Context
Episodic memory impairments are well characterized in schizophrenia, but their neural origin is unclear.
Objective
To determine whether the episodic memory impairments in schizophrenia may originate from reduced parahippocampal connectivity.
Design
Experimental in silico model.
Setting
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
Interventions
A new, in silico medial temporal lobe model that simulates normal performance on a variety of episodic memory tasks was devised. The effects of reducing parahippocampal connectivity in the model (from perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex to entorhinal cortex and from entorhinal cortex to hippocampus) were evaluated and compared with findings in schizophrenic patients. Alternative in silico neuropathologies, increased noise and loss of hippocampal neurons, were also evaluated.
Results
In the model, parahippocampal processing subserves integration of different cortical inputs to the hippocampus and feature extraction during recall. Reduced connectivity in this area resulted in a pattern of deficits that closely mimicked the impairments in schizophrenia, including a mild recognition impairment and a more severe impairment in free recall. Furthermore, the schizophrenic model was not differentially sensitive to interference, also consistent with behavioral data. Notably, neither increased noise levels nor a reduction of hippocampal nodes in the model reproduced this characteristic memory profile.
Conclusions
Taken together, these findings highlight the importance of parahippocampal neuropathology in schizophrenia, demonstrating that reduced connectivity in this region may underlie episodic memory problems associated with the disorder.
Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is broadly based, with varying degrees of deficit in different ability domains.1-4 Particularly pronounced impairments are observed in episodic memory,5,6 including moderate to severe deficits in free recall, lesser ones in cued recall, and a small but significant deficit in recognition.5,7 These impairments do not appear to be related to an increased forgetting rate.5-9 They are relatively unresponsive to medication,5,10-12 age, and severity and duration of illness,5 and can be identified in approximately 75% of patients.13
Until recently, the neural origin of the aforementioned deficits was unclear. However, recent in vivo imaging studies have related episodic memory performance in schizophrenic patients to volume14,15 and activity measures in the (para)hippocampal region.16,17 The pattern of neuropsychological impairment has also provided some indication of the brain regions involved. For instance, a frontal origin is improbable because the memory deficits are not correlated with attentive or executive aspects of memory processing.4,5,18 Finally, the absence of amnesic signs argues against gross damage to the hippocampus proper (dentate and Ammon’s horn).
The present study investigates whether medial temporal lobe (MTL) regions other than the hippocampus proper may underlie the observed deficits in schizophrenia, in particular, the parahippocampal region, including the entorhinal, perirhinal, and parahippocampal cortices. This region is adjacent to the hippocampus proper and forms the main source of its “sensory” input. Interestingly, this region shows the largest volume deficit within the MTL and substantial loss in the density of some synaptic and dendritic molecules.19-21 Thus, it may be hypothesized that afferent, rather than intrinsic, hippocampal connectivity is preferentially affected in schizophrenia. According to current views of hippocampal functioning, this would leave the recurrent hippocampal pathways, subserving autoassociative storage and pattern completion, intact.22 While our emphasis is on connectivity as a final common pathway for dysfunction, we are agnostic as to etiology. Reductions of connectivity may result from a variety of factors: environmental insult, failures in expression of trophic factors having to do with neurite growth, abnormal migration and attendant cytoarchitectural disruption, or factors in compromised neurotransmission that result in reduced functional connectivity.
It is difficult to envisage how parahippocampal abnormalities might affect memory processing, as their contribution is poorly understood. Therefore, an MTL memory model was developed that accounts for the differential role of this region in episodic memory. The model simulates performance in a number of typical episodic memory paradigms assessing recognition, retrieval, and interference. The consequences of reduced connectivity between parahippocampal regions and from these regions to the hippocampus in the model were assessed and compared with findings in schizophrenic patients. To evaluate the specificity of our findings, we contrasted the effects of reduced connectivity to other possible (although, in our view, less likely) abnormalities, such as “noise” and hippocampal neuronal loss. While the various symptoms of schizophrenia are likely related to multiple abnormalities at the neuronal level, the present results assist in understanding how a major cognitive impairment may result from a specific class of pathological changes, ruling in and ruling out candidate mechanisms.
An apparent function of MTL is to bind memories and their instance-specific context and then store these for later retrieval. The model described herein is based on studies showing that the bulk of hippocampal cortical input is segregated over 2 pathways.23,24 One of these runs over the perirhinal area and targets the anterior and lateral entorhinal cortex; the other, via the parahippocampal cortex, projects mostly to medial posterior portions of entorhinal cortex. In primates, the perirhinal region conveys information regarding meaningful objects,25,26 while the parahippocampal cortex is involved in memory for spaces and spatial relations.27,28 These different aspects of an event may thus access the hippocampus over dual input streams.
The 2 streams are interconnected at various levels within the parahippocampal region23,24 (Figure 1). These interconnections likely contribute to the integration of cortical inputs into a representation of their co-occurrence. In support of this notion, recordings of entorhinal neurons during delayed-response tasks indicate that their activity carries information about both objects and spatial locations.29
The hippocampus proper may quickly associate a small code to the conjunction of cortical inputs,30-33 enhancing pattern separation in the system: similar entorhinal patterns are separated by their associated hippocampal patterns, which tend to resemble one another less than do the entorhinal patterns. Such de-correlation occurs through the combination of a dense projection from the entorhinal to the hippocampal layer, sparse firing in the hippocampal layer, and long-term depression in the connections from the entorhinal to the hippocampal layer.34
On the basis of the foregoing information, we implemented episodic memory storage as a process that entails 2 steps in binding: (1) First, item-context co-occurrence patterns are formed in the entorhinal cortex. We assume 1 such pattern is formed for each item-context combination. These relatively rich patterns are reciprocally connected with individual constituents (features) of the episode, which are themselves encoded in lower-order regions (perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices). The co-occurrence patterns show considerable overlap, because the context input may be similar for many item-context combinations (eg, in list-learning experiments). (2) In the second step, entorhinal patterns are reciprocally associated with small hippocampal codes with very little overlap. These hippocampal patterns are not directly associated with individual features, but serve to separate the large number of overlapping entorhinal patterns. The representational overlap in entorhinal cortex, combined with the pattern separation system in the hippocampus proper, enormously increases the storage capacity of the memory store with respect to any single layer system.32
We will show in following sections how this network organization sustains 3 interconnected functions that are crucial to episodic memory: (1) sampling, the process whereby a cue (part of previously stored information) is used to select a subset from multiple stored patterns in a memory store; (2) pattern completion, the activation of “missing” components of a representation through previously strengthened connections; and (3) feature extraction, whereby activated co-occurrence patterns lead to reinstatement of associated lower-order representations, which contain the feature information that the more abstract higher-order representations are lacking.
The model was built with the Walnut/Nutshell software developed by our group35 and consists of linear threshold nodes. It captures the basic organization of the (para)hippocampal regions in a simplified manner, using 4 modules. All model connections are fanning (ie, contacted nodes are randomly distributed over the target layer), which is consistent with neuroanatomic data regarding most (para)hippocampal connections. None of the modules has intrinsic connectivity. The reciprocal connections between the hippocampal and entorhinal layers have a high learning parameter reflecting high plasticity. Conversely, plasticity of the connections from the 2 input layers to the entorhinal module is relatively low, so synaptic weights changed negligibly on the time scale of the simulations.
In both higher-level modules, global inhibition was mimicked by k-winner-take-all dynamics, which limits activity in a layer to a predetermined number of nodes (k) receiving the largest input. In accordance with electrophysiologic data,29k is relatively large in the entorhinal layer and small in the hippocampal layer. These and other parameter settings were obtained through optimization procedures for free recall. Parameter settings for the normal model are shown in Figure 1.
Learning was implemented with an asymptotic, hebbian-type rule,36,37 in which the long-term expected value of weights is equal to the relative frequency of presynaptic and postsynaptic coactivation.37 We used this characteristic to initialize most weights in our network to the value they would have after many independent patterns are learned (standard deviations were estimated with Monte Carlo simulations). This simulated the background of a “full memory.” To also simulate earlier encounters of the subject with the items used in the list-learning experiments, we stored each item (including foils) in a random context, using a variable learning parameter. Retrieval was thus tested under competitive circumstances. These procedures were carried out for each permutation of the network, before running the actual simulations of memory paradigms. More methodologic detail is provided on the authors’ Web site (http://www.neuromod.org/data/archives).
Episodic memory processing
To simulate learning of an episode, a set of nodes is activated in the item layer and another set in the context layer. The patterns in the 2 input layers stimulate a set of nodes in the entorhinal layer. Out of this set, the nodes with the largest input become active, forming the entorhinal representation of the item-context co-occurrence. Some of the activated entorhinal nodes represent information from just one input layer, but most receive both types of input, and thus cross-associate the item and context patterns (Figure 2A). Similarly, the activated entorhinal nodes select a smaller group of hippocampal nodes. Through highly plastic, bidirectional connections between the entorhinal and hippocampal layers, these 2 representations are bound together, forming the episodic trace.
The memory system can be sampled by means of cues consisting of partial input patterns, for instance, part of a context representation from a previously experienced episode. Initially, such a cue may activate only part of an associated entorhinal pattern, but if the set of activated entorhinal nodes sufficiently resembles a stored representation, their combined firing will tend to activate associated hippocampal nodes, through the previously strengthened connections with these nodes. The hippocampal nodes, in turn, will recruit missing nodes of the entorhinal representation. Over a number of cycles, this pattern completion process will reinstate the original pattern in the entorhinal layer, which, in turn, can reinstate associated information in the input layers, namely, item representations that have been experienced in that particular context (feature extraction). Thus, all features of an episode can be recalled, even when only one of the input layers is cued.
Simulation of episodic memory tasks
Item and context representations consisted of sets of 8 nodes in the item and context layers, respectively. Item representations were nonoverlapping, while context representations overlapped randomly. During learning, synaptic transmission in the feedback connections of the hippocampal layer was dampened so that the activity in the network was largely determined by the “online” inputs.38,39 Learning then occurred over 3 iterations for each item-context pairing. Following the learning session, synaptic transmission in the feedback connections was restored, to allow the influence of feedback activity during subsequent retrieval sessions.
As the performance measure, we used feedback from the entorhinal layer to the item representations. The assumption underlying this mechanism was that episodic memory only stores feature-impoverished co-occurrences of patterns that are themselves stored elsewhere, and that episodic retrieval serves to reactivate these patterns in the (cortical) brain areas where they are encoded. After cue setting, the model was allowed to update its activity over 50 cycles, or until any item representation reached threshold. If the feedback input to any item pattern (averaged over nodes that were within the pattern but not part of a cue) crossed a threshold, it was counted as retrieved, and the entorhinal and hippocampal layers were reset to a random activity state. Thresholds were set halfway between the distributions derived for feedback to the item layer for studied items and foils, respectively. Each simulation was repeated at least 50 times, with random initial settings; presented results are averages. Simulations were kept on a semiquantitative level (ie, parameter space was not searched to produce a best quantitative fit for the experimental data).
Procedure for Retrieval and Recognition
In typical episodic memory experiments, participants learn a list of items and then are asked to retrieve the learned material. List learning was implemented by presenting the model with a list of 10 items. The item representations were activated one at a time, together with one stable context representation, common to all the items. Here, “context” represents all information that remains stable over the course of the learning trial, such as the environment in which learning takes place. A characteristic of episodic memory is that retrieval is highly sensitive to context.40,41 The model was allowed to learn each item-context combination. Retrieval was then tested under conditions representing free recall and recognition. In free recall, participants must reproduce the entire list, using only context information as a retrieval cue. A context cue was set by activating part (75%) of the context pattern that had been active during learning. In recognition, an additional item cue was given, consisting of 75% of an entire item pattern (not 100%, because aspects of item presentation may differ between study and test phase). This paradigm included presentation of foils (items that did not occur on the studied list) at test, which had to be rejected.
In free recall, the number of different list items retrieved over 150 test iterations, with only the context cue set, was used as the performance measure. Intrusions were scored when an item not on the list was retrieved, omissions when list items were not retrieved. Any pattern reaching threshold during recognition was counted as an “old” response by the model, except when this occurred in response to a foil item; then a false alarm was scored. When no pattern was retrieved, a “new” response was counted. Parameters for each simulation are listed in the Table.
Procedure for Proactive Interference
Proactive interference was evaluated through a typical design, in which 2 lists of paired associates are learned sequentially. The task is to generate the second word of a pair (target word) when cued with the first word (cue word). Cue words used in the first list (the AB list) are repeated in the second list (the AC list), but are then associated with different target words (eg, dog-lamp in the first list, dog-egg in the second list). Participants are tested on the AB list after studying only that list, then they study the second list (AC) and are tested on it. A typical finding is decreased performance on the AC list with respect to the AB list, presumably because the first response associated with a cue word interferes with storage or retrieval of the second response associated with the cue.
The AB-AC paradigm was implemented with pairs of 4-node items. The simulation started with the successive presentation of 10 pairs in the AB list, which were activated with the associated context. The activations emerging in the model were learned. In a subsequent retrieval test, the model was presented with the first item of each item pair (cue word), as well as a 75% context cue. The model searched through memory for the associated target items. Next, the AC list of 10 pairs was learned with a second associated context and retrieval was tested again. To simulate that the 2 lists are presented during the same experimental session with only a brief delay interposed, the context patterns associated with the first and second lists overlapped by 50%.
Schizophrenia was modeled as decreased connectivity from the input layers (item and context) to the entorhinal layer, and from the entorhinal to the hippocampal layer. Both sets of connections were decreased by 50%. Although the size of the reduction was chosen to clearly show the effects of the manipulation, reported reductions in synaptic markers in the hippocampal formation are indeed substantial. Harrison and Eastwood21 reviewed connectivity-related alterations in the hippocampus proper: synaptic proteins were reduced by 25% to 60% and spine density by about 70%. Recently, spinophilin, a dendritic spine marker, was shown to be reduced in entorhinal cortex by 27%.42 Connectivity findings are probably broadly similar in entorhinal and perirhinal cortices and the hippocampus (Paul Harrison, PhD, MD, e-mail communication, May 17, 2004). To analyze the effects of individual pathways, we also considered models in which just 1 of the 2 levels of connectivity (inputs-to-entorhinal or entorhinal-to-hippocampus) was reduced by 50%, as illustrated in Figure 2B and C.
To investigate the specificity of the schizophrenia manipulation, 2 alternative manipulations to reduced wiring were evaluated: progressively reducing nodes in the hippocampal layer, to test whether partial hippocampal damage could cause the deficits, and progressively increasing noise in the system’s entorhinal and hippocampal layers. The latter manipulation was chosen in view of speculations that a decreased signal-to-noise ratio in regions involved in semantic processing may underlie some of the cognitive deficits in the disorder.43 Both lesions are described in more detail in additional materials (http://www.neuromod.org/data/archives).
Retrieval and recognition
The model was able to free recall, on average, 49% of list items in the given time. In recognition, the partly activated item representations guided the search process, leading to retrieval of 87.5% of patterns (Figure 3). The model produced only a small percentage of false alarms and intrusions. The relative performance of free recall and recognition is consistent with that in healthy subjects (Figure 3A and B).
The full (2-level) wiring manipulation resulted in a preferential reduction of free recall and a much milder deficit in recognition (Figure 3). The relative impairments in free recall and recognition accurately reproduce the data pattern in schizophrenic patients (Figure 3A and B). This is also the case when recognition is measured as d′ (a signal detection measure of discriminability), with d′ in the model and in schizophrenic subjects being reduced to 65.4% and 65.8% of control value, respectively.
The 2 levels of connectivity contributed differently to the memory impairment (Figure 3C): reduction of the lower level (inputs-to-entorhinal) severely impaired free recall, with only a slight reduction in recognition hit rate. In addition, false alarms were increased. These effects can be understood in the following manner: Successful free recall depends on the formation of entorhinal patterns in the intersection of projections from the context and the active item. However, the decreased density of input projections reduces the probability that a given entorhinal node receives input from both sources (see again Figure 2B). This favors the inclusion of nodes receiving only context or only item input in entorhinal representations. The latter nodes cannot be activated by the context cue, compromising free recall. Impairment is far less pronounced when item cues are additionally provided, because, with cues from both sources, entorhinal nodes receiving single-source input can potentially be activated.
In contrast, reduction of entorhinal outputs to the hippocampal module led to mild deficits on both memory paradigms. Here, the cause is hampered orthogonalization over the entorhinal-hippocampal pathway, increasing mean overlap between hippocampal patterns (from 6% to 13%). Consequently, nodes in the hippocampal layer activated overlapping and competing representations in the entorhinal layer, resulting in fewer entorhinal patterns being completed by their hippocampal layer companion and fewer items reaching threshold (see again Figure 2C). Notably, this deficit involves not the sampling of memories, but their completion once they are sampled. Retrieval therefore suffers irrespective of cueing condition, and performance in both memory tasks drops to a similar extent.
In essence, the reduction of the lower level of connectivity caused episodic traces to be poorly associated to both cortical input sources. This preferentially affected retrieval after single-source cues, as in free recall. The reduction of entorhinal outputs to the hippocampal layer led to a mild deficit in pattern segregation, making some patterns entirely irretrievable. The schizophrenia memory profile resulted from the concatenation of these 2 problems.
Both increased levels of noise (Figure 4) and lesions to the hippocampal layer (Figure 5) affected memory performance. Specifically, free-recall performance benefited from hippocampal noise but was highly sensitive to entorhinal noise. Recognition, on the other hand, was resistant to noise: although the maximal noise levels in the simulation provided more input than each cue separately, context and item cues were still able to guide retrieval to the appropriate patterns. At levels of noise where recognition performance did start to deteriorate, free recall had already reached extremely low levels.
For all grades of hippocampal node reduction, free recall, cued recall, and recognition were affected to a similar extent, while there were no increases in intrusions, false alarms, or responses in a novel context. In view of the global effects on memory performance, this pattern is akin to that seen after mild damage to the hippocampus proper,44,45 rather than that described in schizophrenia. There was thus no degree of noise or of hippocampal node reduction that reproduced the episodic memory profile observed in schizophrenia.
In normal subjects, retrieval of items suffers from the presence of similar items in memory. These proactive interference effects appear to be dwarfed by the general memory deficit in schizophrenic patients (Figure 6A).46,47 As an additional test of the model, we investigated whether it would reproduce these surprising findings.
In this simulation, both the control and the schizophrenia condition produced an interference effect, in that the number of hits was lower on the AC list than on the AB list (Figure 6B). In raw scores, the interference effect was slightly larger in the schizophrenia condition (5% vs 10%). This was also the case in the study by Elvevåg et al,46 but the observed difference was not statistically significant. We therefore subjected our data to a repeated-measures analysis of variance with model permutation (simulated schizophrenia, normal model) as the between-group factor and list (list 1, list 2) as the within-group factor. Not surprisingly, there was a main effect of group, due to the simulated patients’ inferior performance (F1,198 = 1595.6; P<.001), and of list, with performance on list 1 being superior to that on list 2 (F1,198 = 13.8; P<.001). However, there was no significant interaction between group and list (F1,198 = 3.05; P = .08), despite 100 simulated participants per model condition. As in Elvevåg and coworkers’ analysis,46 the interference effect was dominated by the overall memory deficit in the patient group.
As an additional measure of interference, intrusions were scored whenever the model produced a response associated with the first list (AB) during testing of the second list (AC). In line with Elvevåg and coworkers’ results,46 we did not find a preferential increase in the number of intrusions (from 10% of all answers in the intact model to 14% in the schizophrenia manipulation, which amounts to 20.3% and 20.7%, respectively, of all errors).
As in the earlier simulations, the schizophrenia data pattern was not replicated with the use of alternative lesions (Figure 6C). Increasing noise levels, surprisingly, led to a disappearance of interference effects. Reducing the size of the hippocampal layer, on the other hand, increased the size of interference effects substantially, while leaving overall memory performance relatively intact. Both of these results are discussed in the additional material on the authors’ Web site.
In this study, an integrated model of the MTL was used to elucidate differential contributions of the parahippocampal gyrus and hippocampus proper to episodic memory. In the model, dual sensory processing pathways, conveying object-based and contextual aspects of an episode, converge on the hippocampal formation, where a representation of their co-occurrence is stored. The function of the parahippocampal region in the model is to cross-associate the information in the 2 input streams. In line with previous studies, the hippocampal module is required to quickly store the co-occurrence representations while enhancing pattern separation.34,48 As shown in the current and in previous studies, these 2 aspects of memory processing rely on different network characteristics and, thus, are most efficiently subserved by dedicated circuitry.32
Although the model incorporates only crude principles of MTL organization, it captures several realistic features of human memory. For instance, retrieval is highly context sensitive; sampling with a context cue elicits few intrusions and false alarms. Furthermore, even small differences in context can discriminate between episodes incorporating the same item, for instance, in the AB-AC paradigm, where correct episodes are retrieved with small, albeit significant, interference effects.
While we tuned the model to replicate normal memory function, MTL abnormalities and their effects on performance were investigated in a principled manner. Out of various in silico lesions, only reduced wiring of the parahippocampal region and its hippocampopetal projections produced the memory deficit profile observed in schizophrenia. This manipulation led to compromised cross-association of episodic features and a superimposed, mild reduction of pattern separation in the system. The latter malfunction made some patterns irretrievable, affecting all memory tasks including recognition, and leading to what has been interpreted as an encoding deficit. The cross-association problem hampered “searching” of the memory store. This preferentially affected tasks with a large retrieval demand, such as free recall, leading to what has been interpreted as a retrieval deficit. In fact, our results indicate that both manifestations of memory impairment are due to abnormal encoding.
The reduced parahippocampal connectivity also led to a shift in the balance of inputs on the entorhinal layer, in favor of top-down ones. Thus, the system became biased to reinstate stored patterns, even when these were not associated with the online sensory input cues. This contributed to the increase in false-positive memories and intrusions. However, this network abnormality may also underlie cognitive problems that are less obviously related to memory impairment.
Our model has interesting correspondences to models developed by McGlashan and Hoffman.49 In their simulations, excessive pruning of connections in modules representing association cortex caused the network to produce percepts spontaneously, in the absence of input. They related these observations to formation of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Their results and ours both suggest that altered connectivity may play an important role in the syndrome.
The model currently has several limitations. For one, it learns only associations between item and context, disregarding interitem associations. Furthermore, forgetting was not explicitly modeled, although some overwriting of older patterns by newer ones did occur. Finally, in the present model, the 2 putative process components of recognition, recollection and familiarity,50-52 are only distinguished in a rudimentary manner (ie, retrieval via the hippocampal layer might reflect recollection, and direct activation by input nodes of associated entorhinal nodes may contribute familiarity). While implementing these features would make the model more general, there is no a priori reason to think that they would change the patterns in performance presented herein.
Finally, an important question for this model relates to the falsifiable hypotheses that it generates. The answer can be approached on 2 levels. First, the model could be falsified by neuropathological evidence in schizophrenia that MTL abnormalities are not due to connectivity. Second, it could be falsified through specific predictions about schizophrenic memory performance. For example, we demonstrated that what appeared to be disproportionate failures in retrieval were, in fact, failures due to compromised encoding. This leads to the testable prediction that the retrieval deficit should be relatively independent of circumstances at the time of recall. Our model may also be used to predict performance on aspects of episodic memory that have not yet been extensively characterized in schizophrenic patients. For instance, simulations are currently under way to predict effects of connectivity loss on source monitoring, familiarity, and word frequency effects in free recall and recognition.
In conclusion, we devised a model of MTL episodic memory function that was tuned to mimic normal performance in free recall, cued recall, recognition, and paired associate learning with interference. We then subjected our in silico model to a variety of lesions in a principled manner. We found decisive evidence that only massive losses in connectivity resulted in a “schizophrenia-like” profile of memory performance. From an information-processing standpoint, we also were able to demonstrate that what appeared to be disproportionate failures in retrieval were due to compromised encoding.
Correspondence: Terry E. Goldberg, PhD, Clinical Brain Disorders Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, 10 Center Dr, MSC 1379, Bethesda, MD 20892 (GoldberT@INTRA.NIMH.nih.gov).
Submitted for Publication: September 18, 2003; final revision received August 11, 2004; accepted September 9, 2004.
Acknowledgment: We thank Menno Witter, PhD, for suggestions regarding structure and function of the MTL and Ester Holzhausen, PhD, for her help in defining the neuropsychological memory profile in schizophrenia.
2.Pantelis
CedNelson
HEedBarnes
TREed Schizophrenia: A Neuropsychological Perspective. Chichester, England John Wiley & Sons1996;
4.Saykin
AJShtasel
DLGur
REKester
DBMozley
LHStafiniak
PGur
RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51124- 131
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Aleman
AHijman
RDe Haan
EKahn
R Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;1561358- 1366
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Rushe
TMWoodruff
PWMurray
RMMorris
RGM Episodic memory and learning in patients with chronic schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 1999;3585- 96
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Paulsen
JSHeaton
RKSadek
JRPerry
WDelis
DCBraff
DKuck
JZisook
SJeste
DV The nature of learning and memory impairments in schizophrenia.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1995;188- 99
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Brebion
GAmador
XSmith
MJGorman
JM Mechanisms underlying memory impairment in schizophrenia.
Psychol Med 1997;27383- 393
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Gold
JMRehkemper
GBinks
SW
IIICarpenter
CJFleming
KGoldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Learning and forgetting in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol 2000;109534- 538
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Goldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Effects of neuroleptic medication on the cognition of patients with schizophrenia: a review of recent studies.
J Clin Psychiatry 1996;5762- 65
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Harvey
PDKeefe
RSE Studies of cognitive change in patients with schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treatment.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158176- 184
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Weickert
TEgan
MFWeinberger
DRGoldberg
TE Preserved and compromised intellect in schizophrenia: neurocognitive implications.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57907- 913
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Goldberg
TETorrey
EFBerman
KFWeinberger
DR Relations between neuropsychological performance and brain morphological and physiological measures in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res 1994;5551- 61
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Gur
RETuretsky
BICowell
PEFinkelman
CMaany
VGrossman
RIArnold
SEBilker
WBGur
RC Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57769- 775
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Crespo-Facorro
BWiser
AKAndreasen
NCO’Leary
DSWatkins
GLBoles Ponto
LLHichwa
RD Neural basis of novel and well-learned recognition memory in schizophrenia: a positron emission tomography study.
Hum Brain Mapp 2001;12219- 231
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Gold
JMRandolph
CCarpenter
CJGoldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Forms of memory failure in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol 1992;101487- 494
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Arnold
SE Cellular and molecular neuropathology of the parahippocampal region in schizophrenia.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;911275- 292
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Hemby
SEGinsberg
SDBrunk
BArnold
SETrojanowski
JQEberwine
JH Gene expression profile for schizophrenia: discrete neuron transcription patterns in the entorhinal cortex.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59631- 640
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Harrison
PJEastwood
SL Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia.
Hippocampus 2001;11508- 519
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Nakazawa
KQuirk
MCChitwood
RAWatanabe
MYeckel
MFSun
LDKato
ACarr
CAJohnston
DWilson
MATonegawa
S Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall.
Science 2002;297211- 218
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Suzuki
WAAmaral
DG The perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents.
J Comp Neurol 1994;350497- 533
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Aggleton
JPBrown
MW Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis.
Behav Brain Sci 1999;22425- 489
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Murray
EABussey
TJHampton
RRSaksida
LM The parahippocampal region and object identification.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;911166- 174
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Bohbot
VDAllen
JJNadel
L Memory deficits characterized by patterns of lesions to the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;911355- 368
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Vann
SDBrown
MWErichsen
JTAggleton
JP Fos imaging reveals differential patterns of hippocampal and parahippocampal subfield activation in rats in response to different spatial memory tests.
J Neurosci 2000;202711- 2718
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Suzuki
WAMiller
EKDesimone
R Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex.
J Neurophysiol 1997;781062- 1081
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Hasselmo
MEWyble
BP Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function.
Behav Brain Res 1997;891- 34
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Meeter
MMurre
JMJTalamini
LM A computational approach to memory deficits in schizophrenia.
Neurocomputing 2002;44-46929- 936
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Vargha-Khadem
FGadian
DGWatkins
KEConnelly
APaesschen
WVMishkin
M Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory.
Science 1997;277376- 390
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.O’Reilly
RCMcClelland
JL Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off.
Hippocampus 1994;4661- 682
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Levy
WBColbert
CMDesmond
NL Elemental adaptive processes in neurons and synapses: a statistical/computational perspective. Gluck
MARumelhart
DEeds.Neuroscience and Connectionist Theory Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence A Erlbaum Assoc1990;187- 235
38.Hasselmo
MEBradley
PWyble
BPWallenstein
GV Encoding and retrieval of episodic memories: role of cholinergic and GABAergic modulation in the hippocampus.
Hippocampus 1996;6693- 708
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Meeter
MMurre
JMJTalamini
LM Mode shifting between storage and recall based on novelty detection in oscillating hippocampal circuits.
Hippocampus 2004;14722- 741
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Godden
DRBaddeley
AD Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater.
Br J Psychol 1975;66325- 331
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Law
AJWeickert
CSHyde
TMKleinman
JEHarrison
PJ Reduced spinophilin but not microtubule-associated protein 2 expression in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia and mood disorder: molecular evidence for a pathology of dendritic spines.
Am J Psychiatry 2004;1611848- 1855
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Reed
JMSquire
LR Impaired recognition memory in patients with lesions limited to the hippocampal formation.
Behav Neurosci 1997;111667- 675
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Rempel-Clower
NLZola
SMSquire
LRAmaral
DG Three cases of enduring memory impairment after bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation.
J Neurosci 1996;165233- 5255
PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.Elvevåg
BEgan
MFGoldberg
TE Paired-associate learning and memory interference in schizophrenia.
Neuropsychologia 2000;381565- 1575
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.O’Carroll
REMurray
CAustin
MPEbmeier
KPGoodwin
GMDunan
J Proactive interference and the neuropsychology of schizophrenia.
Br J Clin Psychol 1993;32353- 356
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.McGlashan
THHoffman
RE Schizophrenia as a disorder of developmentally reduced synaptic connectivity.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57637- 648
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Norman
KAO’Reilly
RC Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary learning systems approach.
Psychol Rev 2003;110611- 646
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Yonelinas
AP The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of research.
J Mem Lang 2002;46441- 517
Google ScholarCrossref