- 889
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Bentley
PVuilleumier
PThiel
CMDriver
JDolan
RJ Cholinergic enhancement modulates neural correlates of selective attention and emotional processing.
Neuroimage 2003;2058- 70
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Baas
JMGrillon
CBocker
KBBrack
AAMorgan
CA
IIIKenemans
JLVerbaten
MN Benzodiazepines have no effect on fear-potentiated startle in humans.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161233- 247
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Zangara
ABlair
RJCurran
HV A comparison of the effects of a beta-adrenergic blocker and a benzodiazepine upon the recognition of human facial expressions.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;16336- 41
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.File
SEBond
AJ Impaired performance and sedation after a single dose of lorazepam.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1979;66309- 313
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.File
SEBond
AJLister
RG Interaction between effects of caffeine and lorazepam in performance tests and self-ratings.
J Clin Psychopharmacol 1982;2102- 106
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Greenblatt
DJScavone
JMHarmatz
JSEngelhardt
NShader
RI Cognitive effects of beta-adrenergic antagonists after single doses: pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of propranolol, atenolol, lorazepam, and placebo.
Clin Pharmacol Ther 1993;53577- 584
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Buchanan
TWKarafin
MSAdolphs
R Selective effects of triazolam on memory for emotional, relative to neutral, stimuli: differential effects on gist versus detail.
Behav Neurosci 2003;117517- 525
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Blair
RJCurran
HV Selective impairment in the recognition of anger induced by diazepam.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;147335- 338
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Whiting
PJ The GABAA receptor gene family: new opportunities for drug development.
Curr Opin Drug Discov Devel 2003;6648- 657
PubMedGoogle Scholar 54.Schoepp
DDWright
RALevine
LRGaydos
BPotter
WZ LY354740, an mGlu2/3 receptor agonist as a novel approach to treat anxiety/stress.
Stress 2003;6189- 197
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref