Context
Limbic structures are implicated in the genesis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) by the presence of mood and cognitive disturbances in affected individuals and by elevated rates of mood disorders in family members of probands with ADHD.
Objective
To study the morphology of the hippocampus and amygdala in children with ADHD.
Design
A cross-sectional case-control study of the hippocampus and amygdala using anatomical magnetic resonance imaging.
Settings
University research institute.
Patients
One hundred fourteen individuals aged 6 to 18 years, 51 with combined-type ADHD and 63 healthy controls.
Main Outcome Measures
Volumes and measures of surface morphology for the hippocampus and amygdala.
Results
The hippocampus was larger bilaterally in the ADHD group than in the control group (t = 3.35; P<.002). Detailed surface analyses of the hippocampus further localized these differences to an enlarged head of the hippocampus in the ADHD group. Although conventional measures did not detect significant differences in amygdalar volumes, surface analyses indicated the presence of reduced size bilaterally over the area of the basolateral complex. Correlations with prefrontal measures suggested abnormal connectivity between the amygdala and prefrontal cortex in the ADHD group. Enlarged subregions of the hippocampus tended to accompany fewer symptoms.
Conclusions
The enlarged hippocampus in children and adolescents with ADHD may represent a compensatory response to the presence of disturbances in the perception of time, temporal processing (eg, delay aversion), and stimulus seeking associated with ADHD. Disrupted connections between the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex may contribute to behavioral disinhibition. Our findings suggest involvement of the limbic system in the pathophysiology of ADHD.
The neural basis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is currently unknown. Affecting 3% to 7% of all children and adolescents,1,2 ADHD is defined by distractibility, hyperactivity, and impulsivity.3 Children with ADHD often also struggle with deficits in executive functioning,4 working5 and visuospatial memory,6 temporal processing,7 and difficulty tolerating delayed rewards.8 The hippocampus likely subserves these functions in attention and cognition, disturbances of which are among the defining hallmarks of ADHD.1,5,9-13 Involvement of the amygdala in the pathophysiology of ADHD14,15 likely contributes to the increased risk for affective disorders in children with ADHD and their family members,11,16-18 even in family members who themselves do not have ADHD.19-21 Indeed, the association of affective and ADHD symptoms is sufficiently tight that affective symptoms previously were listed as associated features of ADHD in DSM-III-R.22
Replicated findings in anatomical magnetic resonance imaging studies of children with ADHD include reduced cerebral volumes23-25 and more localized reductions in volume of the prefrontal cortex (PFC),25-27 particularly its inferior aspect.28 Connectivity of these prefrontal regions, especially the ventral medial PFC, with the hippocampus and amygdala regulates a variety of attentional, memory, and emotional processes29-31 implicated in the pathophysiology of ADHD. Circuits connecting the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) support decision making32 and reward reinforcement,33 and disturbances of these circuits seem to cause behavioral disinhibition and impulsivity.34-37
We used magnetic resonance imaging to study hippocampus and amygdala morphologies in children with ADHD and age-matched healthy controls. Our a priori hypothesis was that volumes would differ across diagnostic groups.
Subjects included 114 children and adolescents aged 7 to 18 years. We recruited children who met DSM-IV criteria3 for the combined-type ADHD. Healthy controls were recruited randomly from a telemarketing list of 10 000 names, matched by zip code to subjects with ADHD. Exclusion criteria for controls included a lifetime history of ADHD, tic disorder, or obsessive-compulsive disorder or a current DSM-IV Axis I disorder. Exclusion criteria for children with ADHD included lifetime obsessive-compulsive disorder or tics or premature birth (gestation ≤36 weeks). Additional exclusion criteria for both groups included epilepsy, head trauma with loss of consciousness, lifetime substance abuse, psychotic disorder, developmental delay, or IQ less than 80, as measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–III,38 the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III,39 or the Kaufmann Brief Intelligence Test.40 Written informed consent was obtained from all parents, and participants provided written assent.
Clinical diagnoses were established using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version41 and a “best-estimate consensus procedure” that considered all available clinical and diagnostic information.42 The ADHD symptoms were further assessed by the Conners Parent and Teacher Rating scales43,44 and the DuPaul-Barkley ADHD rating scale45,46; anxiety symptoms were assessed with the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale47; and depressive symptoms, with the Children's Depression Inventory.48 Socioeconomic status was estimated using the Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status.49
Subjects were predominantly right-handed (90.2% of children with ADHD, 93.7% of controls).50 Statistical analyses included 51 children with ADHD and 63 controls of comparable age (mean [SD] age, children with ADHD, 12.3 [3.01] years; controls, 11.5 [3.04] years; t = 1.4; P = .16), socioeconomic status (mean [SD] Hollingshead index score, children with ADHD, 45.0 [13.0]; controls, 48.3 [9.9]; t = 1.5; P = .14), and IQ (mean [SD] full-scale IQ, children with ADHD, 108.3 [19.3]; controls, 114.6 [17.1]; t = −1.7; P = .08). The ADHD group contained fewer females (ADHD, 9%; controls, 21%; χ2P<.06). Thirty-five (69%) of the subjects with ADHD were taking medication: all of them were taking stimulants, 3 were taking α-agonists, and 2 were taking selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. No controls were taking psychotropic medication. In the ADHD group, 14 (27%) had a lifetime diagnosis of depression, 3 of whom were currently depressed; 14 subjects (27%) had oppositional defiant disorder in their lifetimes, 5 currently; 8 (16%) met lifetime criteria for specific developmental disorder (eg, reading, mathematics, written expression, or motor coordination); and 6 (11%) had a lifetime diagnosis of specific phobia, 2 had a current diagnosis of specific phobia.
Magnetic resonance imaging and image analysis
Head position was standardized using canthomeatal landmarks. T1-weighted, sagittal, 3-dimensional volume images were acquired using a spoiled gradient echo pulse sequence with repetition time = 24 milliseconds, echo time = 5 milliseconds, 45° flip angle, 256 × 192 matrix, 30-cm field of view, 2 excitations, section thickness = 1.2 mm, and 124 contiguous sections.
Image processing was performed on Sun Ultra 10 workstations with ANALYZE 7.5 software (Biomedical Imaging Resource, Mayo Foundation, Rochester, Minn). Operators were blind to subject characteristics and hemisphere (images were randomly flipped left to right prior to analysis). Large-scale variations in image intensity were removed,51 and images were reformatted to standardize head positioning prior to region definition.52 Axial sections were oriented parallel to both the anterior and posterior commissures, and sagittal sections were oriented parallel to standard midline landmarks.52
Methods for defining the hippocampus and the amygdala followed previously published algorithms (Figure 1).53 The rostral extent of the amygdala coincided with the most anterior section in which the anterior commissure crossed the midline. The transition between the amygdala and hippocampus was determined with a line connecting the inferior horn of the lateral ventricle with the amygdaloid sulcus or, when the sulcus was not obvious, with a straight horizontal line connecting the inferior horn of the lateral ventricle with the surface on the uncus.54 The most posterior section was the last section in which the crus of the fornix and the fimbria of the hippocampal formation could be delineated. Intraclass correlation coefficients, calculated using 2-way random effects,55 were 0.91 and 0.92 for the right and left hippocampus and 0.89 and 0.88 for the right and left amygdala, respectively.
An isointensity contour function was used in conjunction with manual editing to isolate the cerebrum. This whole brain volume (WBV) measure included gray and white matter, ventricular cerebrospinal fluid, cisterns, fissures, and cortical sulci. Cerebrospinal fluid was included using a connected components analysis. The WBV did not differ significantly between the diagnostic groups and was therefore used as a covariate in statistical analyses to control for scaling effects.56
Prefrontal regions were delineated by subdividing the cerebrum into dorsal prefrontal, inferior occipital, midtemporal, orbitofrontal, premotor, parieto-occipital, subgenual, and sensorimotor regions, as described previously.52 Additionally, corresponding gray matter volumes were defined and calculated for the different cortical regions. Volumes of gray matter in the dorsal prefrontal cortex (DPFC) and OFC were used for further analyses. Intraclass correlation coefficients were >0.98 for WBV and all cortical subdivisions.
Surface morphologies of the hippocampus and amygdala were compared across diagnostic groups while covarying statistically for age and sex to localize the portions of each structure that contributed most to the observed differences in global volume between groups. We computed the distance from each point on the surfaces of the hippocampus and amygdala of each subject to the corresponding point on the hippocampus and amygdala of a reference subject (R.B., L. H. Staib, PhD, D. Xu, PhD, H.Z., B.S.P., unpublished data, October-November 2005):
A rigid-body similarity transformation was used to register the cerebrum of each subject with that of a reference subject. The parameters of this transformation (3 translations, 3 rotations, and global scaling) were estimated with the constraint that they maximized the mutual information in gray-scale values across the 2 brains.57
These estimated parameters were used to transform the manually defined hippocampus and amygdala from each subject into this common coordinate space. Here the global-scaling parameter in the rigid registration process for the entire cerebrum, described in step 1, was applied to each hippocampus and amygdala, thereby accounting for scaling differences in these structures. These analyses therefore did not require further correction for overall brain size.
The transformed hippocampus and amygdala of each subject were individually and rigidly coregistered to the corresponding structure of the reference brain to further refine and improve their rigid-body registrations.
The hippocampus and the amygdala of each subject were warped to the hippocampus and the amygdala of a reference brain, respectively, using a high-dimensional, nonrigid warping algorithm based on fluid-flow dynamics.57,58 Structures were warped to be exactly the same size and shape as the reference structure, permitting precise identification of corresponding points on the surfaces of structures from the subject and reference brains.
The warped hippocampus and amygdala were then unwarped into the refined coordinate space identified in step 3 by simply reversing the high-dimensional, nonlinear warping used to identify point correspondences in step 4 while maintaining the labels identifying corresponding points on the surfaces of the subject and the reference structures.
Detecting, localizing, and interpreting the statistically significant differences between groups in these surface analyses could conceivably depend on the choice of the reference brain. Therefore, in the steps to determine point correspondences between structures of each brain, we first selected a reference subject who was demographically as representative as possible of the children studied. The brains for all remaining subjects were coregistered to this preliminary reference. The point correspondences on the surfaces of their hippocampus and amygdala were determined, and we computed distances between the corresponding points. We then selected as the final reference the brain for which all points across the surface of the hippocampus and amygdala were closest, in terms of least squares, to the average of the computed distances. The procedures for registration, determination of point correspondences, and calculation of distances from the final reference structure were repeated for all subjects. The distances were then compared across groups.
A Priori Hypothesis Testing
We tested our hypothesis that volumes would differ across diagnostic groups by assessing the main effect of group and the group × region interaction in a mixed-model analysis with repeated measures over a spatial domain (amygdalar and hippocampal volumes in each hemisphere). The model included the within-subjects factors “hemisphere” with 2 levels (left and right) and “region” with 2 levels (amygdala and hippocampus). Diagnosis (ADHD and control) was a between-subjects factor. Covariates included age, sex, and WBV. Beyond these independent variables, we considered all 2- and 3-way interactions of diagnosis (ADHD), sex, hemisphere, region, and age, as well as the 2-way interactions of WBV with hemisphere or region. Other variables considered in the model were handedness, socioeconomic status, medication, IQ, lifetime diagnoses of depression, oppositional defiant disorder, or specific developmental disorder (and their 2-way interaction with region); these were treated as potential confounding variables. Statistically nonsignificant terms were eliminated via backward stepwise regression, with the constraint that the model at each step had to be hierarchically well formulated (ie, all possible lower-order terms were included in the model, regardless of statistical significance).59 To control for a trend toward a sex imbalance across diagnostic groups, the procedure was repeated for boys only (n = 42 in both groups). We considered P values <.025 statistically significant, given our testing of 2 a priori hypotheses. All P values were 2-sided. Statistical procedures were performed in SAS version 9.0 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) or Statistical Product and Service Solutions (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill).60
Correlations With Symptom Severity
Associations of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes with the severity of current ADHD symptoms were assessed in the ADHD group (n = 47) while controlling for WBV, age, and sex using multiple linear regression. The correlations were restricted to the ADHD group only because of the presence of insufficient symptom variance in the control group. Correlations of the amygdala and hippocampus with anxiety and depression symptoms were assessed similarly but in both diagnostic groups.
Group Comparisons of Prefrontal Volumes
Prefrontal gray matter volumes (OFC and DPFC volumes bilaterally) were compared between children with ADHD and controls using a 2-sided t test. This comparison was not part of our a priori hypothesis testing. We report the results of this comparison merely to document the presence of anatomical abnormalities in the frontal cortices of this sample that are similar to abnormalities reported previously in other samples of individuals with ADHD.
Correlations With Gray Matter Volumes of the PFC
We explored the presumed connectivity between the hippocampal, amygdalar, and PFC subregions (DPFC and OFC gray matter volumes). Correlations were controlled for WBV, age, and sex. Differences in correlation coefficients between diagnostic groups were tested using the test statistic D for comparing 2 Pearson correlations while correcting for dfi:
where Zi is the Fisher transformation of the correlation coefficients for samples of size ni, and dfi = n−p−1, for partial correlations with P = 3 covariates (WBV, age, and sex).
The signed Euclidean distances between points on the surfaces of the amygdala and hippocampus for each subject and corresponding points on the respective reference structures were compared statistically between groups using linear regression at each voxel on the surface while covarying for age and sex. P values were color coded at each voxel and displayed across the surface of the reference structures. To minimize type I errors, a threshold of P<.0001 was set. Similar maps were constructed for P values associated with partial correlations r of surface measures with symptom severity in the ADHD group, while covarying with sex and age.
Correction for Multiple Comparisons in Surface Morphologies
Testing the null hypothesis at each point on a surface generally requires many statistical comparisons. Correction of P values for these comparisons is complicated by intercorrelations among the signed distances at neighboring points. We therefore used the theory of gaussian random fields (GRFs)61 to correct P values appropriately for these multiple comparisons in the presence of intercorrelated measures across voxels. The signed distances determine a t statistic at each corresponding point, which together across the surface compose a random field f. The expected value of the Euler characteristic of the random field f was used to approximate the critical point for determining locations on the surface where the t statistics differ between groups at a prespecified significance level or statistical threshold (R.B., L. H. Staib, PhD, D. Xu, PhD, H.Z., B.S.P., unpublished data, October-November 2005).62 Because the expected Euler characteristic was evaluated for a GRF, t statistics at each location of the brain were first converted into values from a gaussian random variable.63 Thus, surface locations where the converted statistics were larger than the estimated critical point were considered statistically significant.
The test for fixed effects in a mixed model revealed a highly significant group × region interaction (F112 = 7.96; P<.006), demonstrating a regional specificity in group differences of amygdalar and hippocampal volumes.
Post hoc assessment of the origin of this regionally specific difference between groups in volume, using a test of differences in least-square means, indicated that the hippocampus was larger bilaterally in the children with ADHD than in the controls (3384.2 mm3 vs 3164.1 mm3; t112 = 3.35; P<.002). Amygdalar volumes did not differ significantly across diagnostic groups (ADHD, 2062.6 mm3 vs 2106.0 mm3; t112 = −0.64; P = .53). Other significant covariates in the model were WBV (F111 = 39.8; P<.0001), indicating the presence of significant scaling effects, and hemisphere (F113 = 6.1; P<.02), reflecting significantly larger volumes in the right hemisphere. A group × region × hemisphere interaction was not significant (at the point of elimination, F111 = 0.3; P = .62), indicating the absence of significant lateralizing effects across groups. The group × region × age interaction also was not significant (F322 = 0.2; P = .70), indicating the stability of findings across the age range of children studied. The variables sex (F109 = 0.5; P = .48) and age (F109 = 0.7; P = .42) were conservatively retained in the final model because of the biological plausibility that these variables could influence the overall findings.
The group × region interaction remained significant (F82 = 4.37; P<.04), with a larger hippocampus in boys with ADHD compared with controls (3398.8 mm3 vs 3222.6 mm3; t82 = 2.23; P<.03).
Statistical maps revealed that global differences in hippocampal volume between groups arose mainly from enlargement of the anterior hippocampus in children with ADHD (Figure 2A), particularly over the anatomical subfields cornu ammonis (CA) and dentate gyrus (DG) (Figure 3). In posterior portions of the hippocampus, in contrast, smaller indented regions bilaterally suggested the presence of reduced volumes locally in underlying tissue in the ADHD group.
Several portions of the surface of the amygdala suggested the presence of locally reduced volumes in the ADHD group that were not evident in the more conventional measures of overall volume of this structure, with several clusters of voxels reaching P values <.0001 (Figure 2B). Differences in size were located primarily over the basal nucleus of the right amygdala and lateral nucleus of the left.
Gaussian random field–based corrections for multiple comparisons produced clusters of significant voxels that were similar in location to, but smaller in size than, clusters identified in uncorrected comparisons at a threshold of P<.0001 (Figures 2, 4, and 5).
Correlations with symptom severity
In children with ADHD only, while controlling for WBV, sex, and age, a statistical trend was detected for an inverse correlation of hippocampal volume with ratings of the severity of ADHD symptoms in the right (r = −0.29; P = .06) and left (r = −0.27; P<.07) hemispheres (Figure 6). Surface analyses also suggested that symptom severity correlated inversely with the local features of hippocampus morphology, particularly in portions that were enlarged relative to controls (Figure 4).
In children with ADHD only, volumes of the left (r = 0.3; P<.07) and right (r = 0.3; P<.06) amygdala showed strong trends toward positive correlations with hyperactivity scores (Figure 6B). Supporting the validity of these trends detected for overall volumes, analyses of symptom severity with surface features exhibited large clusters of positive correlations for hyperactivity scores bilaterally (Figure 5A). Inattention scores, in contrast, correlated inversely with surface morphology mainly in the left amygdala (Figure 5B). Symptoms of anxiety and depression did not correlate significantly with amygdalar or hippocampal volumes.
Group comparisons of prefrontal volumes
The ADHD group had significantly smaller volumes of the left OFC gray matter (ADHD, 10.535 cm3 vs controls, 11.979 cm3; t = 2.24; P<.03) and a trend toward lower mean volumes of right OFC gray matter (ADHD, 10.973 cm3 vs controls, 12.033 cm3; t = 1.68; P<.09) (Table 1). Groups did not differ in volumes of DPFC gray matter.
Correlations with volumes of pfc gray matter
Interregional correlation analyses revealed positive correlations in the control group (n = 56) for the right and left amygdala with OFC gray matter (right, r = 0.66; P<.001; left, r = 0.48; P<.001) (Table 2). None of these correlations were significant in the ADHD group (n = 47). The test statistic D for comparing 2 Pearson correlation coefficients confirmed significant group differences for these correlations for the left (P<.02) and right (P<.001) amygdala.
In separate assessments of the statistical model used for hypothesis testing, none of the possible confounds reached statistical significance: lifetime diagnosis of depression (F111 = 0.3; P = .61), oppositional defiant disorder (F111 = 0.2; P = .67), specific developmental disorder (F111 = 1.96; P = .17), full-scale IQ (F99 = 0.3; P = .62), handedness (F109 = 1.5; P = .22), socioeconomic status (F101 = 1.0; P = .31), medication status (F110 = 1.7; P = .20), and stimulant medication (F96 = 2.2; P = .14). In addition, verbal (right hippocampus, r = −0.10; P = .32; left hippocampus, r = −0.10; P = .35; right amygdala, r = 0.17; P = .11; left amygdala, r = 0.15; P = .16), performance (right hippocampus, r = −0.10; P = .35; left hippocampus, r = −0.19; P = .07; right amygdala, r = 0.15; P = .15; left amygdala, r = 0.07; P = .47), and full-scale IQs (right hippocampus, r = −0.10; P = .33; left hippocampus, r = −0.14; P = .16; right amygdala, r = 0.17; P = .09; left amygdala, r = 0.12; P = .22) did not correlate significantly with regional volumes of either the hippocampus or amygdala while controlling for WBV, sex, and age, further suggesting that IQ measures did not unduly influence findings of the primary analyses.
Children and adolescents with ADHD had larger hippocampal volumes than did healthy controls, primarily deriving from larger volumes of the head of the hippocampus. Larger volumes tended to accompany less severe ADHD symptoms. Although overall volumes of the amygdala did not differ between subjects with ADHD and controls, surface analyses showed that several amygdalar subregions were smaller in children with ADHD than in controls, and these same regions generally correlated significantly and positively with the severity of ADHD symptoms. Finally, interregional correlations suggested that connectivity between the amygdala and the OFC was disrupted in the ADHD group. Medication, comorbid illnesses of affective and anxiety disorders, symptoms of depression and anxiety, and group differences in IQ did not account for our findings.
Surface analyses revealed enlarged anterior-most portions of the hippocampus in the ADHD group, particularly in its dorsal and lateral aspects, corresponding respectively to the DG and CA1-CA2 subregions.54,64 Significant inverse correlations with hyperactivity scores were localized laterally over the CA1 and CA2 subfields, and inverse correlations with inattentive symptoms were located medially, primarily over the CA3 and DG subfields. Although we cannot infer causation from these cross-sectional, correlational findings,69,70 the most likely explanation for the association of more prominent enlargement with fewer ADHD symptoms, particularly in the presence of overall enlargement (ie, progressively fewer symptoms that accompany an increasingly more prominent morphological abnormality relative to controls), would seem to be that the hippocampal enlargement represents a compensatory plastic hypertrophic response to the presence of ADHD symptoms. This interpretation is consistent with abundant preclinical evidence for the presence of synaptic remodeling71,72 and neurogenesis73 within the hippocampus, which supports improved learning and memory functions in response to experiential demands.74,75
An enlarged anterior hippocampus could represent a localized compensatory response of neural processes to the presence of functional disturbances in these same neural systems within the anterior hippocampus, as is thought to occur in the presence of impaired neural processing.76,77 Alternatively, given evidence herein and elsewhere24,28,78,79 for the presence of impaired structure and function of the PFC in children with ADHD, the enlarged anterior portions of the hippocampus may represent a neural compensation for disturbances in prefrontal portions of a PFC-hippocampal network. The absence of a significant contribution of age to the correlations of hippocampus morphology with the severity of symptoms could evidence an initiation of a plastic response early in the course of disease, a possibility consistent with the shorter time frames (days to weeks) in which plasticity typically manifests.80
The anterior hippocampus encodes the spatial and temporal relationships between sensory experiences,81-84 which the posterior hippocampus then consolidates for storage in long-term memory.85,86 Working within a distributed network that includes the PFC, the encoding of temporal relationships within the hippocampus helps to define and encode the serial ordering of events,82,87-90 the cognitive function probably most consistently disturbed in children with ADHD.7,91-96 In humans, the anterior hippocampus plays a prominent role in indexing novelty, detecting change, and exploring new environments,86,97-99 and thus, the stimulus-seeking behaviors of children with ADHD100 may engage these anterior hippocampal functions. Given that stimulus-enriched environments101,102 and physical activity103-105 potently enhance DG neurogenesis, the anterior hippocampal hypertrophy that we detected conceivably could also be a neuronal consequence of exaggerated stimulus-seeking behaviors in the children with ADHD. Moreover, stimulus seeking and attention to nontemporal stimuli are hypothesized to serve as strategies that reduce the length of experienced time while children with ADHD are experiencing the delayed delivery of an anticipated reward,94 an experience to which they have an intense aversion.8 Thus, both stimulus seeking and its presumed morphological consequence, plastic hypertrophy in the anterior hippocampus, could help to allay disturbances in the perception of time and difficulties with delay aversion in children with ADHD.
The hippocampus is also the pacemaker for theta wave activity in the central nervous system,106 and individuals with ADHD have unusually high relative theta activity (4-8 Hz) in their electroencephalograms.107-109 Thus, an enlarged hippocampus could account for excess theta activity in this group, particularly given that theta activity underlies working memory processes and the retrieval and consolidation of long-term memories110-112 and has been documented in animals during exploratory behaviors in unfamiliar surroundings.113-115
Although overall volume of the amygdala did not differ between subjects with ADHD and controls, surface analyses indicated the presence of significant reductions in volume overlying the lateral and basal nuclei, which together with the accessory basal nucleus have been designated the basolateral complex,116 a portion of the amygdala that is particularly densely connected with the PFC.33,117,118 Hyperactivity scores showed a trend toward positive correlation with overall volume of the amygdala and should as a statistical trend be interpreted cautiously. Nevertheless, surface analyses also detected positive correlations of hyperactivity symptoms with amygdala morphology at considerably greater levels of statistical significance, particularly in the region overlying the basolateral complex bilaterally, where volumes were reduced locally in the ADHD group. Inattention scores correlated inversely with surface measures most prominently over the basal and lateral nuclei of the left amygdala. Volume reductions and correlations with measures of symptom severity were localized primarily over the basolateral complex, the portion of the amygdala most consistently implicated in the attribution of affective valence to sensory stimuli,119-121 and the nuclei most likely to subserve fear conditioning.122-125 We postulate that morphological disturbances in the basolateral complex may interfere with both the attribution of valence to sensory stimuli and the development of normal fear responses in children with ADHD, which may in turn disrupt emotional learning and the affective drive to sustain attention to otherwise mundane sensory stimuli.
Interregional connectivity
Interregional correlations suggested the presence of disturbed connectivity between the amygdala and OFC in the children with ADHD. The significant positive correlation of amygdalar volumes bilaterally with volumes of OFC gray matter in healthy controls was inverted significantly in the ADHD group. Connections between these regions are rich,126,127 and they support decision making by supplying information about positive and negative outcomes during choice behaviors.36,37 Neurons in the amygdala are thought to signal the value of specific reinforcers, information that is used subsequently by OFC neurons firing in expectation of the behavioral outcome to guide and reinforce behavior.128 Interaction of the OFC and amygdala is therefore needed to learn reinforcements and to suppress unwanted behaviors,129 as well as to evaluate the emotional and reinforcing salience of sensory stimuli.32,130-132 The poor performance of children with ADHD on delay-aversion tasks,8 their preferences for smaller immediate rewards,133 and their more frequent risk-taking behaviors134 all suggest that they are impaired in decision-making capabilities.135 More generally, learning and behavioral control depend on the integrity of limbic-prefrontal connections, and we suspect that disturbances in these connections contribute to the impulsive behaviors that are a defining hallmark of ADHD.13,136
The basolateral complex of the amygdala, in concert with the hippocampus and medial PFC, plays a central role in the consolidation of learning and memory functions, a role mediated through adrenergic, dopaminergic, and cholinergic neurotransmitter systems.137,138 Disruption of connectivity in amygdala-PFC pathways in children with ADHD is therefore consistent with some of the cognitive deficits associated with the disorder and with the cognition-enhancing effects of stimulant medications, which potentiate noradrenergic and dopaminergic transmission.139
Relation to previous studies
Two previous studies have reported normal hippocampal volumes in children and adolescents with ADHD. In both studies, 1 comprising 5726 and the other 15 boys with ADHD,27 larger hippocampal volumes were detected in the ADHD group, though not at the level of statistical significance. The statistical significance of our hippocampal findings may be attributable to a large sample size and to the use of images with higher resolution and improved signal-to-noise characteristics. Moreover, neither of the prior studies conducted detailed surface analyses of the hippocampus, which in our analyses revealed larger anterior and smaller posterior regions, effects that tend to offset one another when comparing overall volumes across diagnostic groups. These opposing effects within the same structure may explain why morphological abnormalities were difficult to detect previously.
The ultrastructural determinants of group differences in morphology of the hippocampus and amygdala are unknown, as is the extent to which disturbances in surface morphology relate to abnormalities in the underlying nuclei within these structures. Addressing these limitations will require detailed post-mortem studies. Additionally, the multiple statistical tests performed in our analyses increased the likelihood of type I error, which we minimized in our surface-based analyses through use of conservative statistical thresholds and GRF-based corrections for multiple comparisons.62,140 Voxels that did not survive GRF correction of course should be interpreted with caution. Furthermore, correlations of surface morphology with clinical symptoms were exploratory and hypothesis generating and therefore also should be interpreted cautiously, as well as confirmed in future studies. Finally, we cannot entirely discount the possibility that medications or comorbid affective and anxiety disorders contributed to our findings, although we did not detect any evidence for these effects.
Our findings of hippocampal enlargement in children with ADHD and the association of progressively fewer symptoms with an increasing degree of this morphological abnormality suggest that hippocampal enlargement may represent neural responses within the hippocampus that compensate for problems in temporal processing and delay aversion. Disturbances in connectivity between the amygdala and OFC may contribute to problems of self-regulatory control and goal-directed behaviors. This study provides further evidence that the pathophysiology of ADHD involves limbic structures and limbic-prefrontal circuits.
Correspondence: Bradley S. Peterson, MD, Columbia University and the New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1051 Riverside Dr, Unit 74, New York, NY 10032 (petersob@childpsych.columbia.edu).
Submitted for Publication: June 14, 2005; final revision received December 29, 2005; accepted January 6, 2006.
Funding/Support: This work was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health grants MHK02-74677, MH59139, and MH068318; grants from the National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders; the Thomas D. Klingenstein & Nancy D. Perlman Family Fund; the Suzanne Crosby Murphy Endowment at Columbia University; and the Center for Child and Adolescent Mental Health, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway.
Acknowledgment: We thank Xuejun Hao, PhD, and Ning Dong, MASc, for their technical assistance.
1.Swanson
JMSergeant
JATaylor
ESonuga-Barke
EJJensen
PSCantwell
DP Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic disorder.
Lancet 1998;351429- 433
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Goldman
LSGenel
MBezman
RJSlanetz
PJ Diagnosis and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association.
JAMA 1998;2791100- 1107
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
4.Sergeant
JAGeurts
HOosterlaan
J How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?
Behav Brain Res 2002;1303- 28
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Castellanos
FXTannock
R Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3617- 628
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Bedard
ACMartinussen
RIckowicz
ATannock
R Methylphenidate improves visual-spatial memory in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43260- 268
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Barkley
RA Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD.
Psychol Bull 1997;12165- 94
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Sonuga-Barke
EJTaylor
ESembi
SSmith
J Hyperactivity and delay aversion, I: the effect of delay on choice.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1992;33387- 398
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Sonuga-Barke
EJ The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2003;27593- 604
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Biederman
JSpencer
T Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a noradrenergic disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;461234- 1242
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Pliszka
SR Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2000;9525- 540, vii
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Cantwell
DPBaker
L Association between attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and learning disorders.
J Learn Disabil 1991;2488- 95
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Barkley
RA Major life activity and health outcomes associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J Clin Psychiatry 2002;63
((suppl 12))
10- 15
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Levy
F Synaptic gating and ADHD: a biological theory of comorbidity of ADHD and anxiety.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;291589- 1596
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Faraone
SVBiederman
JMick
EDoyle
AEWilens
TSpencer
TFrazier
EMullen
K A family study of psychiatric comorbidity in girls and boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2001;50586- 592
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Peterson
BSPine
DSCohen
PBrook
JS Prospective, longitudinal study of tic, obsessive-compulsive, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders in an epidemiological sample.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40685- 695
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Teicher
MHAndersen
SLPolcari
AAnderson
CMNavalta
CP Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and trauma.
Psychiatr Clin North Am 2002;25397- 426, vii-viii
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Biederman
JNewcorn
JSprich
S Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorders.
Am J Psychiatry 1991;148564- 577
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Biederman
JFaraone
SVKeenan
KBenjamin
JKrifcher
BMoore
CSprich-Buckminster
SUgaglia
KJellinek
MSSteingard
RSpencer
TNorman
DKolodny
RKraus
IPerrin
JKeller
MBTsuang
MT Further evidence for family-genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: patterns of comorbidity in probands and relatives psychiatrically and pediatrically referred samples.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49728- 738
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Braaten
EBBeiderman
JMonuteaux
MCMick
ECalhoun
ECattan
GFaraone
SV Revisiting the association between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and anxiety disorders: a familial risk analysis.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;5393- 99
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostical and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1987;
23.Rapoport
JLCastellanos
FXGogate
NJanson
KKohler
SNelson
P Imaging normal and abnormal brain development: new perspectives for child psychiatry.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2001;35272- 281
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Castellanos
FXLee
PPSharp
WJeffries
NOGreenstein
DKClasen
LSBlumenthal
JDJames
RSEbens
CLWalter
JMZijdenbos
AEvans
ACGiedd
JNRapoport
JL Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
JAMA 2002;2881740- 1748
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Castellanos
FXGiedd
JNBerquin
PCWalter
JMSharp
WTran
TVaituzis
ACBlumenthal
JDNelson
JBastain
TMZijdenbos
AEvans
ACRapoport
JL Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58289- 295
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Castellanos
FXGiedd
JNMarsh
WLHamburger
SDVaituzis
ACDickstein
DPSarfatti
SEVauss
YCSnell
JWLange
NKaysen
DKrain
ALRitchie
GFRajapakse
JCRapoport
JL Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53607- 616
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Filipek
PASemrud-Clikeman
MSteingard
RJRenshaw
PFKennedy
DNBiederman
J Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with normal controls.
Neurology 1997;48589- 601
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Sowell
ERThompson
PMWelcome
SEHenkenius
ALToga
AWPeterson
BS Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Lancet 2003;3621699- 1707
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Davidson
RJJackson
DCKalin
NH Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation: perspectives from affective neuroscience.
Psychol Bull 2000;126890- 909
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Davidson
RJPutnam
KMLarson
CL Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: a possible prelude to violence.
Science 2000;289591- 594
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Bechara
A The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage.
Brain Cogn 2004;5530- 40
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Schoenbaum
GSetlow
BRamus
SJ A systems approach to orbitofrontal cortex function: recordings in rat orbitofrontal cortex reveal interactions with different learning systems.
Behav Brain Res 2003;14619- 29
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Winstanley
CATheobald
DECardinal
RNRobbins
TW Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice.
J Neurosci 2004;244718- 4722
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Wechsler
D WISC-III Manual. Canadian Supplement. Toronto, Ontario Psychological Corp1996;
39.Wechsler
D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. New York, NY Psychological Corp1991;
40.Kaufman
ASKaufman
NL Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Manual. Circle Pines, Minn American Guidance Service1990;
41.Kaufman
JBirmaher
BBrent
DRao
UFlynn
CMoreci
PWilliamson
DRyan
N Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36980- 988
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Leckman
JFSholomskas
DThompson
WDBelanger
AWeissman
MM Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: a methodological study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;39879- 883
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Conners
CKSitarenios
GParker
JDEpstein
JN The revised Conners' Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity.
J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998;26257- 268
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Conners
CKSitarenios
GParker
JDEpstein
JN Revision and restandardization of the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity.
J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998;26279- 291
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Swanson
JMKraemer
HCHinshaw
SPArnold
LEConners
CKAbikoff
HBClevenger
WDavies
MElliott
GRGreenhill
LLHechtman
LHoza
BJensen
PSMarch
JSNewcorn
JHOwens
EBPelham
WESchiller
ESevere
JBSimpson
SVitiello
BWells
KWigal
TWu
M Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTA: success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40168- 179
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.DuPaul
GJ Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: psychometric properties in a community-based sample.
J Clin Child Psychol 1991;20245- 253
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Reynolds
CRRichmond
BO Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS). 3rd Washington, DC Western Psychological Services1985;
48.Kovac
M Children's Depression Inventory. North Tonawanda, NY Multi Health Systems1992;
49.Hollingshead
A Four-Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, Conn Yale University Press1975;
51.Sled
JGZijdenbos
APEvans
AC A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data.
IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1998;1787- 97
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Peterson
BSStaib
LScahill
LZhang
HAnderson
CLeckman
JFCohen
DJGore
JCAlbert
JWebster
R Regional brain and ventricular volumes in Tourette syndrome.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58427- 440
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Kates
WRAbrams
MTKaufmann
WEBreiter
SNReiss
AL Reliability and validity of MRI measurement of the amygdala and hippocampus in children with fragile X syndrome.
Psychiatry Res 1997;7531- 48
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Watson
CAndermann
FGloor
PJones-Gotman
MPeters
TEvans
AOlivier
AMelanson
DLeroux
G Anatomic basis of amygdaloid and hippocampal volume measurement by magnetic resonance imaging.
Neurology 1992;421743- 1750
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Shrout
PFleiss
J Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability.
Psychol Bull 1979;86420- 428
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Arndt
SCohen
GAlliger
RJSwayze
VW
IIAndreasen
NC Problems with ratio and proportion measures of imaged cerebral structures.
Psychiatry Res 1991;4079- 89
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Bansal
RStaib
LHWhiteman
RWang
YMPeterson
BS ROC-based assessments of 3D cortical surface-matching algorithms.
Neuroimage 2005;24150- 162
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Morrell
CHPearson
JDBrant
LJ Linear transformations of linear mixed-effects models.
Am Stat 1997;51338- 343
Google Scholar 60. SPSS Base 10.0 for Windows User's Guide. Chicago, Ill SPSS Inc1999;
61.Adler
RJ The Geometry of Random Fields. New York, NY J Wiley1981;
63.Worsley
KJEvans
ACMarrett
SNeelin
P A three-dimensional statistical analysis for CBF activation studies in human brain.
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1992;12900- 918
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Duvernoy
H The Human Hippocampus. New York, NY Springer Verlag2005;
65.Duvernoy
H The Human Brain Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy with MRI, and Blood Supply. New York, NY Springer1999;
66.Hanaway
JRoberts
MPWoolsey
TAGado
MHRoberts
MPJ The Brain Atlas. Weinheim, Germany Wiley-VCH Verlag GmbH2000;
67.Aggleton
JP The Amygdala, a Functional Analysis. New York, NY Oxford University Press2000;
69.Peterson
BS Conceptual, methodological, and statistical challenges in brain imaging studies of developmentally based psychopathologies.
Dev Psychopathol 2003;15811- 832
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Kraemer
HCYesavage
JATaylor
JLKupfer
D How can we learn about developmental processes from cross-sectional studies, or can we?
Am J Psychiatry 2000;157163- 171
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Bliss
TVLomo
T Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path.
J Physiol 1973;232331- 356
PubMedGoogle Scholar 72.Hebb
D The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY Wiley1949;
73.Eriksson
PSPerfilieva
EBjork-Eriksson
TAlborn
AMNordborg
CPeterson
DAGage
FH Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus.
Nat Med 1998;41313- 1317
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Gould
EBeylin
ATanapat
PReeves
AShors
TJ Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation.
Nat Neurosci 1999;2260- 265
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Gage
FHBjorklund
AStenevi
U Local regulation of compensatory noradrenergic hyperactivity in the partially denervated hippocampus.
Nature 1983;303819- 821
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Rubia
KSmith
ABBrammer
MJToone
BTaylor
E Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD.
Am J Psychiatry 2005;1621067- 1075
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 79.Durston
SHulshoff Pol
HESchnack
HGBuitelaar
JKSteenhuis
MPMinderaa
RBKahn
RSvan Engeland
H Magnetic resonance imaging of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43332- 340
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 82.Agster
KLFortin
NJEichenbaum
H The hippocampus and disambiguation of overlapping sequences.
J Neurosci 2002;225760- 5768
PubMedGoogle Scholar 83.Huerta
PTSun
LDWilson
MATonegawa
S Formation of temporal memory requires NMDA receptors within CA1 pyramidal neurons.
Neuron 2000;25473- 480
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Beiser
DGHouk
JC Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events.
J Neurophysiol 1998;793168- 3188
PubMedGoogle Scholar 89.Fortin
NJAgster
KLEichenbaum
HB Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events.
Nat Neurosci 2002;5458- 462
PubMedGoogle Scholar 90.Shapiro
MLEichenbaum
H Hippocampus as a memory map: synaptic plasticity and memory encoding by hippocampal neurons.
Hippocampus 1999;9365- 384
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 91.Barkley
RAKoplowitz
SAnderson
TMcMurray
MB Sense of time in children with ADHD: effects of duration, distraction, and stimulant medication.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1997;3359- 369
PubMedGoogle Scholar 92.Kerns
KAMcInerney
RJWilde
NJ Time reproduction, working memory, and behavioral inhibition in children with ADHD.
Child Neuropsychol 2001;721- 31
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 93.Smith
ATaylor
ERogers
JWNewman
SRubia
K Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002;43529- 542
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 94.Shaw
GBrown
G Arousal, time estimation, and time use in attention-disordered children.
Dev Neuropsychol 1999;16227- 242
Google ScholarCrossref 95.Toplak
MERucklidge
JJHetherington
RJohn
SCTannock
R Time perception deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbid reading difficulties in child and adolescent samples.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2003;44888- 903
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 96.Sonuga-Barke
EJDe Houwer
JDe Ruiter
KAjzenstzen
MHolland
S AD/HD and the capture of attention by briefly exposed delay-related cues: evidence from a conditioning paradigm.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45274- 283
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 97.Strange
BAFletcher
PCHenson
RNFriston
KJDolan
RJ Segregating the functions of human hippocampus.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;964034- 4039
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 98.Tulving
EMarkowitsch
HJCraik
FEHabib
RHoule
S Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval.
Cereb Cortex 1996;671- 79
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 100.Antrop
IRoeyers
HVan Oost
PBuysse
A Stimulation seeking and hyperactivity in children with ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41225- 231
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 102.Brown
JCooper-Kuhn
CMKempermann
GVan Praag
HWinkler
JGage
FHKuhn
HG Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis.
Eur J Neurosci 2003;172042- 2046
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 103.van Praag
HShubert
TZhao
CGage
FH Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice.
J Neurosci 2005;258680- 8685
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 104.van Praag
HKempermann
GGage
FH Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus.
Nat Neurosci 1999;2266- 270
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 105.Holmes
MMGalea
LAMistlberger
REKempermann
G Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and voluntary running activity: circadian and dose-dependent effects.
J Neurosci Res 2004;76216- 222
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 107.Willis
WGWeiler
MD Neural substrates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: electroencephalographic and magnetic resonance imaging evidence.
Dev Neuropsychol 2005;27135- 182
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 108.Barry
RJClarke
ARJohnstone
SJ A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, I: qualitative and quantitative electroencephalography.
Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114171- 183
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 109.Bresnahan
SMAnderson
JWBarry
RJ Age-related changes in quantitative EEG in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;461690- 1697
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 111.Kirk
IJMackay
JC The role of theta-range oscillations in synchronising and integrating activity in distributed mnemonic networks.
Cortex 2003;39993- 1008
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 112.Seidenbecher
TLaxmi
TRStork
OPape
HC Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval.
Science 2003;301846- 850
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 115.Green
EJMcNaughton
BLBarnes
CA Exploration-dependent modulation of evoked responses in fascia dentata: dissociation of motor, EEG, and sensory factors and evidence for a synaptic efficacy change.
J Neurosci 1990;101455- 1471
PubMedGoogle Scholar 116.Sah
PFaber
ESLopez De Armentia
MPower
J The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology.
Physiol Rev 2003;83803- 834
PubMedGoogle Scholar 117.Vazdarjanova
AMcGaugh
JL Basolateral amygdala is involved in modulating consolidation of memory for classical fear conditioning.
J Neurosci 1999;196615- 6622
PubMedGoogle Scholar 118.Setlow
BGallagher
MHolland
PC The basolateral complex of the amygdala is necessary for acquisition but not expression of CS motivational value in appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioning.
Eur J Neurosci 2002;151841- 1853
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 121.Cardinal
RNParkinson
JAHall
JEveritt
BJ Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002;26321- 352
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 122.Maren
SAharonov
GFanselow
MS Retrograde abolition of conditional fear after excitotoxic lesions in the basolateral amygdala of rats: absence of a temporal gradient.
Behav Neurosci 1996;110718- 726
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 123.LeDoux
JECicchetti
PXagoraris
ARomanski
LM The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning.
J Neurosci 1990;101062- 1069
PubMedGoogle Scholar 124.Campeau
SDavis
M Involvement of subcortical and cortical afferents to the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle in rats trained concurrently with auditory and visual conditioned stimuli.
J Neurosci 1995;152312- 2327
PubMedGoogle Scholar 125.Rosenkranz
JAGrace
AA Dopamine-mediated modulation of odour-evoked amygdala potentials during pavlovian conditioning.
Nature 2002;417282- 287
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 126.Ray
JPPrice
JL The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys.
J Comp Neurol 1993;3371- 31
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 127.Krettek
JEPrice
JL The cortical projections of the mediodorsal nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat.
J Comp Neurol 1977;171157- 191
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 128.Pickens
CLSaddoris
MPSetlow
BGallagher
MHolland
PCSchoenbaum
G Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task.
J Neurosci 2003;2311078- 11084
PubMedGoogle Scholar 129.Elliott
RDolan
RJFrith
CD Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies.
Cereb Cortex 2000;10308- 317
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 131.Schoenbaum
GChiba
AAGallagher
M Changes in functional connectivity in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during learning and reversal training.
J Neurosci 2000;205179- 5189
PubMedGoogle Scholar 132.Schoenbaum
GSetlow
BSaddoris
MPGallagher
M Encoding predicted outcome and acquired value in orbitofrontal cortex during cue sampling depends upon input from basolateral amygdala.
Neuron 2003;39855- 867
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 133.Oosterlaan
JSergeant
JA Effects of reward and response cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anxious, and normal children.
J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998;26161- 174
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 134.Farmer
JEPeterson
L Injury risk factors in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Health Psychol 1995;14325- 332
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 135.Ernst
MGrant
SJLondon
EDContoreggi
CSKimes
ASSpurgeon
L Decision making in adolescents with behavior disorders and adults with substance abuse.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;16033- 40
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 136.Rowland
ASLesesne
CAAbramowitz
AJ The epidemiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a public health view.
Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2002;8162- 170
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 137.Lalumiere
RTNguyen
LTMcGaugh
JL Post-training intrabasolateral amygdala infusions of dopamine modulate consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory: involvement of noradrenergic and cholinergic systems.
Eur J Neurosci 2004;202804- 2810
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 138.McGaugh
JL The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences.
Annu Rev Neurosci 2004;271- 28
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref