Context
Relapse is a common clinical problem in individuals with substance dependence. Previous studies have implicated a multifactorial process underlying relapse; however, the contribution of specific neural substrates has not yet been examined.
Objective
To determine whether results from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) shortly after drug cessation could predict relapse in stimulant-dependent individuals.
Participants and Design
Treatment-seeking methamphetamine-dependent males (N = 46) underwent fMRI 3 to 4 weeks after cessation of drug use. Of the 40 subjects who were followed up a median of 370 days, 18 relapsed and 22 did not.
Main Outcome Measure
Blood oxygen level–dependent fMRI activation during a simple 2-choice prediction task.
Results
The fMRI activation patterns in right insular, posterior cingulate, and temporal cortex obtained early in recovery correctly predicted 20 of 22 subjects who did not relapse and 17 of 18 subjects who did. A Cox regression analysis revealed that the combination of right middle frontal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and posterior cingulate activation best predicted the time to relapse.
Conclusion
To our knowledge, this is the first investigation to show that fMRI can be used to predict relapse in substance-dependent individuals.
In 2002, about 22 million Americans, or 9.4% of the US population 12 years or older, were estimated to have substance abuse or dependence.1 Among these, the prevalence with methamphetamine-related problems has more than doubled between 1991 and 1998.1 Worldwide, more than 40 million people have used methamphetamine and other synthetic stimulants, making amphetamines and cocaine the most widely misused illicit substances after cannabis.2 Many of these men and women develop repetitive problems with these drugs and fulfill criteria for dependence.3
A central characteristic of substance dependence is the relapsing nature of the disorder.4 The return to drug use is a complex process that occurs within a year in more than 50% of people with stimulant dependence who seek treatment.5 A multitude of factors contribute to relapse, such as the environmental context, presence of substance cues, personal coping repertoire, and an escalation of craving after experience with the drug.6 The processes of relapse are thought to include cognitive-behavioral,7 person-situation interactional,8 cognitive-appraisal,9 and outcome-expectation factors.10,11 In comparison, neural system factors have been less well studied.
The ability to make appropriate decisions (to select the best action from a set of alternatives with uncertain outcomes) is critical for daily functioning in healthy human beings. Dysfunctional decision making may characterize substance-dependent individuals12 and may contribute to relapse. Methamphetamine-dependent subjects tend to select actions associated with short-term gains, even when they lead to long-term losses, more often than actions with small short-term but larger long-term gains.13 Relative to healthy volunteers, these individuals are more likely to select risky responses14 and less likely to consider long-term sequelae of choices.15 In addition, stimulant-dependent individuals do not appropriately take into account the probability and magnitude of reward of the available options.16 Finally, these subjects generate perseverative response patterns when making a prediction and select actions that are more stimulus bound and less related to changes in the frequency of prediction errors.17,18 These behavioral dysfunctions may reflect an alteration of brain circuits that are critical for decision making. Functional neuroimaging studies have revealed dysfunctions of the inferior prefrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in stimulant-dependent subjects.19-21 It has been argued that some of these abnormalities may remit over time.20
The main aim of this study was to determine whether functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a decision-making task can be used to predict relapse in treatment-seeking methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Specifically, we hypothesized that relapsing methamphetamine-dependent subjects relative to nonrelapsing individuals show less activation in inferior and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, and insula. Support for this hypothesis would provide an approach for assessing susceptibility to relapse during early treatment.
The University of California San Diego (La Jolla), Human Research Protections Program approved this study, and each subject gave informed consent prior to participating. The 46 treatment-seeking male subjects all met criteria for current (ie, at least prior 6 months) dependence on methamphetamine according to DSM-IV as gathered from face-to-face interviews using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV.3,22 These individuals were not dependent on any other drug or alcohol but could fulfill criteria for abuse. Twelve subjects had comorbid marijuana abuse and 14 had alcohol abuse. All men had voluntarily entered and completed a 28-day inpatient alcohol and drug treatment program at the San Diego Veterans Affairs Medical Center, which is based on a cognitive-behavioral model and includes a 12-step component with intense daily group sessions, education, and regularly scheduled Alcoholics Anonymous meetings. All individuals are randomly tested for drugs and presence of alcohol; any positive screen resulted in immediate discharge from the program. At the time of scanning, participants were abstinent from methamphetamine for 27.6 days (SD = 9.5; range, 14-51 days) and had used methamphetamine for 16.0 years (SD = 9.1; range, 3-34 years).
A comprehensive assessment was obtained to determine whether performance on the decision-making task or fMRI activation patterns at baseline were related to patients’ psychiatric symptom profiles. Interview-based symptomatic assessment was based on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale,23 Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale,24 and the Young Mania Rating Scale.25
The 2-choice prediction task has been described in detail elsewhere.26 Briefly, subjects were instructed to “predict” where a stimulus would appear on a computer screen. Unbeknownst to the participants, the outcome for each trial was predetermined in such a way that individuals “correctly predict” 50% of all trials. To isolate the brain activation due to the decision-making process, a comparison task was used during which subjects were asked to push a button to acknowledge a target on the screen. The key difference between these 2 tasks was that during the prediction task, the subject did not know the correct response in advance and had to decide in the presence of uncertainty using the previous responses, stimuli, and outcomes. In comparison, during the response task, the subject knew the correct answer before selecting a response, decided in the presence of certainty, and did not need to use the sequences of previous responses or outcomes.
fMRI PROTOCOL AND IMAGE ANALYSIS PATHWAY
Magnetic resonance images were obtained using a 1.5-T, whole-body system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Anatomical T1-weighted images of the whole brain (magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo, repetition time = 11.4 milliseconds, echo time = 4.4 milliseconds, flip angle = 10°, field of view = 256 × 256, 1-mm3 voxels) were obtained sagittally to identify the anterior and posterior commissures for coregistration of the echo planar images and transformation of the images into Talairach space.27 Thirty-two sections of T2-weighted images were obtained in the transverse plane using gradient-recalled echo planar imaging (echo time = 40 milliseconds, flip angle = 90°, 64 × 64-pixel field of view = 220 × 220 mm, 3-mm contiguous section thickness) every 3000 milliseconds for 112 (n = 19) or 128 (n = 27) repetitions yielding a voxel size of 3.43 mm2 × 3 mm.
All structural and functional image processing used the Analysis of Functional Neuroimages software package.28 Echo planar images were coregistered using a 3-dimensional coregistration algorithm29 that resulted in the smallest amount of image translation and rotation relative to all other images. All data were resampled to 4.0-mm3 voxels, and a Gaussian filter with full width at half maximum of 6 mm was applied to account for individual variations in anatomical landmarks. Data for each subject were normalized to standard coordinates.27 The voxel-wise percentage signal change data (ie, signal difference between echo planar image intensity during the prediction task and the response task) were entered into a mixed model analysis of variance, which nested group (relapsers vs nonrelapsers) and subjects as a random factor. A threshold-adjustment method based on Monte-Carlo simulations guarded against identifying false-positive areas of activation,30 and labels for brain activation foci were confirmed using the Talairach Daemon software.31
Participants were contacted 1 year after the imaging session. Several approaches that have been successful in large-scale longitudinal studies32 were used. For example, individuals were contacted by telephone or mail 12 months after the initial evaluation, using addresses of friends or family members or the Department of Motor Vehicle records to locate them. Despite these efforts, 6 individuals could not be reached for follow up. Four subjects who were unable to complete in-person or telephone interviews returned questionnaires by mail. Sobriety was assessed using a questionnaire based on the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism,33 focusing on the use of methamphetamines during the interval between the neuroimaging evaluation and date of the follow-up. Relapse was defined as any use of methamphetamine during any time after discharge from the inpatient unit, with the date of relapse defined as the day of first use. No other substance use was considered. The median duration of the follow-up interval for the 40 individuals for whom follow-up data were available was 370 days (SD = 203.3; range, 360-967 days).
Several analyses were carried out to determine the degree to which brain activation 1 month after cessation of drug use predicted relapse. The receiver operating characteristic curves were determined for each functional region of interest. Two exploratory analyses were carried out to determine whether activation patterns at baseline predicted relapse at follow-up. A stepwise linear discriminant function analysis (Fenter: P<.05) was computed with relapse as the dependent measure and the activation patterns in the areas that differed across relapsers and nonrelapsers as independent measures. A cross-validation procedure using a leave-1-out classification method (predictions were generated by resampling with 1 subject removed) was used to determine sensitivity and specificity of the activation patterns to predict relapse. Finally, a stepwise Cox regression analysis (Fenter: P<.05) with time to relapse as the dependent measure and activation in the regions that differentiated relapsers from nonrelapsers as independent measures was used to determine whether the activation patterns at baseline were able to predict the time to relapse. For the Cox regression analyses, time zero was taken as the sobriety date before entering the alcohol and drug treatment program and relapse was defined by the date of first use.
Behavioral characteristics of relapsers vs nonrelapsers
During the follow-up period, 18 of 40 subjects relapsed a median of 279 days after initial testing (SD = 214; range, 36-820 days) (Figure 1). None of the sociodemographic characteristics, baseline symptom indicators, use characteristics, or behavioral measures on the 2-choice prediction task significantly differentiated those who relapsed during the follow-up period vs those who did not (Table 1). Specifically, both nonrelapsers and relapsers generated nonrandom sequences of responses as evidenced by significantly more win-stay (nonrelapsers, t17 = 2.63; P = .01; relapsers, t17 = 4.65; P<.01) as well as lose-shift (nonrelapsers, t17 = 2.42; P = .03; relapsers, t17 = 3.85; P<.01) consistent responses than would be expected by chance. There were no significant differences between the groups except relapsers had higher scores on the Young Mania Rating Scale (ie, exhibited more symptoms consistent with hypomania or mania at time of fMRI testing); however the Young Mania Rating Scale scores were not correlated with any brain activation during the 2-choice prediction task.
Functional neuroimaging characteristics
Similar to previous studies,34 bilateral prefrontal cortex, striatum, posterior parietal cortex, and anterior insula were more active during the 2-choice prediction task relative to the 2-choice response task.35 Within these regions, 9 areas differentiated relapsing and nonrelapsing subjects (Figure 2) (Table 2), including prefrontal, parietal, and insular cortex. Nonrelapsing individuals showed more activation in these areas relative to relapsing individuals (Figure 2). The difference in activation patterns was not due to differences in motion between relapsers and nonrelapsers during imaging (troll38 = 0.48; P = .63; tpitch24 = 0.67; P = .50; tyaw24 = 0.90; P = .37) and were not correlated with motion parameters obtained during magnetic resonance imaging.
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RELAPSE AND fMRI FINDINGS
For each area with a specificity of at least 83.3% (correctly predicting 15 of 18 individuals who relapsed), sensitivity ranged from 54.5% (12 of 22 correctly predicted to not relapse) to 90.9% (20 of 22 correctly predicted to not relapse) (Figure 2). The stepwise discriminant function (Table 3), with the areas of differences between relapsers and nonrelapsers as the independent measure and relapse status as the dependent measure, revealed that right insula, right posterior cingulate, and right middle temporal gyrus response best differentiated between relapsing and nonrelapsing methamphetamine-dependent subjects (Wilks λ = 0.37; χ2 = 36.5; P<.01). As presented in Table 4, cross-validation analysis was able to correctly predict 19 of 22 individuals who did not relapse (but predicted 3 individuals to relapse who did not) and predicted 17 of 18 who relapsed (94.4% sensitivity; 86.4% specificity). The stepwise Cox regression analysis (Table 5) showed that time to relapse was best predicted by right middle frontal gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, and right posterior cingulate cortex activation. In combination, low activation in these areas at baseline was highly predictive of time to relapse (χ23 = 23.9; P<.01). Additional Cox regression and discriminant function analyses using individuals who were followed up for up to mean ± SD 365 ± 30 days yielded similarly sensitive and specific results in a stepwise discriminant function analysis (data not shown).
To our knowledge, this investigation provides evidence for the first time that functional neuroimaging can be used to predict relapse in individuals with methamphetamine dependence. As hypothesized, methamphetamine-dependent subjects who relapse during the follow-up interval show less activation in dorsolateral prefrontal, parietal, and temporal cortex, as well as insula, a network of structures that is critical for decision making. More importantly, the degree of predictability (ie, the high sensitivity and specificity) supports the idea that functional neuroimaging may prove to be a useful clinical tool to assess relapse susceptibility.
The neural activation differences between relapsing and nonrelapsing methamphetamine-dependent subjects are part of a system involved with the processing of decision making. Specifically, the right inferior parietal lobule, which maintains bidirectional connections to right dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior insular cortex, has been implicated in a number of processes that are part of the assessment component of decision making. These include sustained, and possibly selective, attention,36 switching from task-relevant local to global targets,37 and voluntary attentional control,38 as well as the distinction between task-irrelevant and task-relevant events.39 Thus, this area may be critical for the extraction and selection of task-relevant information and has been implicated in inhibitory control with a number of different paradigms.40 Moreover, several studies have implicated the right inferior parietal lobule in autonomic arousal processes,41 risk-taking decision making,42 and guessing.43 The right inferior parietal lobule, which integrates attentional resources to select actions (predicting the location of the stimulus) and inhibits previous prediction strategies as they relate to success or failure, may be critical for the assessment process during decision making. Therefore, proper activation during decision making is important for allocating processing resources to actions that have to compete with habitual behavior.44 Attenuated activation by subjects who are at high risk for relapse may represent defective assessment abilities and subsequent reliance on habitual behavior. In other words, these individuals do not mount sufficient executive and/or controlled processing when making a decision.
Similarly, insula activation occurs in a wide variety of task conditions, but there is an emerging consensus that insula activation is frequently associated with the assessment of emotionally aversive states. Specifically, functional neuroimaging studies have shown insula-related activation during the processing of fearful45 or disgusted46 faces and during the anticipation of electric shocks,47 as well as during script-evoked sad mood induction.48 Moreover, insula activity was modulated by perceptual awareness of threat,49 penalty,50 or error-related processes.44 The insular cortex appears to be important for integrating cognitive and affective information.51 Anatomical studies in rhesus monkeys have shown that the insula receives input from both dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex.52 In particular, the rostral part of the posterior parietal lobe sends efferents to the insular cortex,53 and the insula receives projections from the amygdala.54 Insula activation during decision-making tasks may alert the individual of expected aversive outcomes. Thus, a reduced activation in this area would be consistent with a diminished ability to differentiate between choices that lead to good vs poor outcomes, which may be a key factor in relapse.
We used a block fMRI paradigm during a simple 2-choice prediction task to assess decision making.18,35 Although this task yields robust activations in circuits that are important for decision making, such as anterior cingulate,55,56 anterior insula,57 posterior parietal cortex,58 and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,35 it is difficult to identify a precise component of decision making. Thus, one cannot rule out that other processes (eg, attentional dysfunction, working memory problems, or impulsivity) contribute to the observed differences. However, the lack of behavioral differences between relapsers or nonrelapsers on response latency, frequency of responses, or use of different strategies render it less likely that general attentional factors account for the neuroimaging differences. Nevertheless, future investigations will need to use event-related designs to begin to better isolate which aspect of decision making is most important for predicting relapse and accounts for the activation differences.
Methamphetamine is known to have neurotoxic effects.59 Specifically, increased aptoptosis60 and altered concentration of N-acetylaspartate, a marker of neuronal integrity, in the basal ganglia61 and in the anterior cingulate62 support the general hypothesis that methamphetamine structurally alters the brain.63 Therefore, it would not be surprising if long-term use affects the neural substrates important for decision making. Although the current study cannot address whether the differences between relapsing and nonrelapsing individuals are due to structural brain differences, the current results provide compelling evidence that functional differences exist and may stimulate future investigations into the underlying structural differences.
The degree of outcome predictability based on the neuroimaging findings is striking. However, several cautionary notes are necessary. First, subjects in this study were treatment-seeking male veterans with methamphetamine dependence who were included in the study only if they did not fulfill criteria for another comorbid substance dependence. Thus, it is unclear how these findings generalize to other substances of abuse or to individuals who use multiple intoxicants. Second, although the follow-up period was initially set for 1 year, we included subjects who were eventually located and studied up to 967 days later. Although the Cox regression model takes different follow-up intervals into account when calculating the factors contributing to relapse, one may argue that a more rigorous study is necessary to substantiate the current finding. Third, a limited number of variables, focused on the presence of psychiatric symptoms at baseline, were included in the assessment. It has been pointed out that models of relapse prediction should be multifactorial and should include cognitive, psychophysiological, sociodemographic or social, and psychological assessment domains.6 Therefore, future studies will need to include these factors and begin to relate differences in these domains to brain activation patterns. Finally, stepwise regression analyses are often sensitive to the sample characteristics. Therefore, it may be beneficial to consider the statistical characteristics of individual functional regions of interest when predicting relapse. In this study, sensitivity ranged from 54% to 91%; future investigations will need to identify why different neural substrates are more predictive (ie, have high specificity and high sensitivity).
The current investigation demonstrates that functional neuroimaging may be useful for long-term clinical predictions in stimulant dependence. Altered activation during decision making may play a critical role in processes that “set the stage” for relapse.6 Understanding the nature of these dysfunctions may ultimately help to develop new treatment approaches that are targeted to attenuate or rehabilitate these dysfunctions and to increase patients’ chances for long-term sobriety.
Correspondence: Martin P. Paulus, MD, Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0603 (mpaulus@ucsd.edu).
Submitted for Publication: May 24, 2004; final revision received November 19, 2004; accepted December 16, 2004.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants DA13186 and DA016663 from the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Paulus).
Acknowledgment: We would like to acknowledge the invaluable help of Kelly Winternheimer, BS, and Thuy Le, BS.
1.Department of Health and Human Services, Results From the 2002 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH). Rockville, Md SAMHSA, Office of Applied Studies2003;Office of Applied Studies, NHSDA Series H-22, DHHS Publication SMA 03-3836
3.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
4.Miller
WRWesterberg
VSHarris
RJTonigan
JS What predicts relapse? prospective testing of antecedent models.
Addiction 1996;91
((suppl))
S155- S172
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Marlatt
GAGordon
JR Relapse Prevention: Maintenance Strategies in the Treatment of Addictive Behaviors. New York, NY Guilford Press1985;
8.Litman
GKStapleton
JOppenheim
ANPeleg
MJackson
P The relationship between coping behaviours, their effectiveness and alcoholism relapse and survival.
Br J Addict 1984;79283- 291
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Sanchez-Craig
BM Cognitive and behavioral coping strategies in the reappraisal of stressful social situations.
J Couns Psychol 1976;237- 12
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Rollnick
SHeather
N The application of Bandura's self-efficacy theory to abstinence-oriented alcoholism treatment.
Addict Behav 1982;7243- 250
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Annis
HM A cognitive-social learning approach to relapse: pharmacotherapy and relapse prevention counselling.
Alcohol Alcohol Suppl 1991;1527- 530
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Grant
SContoreggi
CLondon
ED Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making.
Neuropsychologia 2000;381180- 1187
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Bechara
ADamasio
H Decision-making and addiction (part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences.
Neuropsychologia 2002;401675- 1689
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Lane
SDCherek
DR Analysis of risk taking in adults with a history of high risk behavior.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2000;60179- 187
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Petry
NMBickel
WKArnett
M Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts.
Addiction 1998;93729- 738
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Rogers
RDEveritt
BJBaldacchino
ABlackshaw
AJSwainson
RWynne
KBaker
NBHunter
JCarthy
TBooker
ELondon
MDeakin
JFSahakian
BJRobbins
TW Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms.
Neuropsychopharmacology 1999;20322- 339
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Paulus
MPHozack
NFrank
LBrown
GGSchuckit
MA Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;5365- 74
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Paulus
MPHozack
NEZauscher
BEFrank
LBrown
GGBraff
DLSchuckit
MA Behavioral and functional neuroimaging evidence for prefrontal dysfunction in methamphetamine-dependent subjects.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;2653- 63
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.London
EDErnst
MGrant
SBonson
KWeinstein
A Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging.
Cereb Cortex 2000;10334- 342
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Volkow
NDFowler
JS Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex.
Cereb Cortex 2000;10318- 325
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Bolla
KIEldreth
DALondon
EDKiehl
KAMouratidis
MContoreggi
CMatochik
JAKurian
VCadet
JLKimes
ASFunderburk
FRErnst
M Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task.
Neuroimage 2003;191085- 1094
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBGibbon
MFirst
MB The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III–R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49624- 629
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Woerner
MGMannuzza
SKane
JM Anchoring the BPRS: an aid to improved reliability.
Psychopharmacol Bull 1988;24112- 117
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Young
RCBiggs
JTZiegler
VEMeyer
DA A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity.
Br J Psychiatry 1978;133429- 435
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Paulus
MP Long-range interactions in sequences of human behavior.
Physical Review E 1997;553249- 3256
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Talairach
JTournoux
P Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: a 3-Dimensional Proportional System, an Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York, NY Thieme Medical Publishers1988;
28.Cox
RW AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.
Comput Biomed Res 1996;29162- 173
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Forman
SDCohen
JDFitzgerald
MEddy
WFMintun
MANoll
DC Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold.
Magn Reson Med 1995;33636- 647
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Lancaster
JLWoldorff
MGParsons
LMLiotti
MFreitas
CSRainey
LKochunov
PVNickerson
DMikiten
SAFox
PT Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping.
Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10120- 131
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Hesselbrock
MEaston
CBucholz
KKSchuckit
MHesselbrock
V A validity study of the SSAGA—a comparison with the SCAN.
Addiction 1999;941361- 1370
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Paulus
MPHozack
NFrank
LBrown
GG Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making.
Neuroimage 2002;15836- 846
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Paulus
MPHozack
NZauscher
BMcDowell
JEFrank
LBrown
GGBraff
DL Prefrontal, parietal, and temporal cortex networks underlie decision-making in the presence of uncertainty.
Neuroimage 2001;1391- 100
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Coull
JTFrith
CDFrackowiak
RSGrasby
PM A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory.
Neuropsychologia 1996;341085- 1095
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Fink
GRHalligan
PWMarshall
JCFrith
CDFrackowiak
RSDolan
RJ Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees?
Nature 1996;382626- 628
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Downar
JCrawley
APMikulis
DJDavis
KD The effect of task relevance on the cortical response to changes in visual and auditory stimuli: an event-related fMRI study.
Neuroimage 2001;141256- 1267
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Garavan
HRoss
TJStein
EA Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;968301- 8306
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Ernst
MBolla
KMouratidis
MContoreggi
CMatochik
JAKurian
VCadet
JLKimes
ASLondon
ED Decision-making in a risk-taking task: a PET study.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2002;26682- 691
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Menon
VAdleman
NEWhite
CDGlover
GHReiss
AL Error-related brain activation during a go/no-go response inhibition task.
Hum Brain Mapp 2001;12131- 143
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Morris
JSFriston
KJBèuchel
CFrith
CDYoung
AWCalder
AJDolan
RJ A neuromodulatory role for the human amygdala in processing emotional facial expressions.
Brain 1998;12147- 57
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Phillips
MLYoung
AWScott
SKCalder
AJAndrew
CGiampietro
VWilliams
SCBullmore
ETBrammer
MGray
JA Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust.
Proc Biol Sci 1998;2651809- 1817
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Liotti
MMayberg
HSBrannan
SKMcGinnis
SJerabek
PFox
PT Differential limbic–cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;4830- 42
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Critchley
HDMathias
CJDolan
RJ Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy.
Neuron 2002;33653- 663
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Elliott
RFriston
KJDolan
RJ Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems.
J Neurosci 2000;206159- 6165
PubMedGoogle Scholar 51.Sawamoto
NHonda
MOkada
THanakawa
TKanda
MFukuyama
HKonishi
JShibasaki
H Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
J Neurosci 2000;207438- 7445
PubMedGoogle Scholar 52.Selemon
LDGoldman-Rakic
PS Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior.
J Neurosci 1988;84049- 4068
PubMedGoogle Scholar 53.Cavada
CGoldman-Rakic
PS Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey, I: parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections.
J Comp Neurol 1989;287393- 421
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Bush
GVogt
BAHolmes
JDale
AMGreve
DJenike
MARosen
BR Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002;99523- 528
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Rogers
RDRamnani
NMackay
CWilson
JLJezzard
PCarter
CSSmith
SM Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition.
Biol Psychiatry 2004;55594- 602
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Paulus
MPRogalsky
CSimmons
AFeinstein
JSStein
MB Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism.
Neuroimage 2003;191439- 1448
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Davidson
CGow
AJLee
THEllinwood
EH Methamphetamine neurotoxicity: necrotic and apoptotic mechanisms and relevance to human abuse and treatment.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2001;361- 22
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Ernst
TChang
LLeonido-Yee
MSpeck
O Evidence for long-term neurotoxicity associated with methamphetamine abuse: a 1H MRS study.
Neurology 2000;541344- 1349
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Nordahl
TESalo
RPossin
KGibson
DRFlynn
NLeamon
MGalloway
GPPfefferbaum
ASpielman
DMAdalsteinsson
ESullivan
EV Low
N-acetyl-aspartate and high choline in the anterior cingulum of recently abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects: a preliminary proton MRS study.
Psychiatry Res 2002;11643- 52
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Cadet
JLJayanthi
SDeng
X Speed kills: cellular and molecular bases of methamphetamine-induced nerve terminal degeneration and neuronal apoptosis.
FASEB J 2003;171775- 1788
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref