Context
Down syndrome (DS) is the most common genetic cause of mental retardation. However, the biological determinants of this are poorly understood. The serum sodium/myo-inositol cotransporter gene is located on chromosome 21, and myo-inositol affects neuronal survival and function. Nevertheless, few in vivo studies have examined the role of myo-inositol in DS.
Objective
To determine if people with DS have significant differences in brain myo-inositol concentration from controls and if, within people with DS, this is related to cognitive ability.
Design
A case-control study.
Setting
Outpatient.
Participants
The sample was composed of 38 adults with DS without dementia (age range, 18-66 years) and 42 healthy controls (age range, 19-66 years). The DS and control groups did not differ significantly in age, sex, ethnic origin, apolipoprotein E status, or handedness.
Main Outcome Measures
Hippocampal myo-inositol concentration and cognitive performance, as measured by the Cambridge Cognitive Examination.
Results
Hippocampal myo-inositol concentration was significantly higher in people with DS than in controls (P = .006), and within people with DS, increased myo-inositol concentration was significantly negatively correlated with overall cognitive ability (P = .04).
Conclusions
Adults with DS have a significantly increased brain concentration of myo-inositol, and this is associated with reduced cognitive ability. Future studies are required to relate myo-inositol concentration in people with DS to brain development and increased risk for developing Alzheimer disease.
Down syndrome (DS) is associated with trisomy of chromosome 21 and is the most common genetic cause of mental retardation, occurring in approximately 1 in 1000 live births. The neuropathological features of Alzheimer disease (AD) occur in virtually all individuals with DS older than 40 years,1 and the prevalence of dementia in people with DS in their 50s has been estimated at 66%.2 This combination of preexisting mental retardation with a superimposed increase in dementia is difficult to treat and is an expensive management problem. However, the neurobiological basis of mental retardation and dementia in DS is poorly understood. One possible explanation may involve abnormalities in myo-inositol metabolism because the serum sodium (Na+)/myo-inositol cotransporter gene (SLC5A3) is localized to chromosome 21,3 and myo-inositol affects neuronal development and survival, cellular osmolarity, membrane metabolism, signal transduction, protein kinase C activation,4 and amyloid deposition.5 It has therefore been suggested that increased brain myo-inositol concentration may be related to cognitive impairment in DS.6
Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) can be used to measure in vivo brain concentrations of myo-inositol, N-acetylaspartate (NAA) (a marker of neuronal density7 and/or mitochondrial function8), choline-containing compounds (Cho) (a measure of membrane synthesis/turnover8), and creatine and phosphocreatine (Cr + PCr) (a measure of cellular energy metabolism8).
There are few prior studies of people with DS using 1H-MRS. Nonetheless, those that are available offer preliminary evidence that people with DS may have an increase in brain myo-inositol concentration as compared with controls.9-11 Previous 1H-MRS studies of DS were important first steps; however, they used small samples, did not relate 1H-MRS measures to cognitive performance, or did not measure metabolite concentrations in the hippocampus. Deficits in hippocampal function may be of particular importance to DS. For example, hippocampal volume is disproportionately reduced in the DS brain12 and has been reported to be significantly positively correlated with memory ability in healthy adults with DS.13 Also, the hippocampus is particularly affected by the neuropathological features of AD in adults with DS.14 Therefore, we compared hippocampal neuronal integrity (as measured by 1H-MRS) in adults with DS and healthy controls. Also, within people with DS, we investigated whether those metabolite concentrations that differed significantly between groups were related to overall cognitive performance, as measured by the Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG).15
We studied 38 adults with DS without dementia and 42 healthy controls. People with DS were recruited from cohorts in London, Birmingham, and Newcastle upon Tyne, England. Karyotyping was used to assess DS status in all participants. Dementia status was assessed using International Statistical Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision research criteria.16 The DS and control groups did not differ significantly in age, sex, ethnic origin, apolipoprotein E status, or handedness (Table). The mean ages of the DS and healthy control groups were 35.2 years (range, 18-66 years) and 34.6 years (range, 19-66 years), respectively. Nineteen of the 38 participants with DS were older than 35 years at the time of scanning.
All participants underwent standard physical, neurological, and psychiatric screening, including routine blood tests (eg, renal and liver function tests, complete blood cell count, and thyroid function tests) and clinical magnetic resonance imaging. We excluded people with physical or psychiatric disorder affecting brain function (eg, hypertension), or a known history of birth trauma or head injury. In addition, we excluded subjects whose clinical magnetic resonance images suggested brain damage (for example, as indicated by the presence of white matter hyperintensities). None of the participants was taking psychotropic medication at the time of the study. The project was approved by the institutional review board, and after complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent, or assent, was obtained from them or their caregivers.
Participants and controls were scanned using a 1.5-T GE Signa MR system (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wis). A VacFix vacuum fixation cushion (S&S X-Ray Products Inc, Brooklyn, NY) was used to ensure that participants were both comfortable and restrained from movement during the scanning process. This device allows the cushion to be molded closely to the patient’s head, comfortably minimizing subject movement.
Differences in proportions of white and gray matter and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in the 1H-MRS volume of interest may confound group differences in metabolite concentrations. Thus, to ensure that differences in tissue composition did not account for metabolic differences between subject groups, we acquired an axial 3-dimensional inversion-recovery prepared fast spoiled gradient recalled acquisition in a steady state T1-weighted data set (repetition time, 13.8 milliseconds; inversion time, 450 milliseconds; echo time, 2.8 milliseconds; flip angle, 20°; 1 data average; image matrix, 256 × 256 × 124; and 1.5-mm sections). Acquisition time was 6.27 minutes.
In the left and right hippocampi, 1H-MRS voxels of interest (6 mL) were defined (Figure 1) using a previously published location method.17 A point-resolved spectroscopy pulse sequence (echo time, 35 milliseconds; repetition time, 1500 milliseconds; 256 data averages; 2048 points) with automated shimming and water suppression was used to obtain spectra from each voxel with a high signal-noise ratio and clearly resolved myo-inositol, NAA, Cho, and Cr + PCr peaks.18
The 1H-MRS concentrations were derived using LC model on a Sun SPARC-10 workstation (Sun Microsystems Inc, Mountain View, Calif). LC model uses a linear combination of model spectra of metabolite solutions in vitro to analyze the major resonances of in vivo spectra.19 A basis set of concentrations of alanine, aspartate, creatine, γ-aminobutyric acid, glutamine, glutamate, glycerophosphocholine, myo-inositol, lactate, NAA, N-acetyl-aspartylglutamate, scyllo-inositol, and taurine, together with a baseline function, were used for analysis. As expected, many of these metabolite peaks that were included in the LC model did not reach statistical significance when fitted. However, myo-inositol, NAA, Cr + PCr, and Cho concentrations did reach statistical significance for all spectra derived from the hippocampi, and concentrations were therefore derived from these metabolite peaks.
To ensure that differences in tissue composition did not account for metabolic differences between subject groups, the tissue composition of each 1H-MRS voxel was analyzed using SPM (Statistical Parametric Mapping) software20 to determine the percentage of gray and white matter and CSF composition.
Cognitive ability was measured using the CAMCOG.15 The CAMCOG has been validated for use with adults with DS21 and provides a measure of general cognitive function, including measures of episodic memory (which is associated with hippocampal function), orientation, language, attention, praxis, and executive function. The CAMCOG is appropriate for assessing cognitive function in people with mental retardation, unlike more standard tests of cognitive function such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales. The CAMCOG did not produce ceiling or floor effects, with the exception of a small number of CAMCOG subtests.
The British Picture Vocabulary Scale22 was used to test receptive vocabulary (which is highly correlated with full-scale IQ) to give an additional indication of overall cognitive function.
For each participant, neuropsychological testing was completed within 6 months of scanning.
Analysis of 1H-MRS metabolites was carried out using SPSS (SPSS 8.0 for Windows; SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill). Normality of distribution was assessed in both groups and tested for significance using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. Neither group violated the assumption of normality. The data were normally distributed; therefore, parametric tests of difference and correlation were used. Group differences in metabolite concentrations were tested with 1-way analysis of variance, with group as the between-subject factor. There were no significant interactions between the side from which 1H-MRS metabolites were measured (left or right hippocampus) and group or between the effect of myo-inositol on cognitive function and side. Therefore, mean hippocampal metabolite values were considered in the analysis.
We examined the relevant scatterplots and there was no evidence of nonlinearity. Therefore, Pearson correlation coefficient (2-tailed) was used to assess linear correlations between metabolite concentrations and cognitive function and metabolite and age in both groups.
The DS group had a mean mental age of 6.7 years (range, 3.2-16.10 years) as measured by the British Picture Vocabulary Scale.
There were no significant group differences in the tissue composition of the 1H-MRS voxels in mean white matter, gray matter, and CSF. Also, within the DS and control groups, there were no significant correlations between 1H-MRS measures and mean white matter, gray matter, and CSF volume of the 1H-MRS voxels.
Compared with healthy controls, individuals with DS had a significantly higher mean hippocampal concentration of myo-inositol (12.4%; F1,76 = 8.154; P = .006) (Figure 2). There were no significant between-group differences in any other 1H-MRS measure (Table). After covarying for voxel gray matter, white matter, and CSF, the significant between-group difference remained in the mean hippocampal myo-inositol concentration (F1,63 = 5.910; P = .02).
Within the DS group, we carried out a planned exploration of the relationship between overall cognitive ability (as measured by total CAMCOG score) and brain myo-inositol concentration (Figure 3). There was a significant negative correlation between mean myo-inositol concentration and overall cognitive ability (r = −0.337; P = .04). We then carried out further exploratory post hoc analyses and related different domains of the CAMCOG to myo-inositol concentration. There was a significant negative correlation between mean myo-inositol concentration and total praxis score of the CAMCOG (r = −0.396; P = .01). The negative correlation between mean myo-inositol concentration and overall cognitive ability remained significant after the CAMCOG praxis score was excluded from the data set (r = −0.329; P = .046). There was also a trend toward a negative correlation between mean myo-inositol concentration and total memory score of the CAMCOG (r = −0.297; P = .07). However, this additional correlation did not survive Bonferroni correction. Within the DS group, there were no significant correlations between CAMCOG total score and the voxel content of gray and white matter or CSF, and the correlation between myo-inositol concentration and CAMCOG total score remained significant after partialling out tissue segmentation measures.
There were no significant correlations between 1H-MRS measures and age in either group (linear or nonlinear), and there were no significant group × age interactions for any 1H-MRS measure. Within the DS group, the linear correlation (Pearson r) between mean myo-inositol concentration and age was 0.219 (P = .18), and within the healthy controls group, it was 0.172 (P = .30).
We found that adults with DS without dementia have a significantly higher hippocampal myo-inositol concentration than healthy controls, and within individuals with DS, myo-inositol concentration was significantly negatively correlated with overall cognitive performance. The significance of this correlation was not affected when potential outliers were dropped, and the result was not driven by differences in the normality of the data. We therefore suggest that a proportion of cognitive impairment in DS may be related to differences in brain myo-inositol concentration.
Brain tissue composition of 1H-MRS voxels (in terms of gray and white matter) can affect metabolite measurements. However, we found no significant between-group differences in the proportion of gray matter, white matter, or CSF contained in the 1H-MRS voxels or significant correlations between tissue segmentation measures and 1H-MRS measures. In addition, the group difference in myo-inositol concentration remained significant after correcting for tissue segmentation measures. Thus, it is unlikely that differences in voxel composition can fully account for the significant between-group differences we found in myo-inositol concentration.
Our sample of individuals with DS did not have significant white matter hyperintensities, as assessed by clinical magnetic resonance imaging. This suggests that additional brain damage (for example, associated with postoperative hypoxic-ischemic brain injury) was not a significant confound in our DS sample. Further, it is unlikely that the group difference in myo-inositol concentration (which is affected by osmotic balance) can be explained by differences in water intake or hydration because subjects with abnormal renal function or urea and electrolytes levels or hematocrit were excluded from the study.
Because of the demands of complying with the scanning procedure, our sample included few subjects with DS with a profound degree of mental retardation. Our results may therefore not be generalizable to this subgroup of individuals with DS.
The cause of the elevation in myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain, and how this affects cognitive ability, is unknown. It is possible that the (approximately) 12% increase we found in the myo-inositol concentration does not directly affect hippocampal neuronal function but simply reflects another underlying metabolic process that is more closely linked with neuronal dysfunction. For example, it has been proposed that an elevated myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain reflects a gliotic process whereby neuronal degeneration is associated with an increase in the number of glial cells.9 Astrocytes, the most common type of glial cell, have a relatively high concentration of creatine.23 However, after we covaried for concentration of Cr + PCr, the elevation in myo-inositol concentration in the DS group remained highly significant. Thus, the increase in myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain is probably not fully explained by gliosis. Further, there was no significant between-group difference in hippocampal neuronal density or mitochondrial metabolism or membrane turnover (as measured, respectively, by NAA and Cho concentrations) and/or the proportion of the hippocampal voxel occupied by gray and white matter or CSF. Thus, these factors are unlikely to pose significant confounds for our analysis. However, a significantly increased uptake of myo-inositol has been reported in cultured fibroblasts of individuals with DS.24 Thus, on the balance of probabilities, it is likely that the increase in brain myo-inositol concentration in people with DS is related to an up-regulation of myo-inositol transport associated with an extra copy of the Na+/myo-inositol cotransporter gene in trisomy 21 neurons.
One previous 1H-MRS study of individuals with DS reported an approximately 50% increase in brain myo-inositol concentration.11 The authors of that study proposed that myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain is subject to a gene-dose effect because there is also a 50% increase in the dose of the Na+/myo-inositol cotransporter gene in DS cells. Our results do not support this suggestion because we found an approximately 12% increase in myo-inositol concentration. Similarly, 2 other previous 1H-MRS studies of individuals with DS also reported increases in the brain myo-inositol concentration that were substantially less than 50%.9,10 Hence, increased brain myo-inositol concentration, associated with having a 50% increase in the transporter gene, is probably modulated by various downstream effects. For example, there may be other genes localized to chromosome 21 that also affect the phosphoinositide system (eg, synaptojanin125) and whose overexpression in DS also affects brain myo-inositol concentration.
We found that within adults with DS without dementia hippocampal myo-inositol concentration was significantly negatively correlated with overall cognitive ability, as measured by the CAMCOG total score. However, this is an observational study and we can therefore only report an association, as opposed to causation. Hypotheses concerning the potential mechanism(s) underlying a possible causal relationship must be speculative. Nevertheless, abnormally high myo-inositol concentration is likely to disrupt a number of cell processes in the brain, including osmoregulation and membrane metabolism. Myo- inositol is also a key precursor in many neurotransmitter systems, in the phosphoinositide signal transduction system, and also in neuronal calcium signaling. Calcium signaling, in turn, modulates a large number of aspects of brain development and function such as neurotransmission, learning, and memory.26 Abnormalities in calcium signaling are also associated with apoptosis, which may be increased in trisomy 21 neurons.27 Thus, variations in brain myo-inositol concentration may directly affect neuronal development, survival, and function. Also, myo-inositol may initiate a cascade of secondary changes at different levels of the signal transduction process and gene expression in the central nervous system. Thus, there are a number of potential mechanisms whereby increased myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain may be causally linked to neuronal dysfunction.
We do not propose that differences in myo-inositol concentration explain all of the cognitive impairment in DS. There are a number of other important factors, such as the quality of educational environment and individual genetic makeup.28 Further, we did not examine the neuronal integrity of other brain regions critical to higher cognitive function. This was owing to the time constraints involved in scanning people with mental retardation. Also, we only found a trend to a significant relationship between myo-inositol concentration and more specific measures of hippocampal function (eg, episodic memory scores). The results of previous 1H-MRS studies of individuals with DS suggest that increased myo-inositol concentration also occurs in other brain regions. Hence, we suggest that increased brain myo-inositol concentration may explain a proportion of the cognitive deficits in people with DS but that this is most likely not a “hippocampal-specific” effect. Rather, it is likely that there is a generalized increase in brain myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain. This is difficult to assess in people with mental retardation owing to patient compliance and time constraints. Nevertheless, future studies are required using multivoxel magnetic resonance spectroscopy approaches.
We did not include participants with dementia or AD. Thus, we can only speculate on whether increased myo-inositol concentration may predispose to the later development of dementia. Myo-inositol is amyloidogenic,5 and there are numerous reports of an association between AD in the general population and increased brain myo-inositol concentration, as measured by 1H-MRS.29 People with DS invariably develop europathological features of AD when over the age of 40 years, even in the absence of dementia, and may therefore be expected to have an age-related increase in brain myo-inositol concentration. Consistent with this expectation, Huang and colleagues11 reported that brain myo-inositol concentration in adults with DS without dementia significantly increases with age. They suggested that this age-related increase in myo-inositol concentration in people with DS (superimposed on a trait-related increase) reflects a “predementia” phase in which the neuropathological features of AD are accumulating but which precedes loss of neurons. However, we are unable to support this suggestion because we did not find that myo-inositol concentration was related to age in either the DS or the healthy control groups, despite our DS sample being larger (38 compared with 19) and having a wider age range than that studied by Huang and colleagues (18-66 years compared with 28-62 years). Nevertheless, it remains possible that significantly higher hippocampal myo-inositol concentration in adulthood predisposes individuals with DS to later development of AD, even if this does not further increase in older age.
It has been hypothesized that in the DS brain the presence of an extra copy of the amyloid precursor protein gene (which is localized to chromosome 21) leads to abnormalities in amyloid precursor protein processing in neuronal membranes and subsequently to amyloid plaques and AD.30 However, myo-inositol also promotes the formation of amyloid plaques.5 Therefore, the predisposition of people with DS to amyloid deposition and subsequent AD may arise from a significantly higher gene dose of both the amyloid precursor protein and the myo-inositol transporter genes. Nevertheless, our sample did not include people with DS and AD, and there are no studies in the world literature examining between-group differences in myo-inositol concentration in people with DS with and without AD. Thus, we cannot determine if, or how, brain myo-inositol concentration relates to AD in individuals with DS.
We found no evidence that individuals with DS without dementia have differences from controls in neuronal density and/or mitochondrial function, as measured by NAA concentration. Also, we found no evidence that individuals with DS without dementia have differences from controls in neuronal membrane turnover, as measured by Cho concentration. However, phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy would be a more suitable technique for assessing membrane metabolism than 1H-MRS. Finally, we found no evidence that individuals with DS without dementia have differences from controls in cellular energy metabolism, as measured by Cr + PCr concentration. It is possible that people with DS have differences in neuronal integrity that we were unable to detect, but our results suggest that the largest detectable contribution to cognitive impairment, from the metabolites measured using 1H-MRS, comes from increased myo-inositol concentration.
Adults with DS without dementia have a significant increase in brain myo-inositol concentration. Within individuals with DS, increased myo-inositol concentration is significantly associated with reduced overall cognitive ability. Also, increased myo-inositol concentration may predispose people with DS to the later development of AD, possibly mediated by promotion of β-amyloid plaques. There is, at present, no treatment for cognitive impairment in DS. However, the possibility that increased myo-inositol concentration in the DS brain may be associated with a greater degree of mental retardation and/or later AD suggests that trials are required to determine whether reduction in brain myo-inositol concentration improves cognitive outcome in DS.
Correspondence: Declan G. M. Murphy, MRCPsych, MD, P050, Section of Brain Maturation, Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, England (sphadgm@iop.kcl.ac.uk).
Submitted for Publication: December 15, 2004; final revision received May 25, 2005; accepted June 2, 2005.
Funding/Support: This project was generously supported by the Healthcare Foundation and the South London and Maudsley NHS Trust (National Division), London, England.
1.Wisniewski
KEWisniewski
HMWen
GY Occurrence of neuropathological changes and dementia of Alzheimer’s disease in Down’s syndrome.
Ann Neurol 1985;17278- 282
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Visser
FAldenkamp
APvan Huffelen
ACKuilman
MOverweg
Jvan Wijk
J Prospective study of the Alzheimer-type dementia in institutionalized individuals with Down syndrome.
Am J Ment Retard 1997;101400- 412
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Berry
GTMallee
JJKwon
HMRim
JSMulla
WRMuenke
MSpinner
NB The Human osmoregulatory Na+/
myo-inositol cotransporter gene (
SLC5A3): molecular cloning and localization to chromosome 21.
Genomics 1995;25507- 513
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Nishizuka
Y The molecular heterogeneity of protein kinase C and its implications for cellular regulation.
Nature 1988;334661- 665
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.McLaurin
JFranklin
TChakrabartty
AFraser
PE Phosphatidylinositol and inositol involvement in Alzheimer amyloid-beta fibril growth and arrest.
J Mol Biol 1998;278183- 194
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Galdzicki
ZSiarey
RPearce
RStoll
JRapaport
SI On the cause of mental retardation in Down syndrome: extrapolation from full and segmental trisomy 16 mouse models.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2001;35115- 145
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Miller
BL A review of chemical issues in 1H NMR spectroscopy:
N-acetyl-L-aspartate, creatine and choline.
NMR Biomed 1991;447- 52
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Bates
TStrangeward
MKeelan
JDavey
GMunro
PClark
J Inhibition of
N-acetylaspartate production: implications for 1H MRS studies in vivo.
Neuroreport 1996;71397- 1400
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Berry
GTWang
ZJDreba
SFFinucane
BMZimmerman
RA In vivo brain
myo-inositol levels in children with Down syndrome.
J Pediatr 1999;13594- 97
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Huang
WAlexander
GEDaly
EMShetty
HUKrasuski
JSRapoport
SISchapiro
MB High brain
myo-inositol levels in the predementia phase of Alzheimer’s disease in adults with Down’s syndrome: a
1H-MRS study.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;1561879- 1886
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Aylward
EHLi
QHoneycutt
NAWarren
ACPulsifer
MBBarta
PEChan
MDSmith
PDJerram
MPearlson
GD MRI volumes of the hippocampus and amygdala in adults with Down’s syndrome with and without dementia.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;156564- 568
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Krasuski
JSAlexander
GEHorowitz
BRapoport
SISchapiro
MB Relation of medial temporal volumes to age and memory function in nondemented adults with Down’s syndrome: implications for the prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s Disease.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;15974- 81
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Ball
MJNutall
K Neurofibrillary tangles and granulovacuolar degeneration and neurone loss in Down’s syndrome: quantitative comparison with Alzheimer dementia.
Ann Neurol 1980;7462- 465
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Huppert
FABrayne
CGill
CPaykel
ESBeardsall
L CAMCOG—a concise neuropsychological test to assist dementia diagnosis: socio-demographic determinants in an elderly population sample.
Br J Clin Psychol 1995;34529- 541
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.World Health Organization, The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1992;
17.Robertson
DVan Amelsvoort
TDaly
ESimmons
AWhitehead
MMorris
RGMurphy
DGM Effects of estrogen replacement therapy on human brain aging: an in vivo
1H MRS study.
Neurology 2001;572114- 2117
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Barta
PEDhingra
LRoyall
RSchwartz
E Improving stereological estimates for the volume of structures identified in three-dimensional arrays of spatial data.
J Neurosci Methods 1997;75111- 118
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Provencher
SW Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra.
Magn Reson Med 1993;30672- 679
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Hon
JHuppert
FAHolland
AJWatson
P Neuropsychological assessment of older adults with Down's syndrome: an epidemiological study using the Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG).
Br J Clin Psychol 1999;38155- 165
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Dunn
LDunn
LWhetton
CBurley
J British Picture Vocabulary Scale. 2nd ed. London, England NFER Nelson1997;
23.Urenjak
JWilliams
SRGadian
DGNoble
M Proton nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy unambiguously identifies different neural cell types.
J Neurosci 1993;13981- 989
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Fruen
BRLester
BR Down’s syndrome fibroblasts exhibit enhanced inositol uptake.
Biochem J 1990;270119- 123
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Arai
YIjuin
TTakenawa
TBecker
LETakashima
S Excessive expression of synaptojanin in brains with Down syndrome.
Brain Dev 2002;2467- 72
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Busciglio
JYanker
BA Apoptosis and increased generation of reactive oxygen species in Down’s syndrome neurons in vitro.
Nature 1995;378776- 779
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Libb
JWMyers
GJGraham
EBell
B Correlates of intelligence and adaptive behaviour in Down’s syndrome.
J Ment Defic Res 1983;27205- 210
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Firbank
MJHarrison
RMO’Brien
JT A comprehensive review of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies in dementia and Parkinson’s disease.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002;1464- 76
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Prasher
VPFarrer
MJKessling
AMFisher
EMWest
RJBarber
PCButler
AC Molecular mapping of Alzheimer-type dementia in Down’s syndrome.
Ann Neurol 1998;43380- 383
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref