[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Temperature and mean perceived pain intensity (range, 0-100) in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects. A, Stimulus temperature–numerical rating functions plotted in linear coordinates. Vertical bars indicate standard errors. B, Stimulus temperature–numerical rating functions plotted in log-log coordinates. The slopes of these lines and the power function exponents are 4.03 for patients with PTSD and 3.07 for controls. LN indicates natural log.

Temperature and mean perceived pain intensity (range, 0-100) in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects. A, Stimulus temperature–numerical rating functions plotted in linear coordinates. Vertical bars indicate standard errors. B, Stimulus temperature–numerical rating functions plotted in log-log coordinates. The slopes of these lines and the power function exponents are 4.03 for patients with PTSD and 3.07 for controls. LN indicates natural log.

Figure 2. 
Group comparison of responses to fixed-temperature conditions. Brain activity for the fixed-temperature condition is shown in the left hippocampus and the right amygdala (A) and in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the right ventrolateral PFC (B). Each upper row displays the group differences between patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects in coronal sections (P<.001; cluster corrected with a minimum cluster size of 200 voxels, whole-brain corrected). Each lower row displays the region of interest–based random-effects analysis, with individual t values sorted by height in patients with PTSD and controls. Positive values indicate that the condition-specific predictor explained a net signal increase from the mean signal during baseline, whereas negative values explain a net signal decrease. *P<.01, 2-tailed t test. †P<.001, 2-tailed t test.

Group comparison of responses to fixed-temperature conditions. Brain activity for the fixed-temperature condition is shown in the left hippocampus and the right amygdala (A) and in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the right ventrolateral PFC (B). Each upper row displays the group differences between patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects in coronal sections (P<.001; cluster corrected with a minimum cluster size of 200 voxels, whole-brain corrected). Each lower row displays the region of interest–based random-effects analysis, with individual t values sorted by height in patients with PTSD and controls. Positive values indicate that the condition-specific predictor explained a net signal increase from the mean signal during baseline, whereas negative values explain a net signal decrease. *P<.01, 2-tailed t test. †P<.001, 2-tailed t test.

Figure 3. 
Group comparison of responses to individual temperature conditions. Brain activity for the fixed-temperature condition is shown in the right amygdala and the bilateral insula. The upper row displays the group differences between patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects in coronal and transverse sections (*P<.001; cluster corrected with a minimum cluster size of 200 voxels, whole-brain corrected). The lower row displays the region of interest–based random-effects analysis, with individual t values sorted by height in patients with PTSD and controls. Positive values indicate that the condition-specific predictor explained a net signal increase from the mean signal during baseline, whereas negative values explain a net signal decrease. †P<.01, 2-tailed t test.

Group comparison of responses to individual temperature conditions. Brain activity for the fixed-temperature condition is shown in the right amygdala and the bilateral insula. The upper row displays the group differences between patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and control subjects in coronal and transverse sections (*P<.001; cluster corrected with a minimum cluster size of 200 voxels, whole-brain corrected). The lower row displays the region of interest–based random-effects analysis, with individual t values sorted by height in patients with PTSD and controls. Positive values indicate that the condition-specific predictor explained a net signal increase from the mean signal during baseline, whereas negative values explain a net signal decrease. †P<.01, 2-tailed t test.

Table 1. Subject Demographics and Psychometric Data*
Subject Demographics and Psychometric Data*
Table 2. Main Effects of the Fixed-Temperature Condition and the Individual Temperature Condition in Veterans With PTSD and in Control Veterans Without PTSD
Main Effects of the Fixed-Temperature Condition and the Individual Temperature Condition in Veterans With PTSD and in Control Veterans Without PTSD
Table 3. Regions of Interest With Significant Group Differences for the 2 Conditions
Regions of Interest With Significant Group Differences for the 2 Conditions
1.
Treede  RDKenshalo  DRGracely  RHJones  AK The cortical representation of pain.  Pain 1999;79105- 111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Brooks  JCZambreanu  LGodinez  ACraig  ADTracey  I Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging.  Neuroimage 2005;27201- 209PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Casey  KLMinoshima  S Can pain be imaged? In: Jensen TS, Turner JA, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, eds. Proceedings of the 8th World Congress on Pain: Progress in Pain Research and Management8 Seattle, Wash IASP Press1997;855- 866
4.
Davis  KD The neural circuitry of pain as explored with functional MRI.  Neurol Res 2000;22313- 317PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Ingvar  M Pain and functional imaging.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1999;3541347- 1358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Bantick  SJWise  RGPloghaus  AClare  SSmith  SMTracey  I Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI.  Brain 2002;125310- 319PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Singer  TSeymour  BO’Doherty  JKaube  HDolan  RJFrith  CD Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.  Science 2004;3031157- 1162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Talbot  JDMarrett  SEvans  ACMeyer  EBushnell  MCDuncan  GH Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex.  Science 1991;2511355- 1358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Smith  MYEgert  JWinkel  GJacobson  J The impact of PTSD on pain experience in persons with HIV/AIDS.  Pain 2002;989- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Asmundson  GJCoons  MJTaylor  SKatz  J PTSD and the experience of pain: research and clinical implications of shared vulnerability and mutual maintenance models.  Can J Psychiatry 2002;47930- 937PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Beckham  JCCrawford  ALFeldman  MEKirby  ACHertzberg  MADavidson  JRMoore  SD Chronic posttraumatic stress disorder and chronic pain in Vietnam combat veterans.  J Psychosom Res 1997;43379- 389PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Pitman  RKvan der Kolk  BAOrr  SPGreenberg  MS Naloxone-reversible analgesic response to combat-related stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990;47541- 544PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
van der Kolk  BAGreenberg  MSOrr  SPPitman  RK Endogenous opioids, stress induced analgesia, and posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychopharmacol Bull 1989;25417- 421PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Zubieta  JKKetter  TABueller  JAXu  YKilbourn  MRYoung  EAKoeppe  RA Regulation of human affective responses by anterior cingulate and limbic μ-opioid neurotransmission.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;601145- 1153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Zubieta  JKSmith  YRBueller  JAXu  YKilbourn  MRJewett  DMMeyer  CRKoeppe  RAStohler  CS Regional μ opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain.  Science 2001;293311- 315PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Schmahl  CBohus  MEsposito  FTreede  RDDi Salle  FGreffrath  WLudaescher  PJochims  ALieb  KScheffler  KHennig  JSeifritz  E Neural correlates of antinociception in borderline personality disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63659- 667PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Blake  DDWeathers  FWNagy  LMKaloupek  DGGusman  FDCharney  DSKeane  TM The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale.  J Trauma Stress 1995;875- 90PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: SCID-I/P  New York Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute July1997;
19.
Davis  KDPope  GECrawley  APMikulis  DJ Neural correlates of prickle sensation: a percept-related fMRI study.  Nat Neurosci 2002;51121- 1122PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Koyama  YKoyama  TKroncke  APCoghill  RC Effects of stimulus duration on heat induced pain: the relationship between real-time and post-stimulus pain ratings.  Pain 2004;107256- 266PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Koyama  TMcHaffie  JGLaurienti  PJCoghill  RC The single-epoch fMRI design: validation of a simplified paradigm for the collection of subjective ratings.  Neuroimage 2003;19976- 987PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Ness  TJFillingim  RBRandich  ABackensto  EMFaught  E Low intensity vagal nerve stimulation lowers human thermal pain thresholds.  Pain 2000;8681- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Talairach  JTournoux  P Coplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging.  New York, NY Thieme Medical1988;
24.
Stiglmayr  CEBraakmann  DHaaf  BStieglitz  RBohus  M Development and characteristics of Dissociation-Tension-Scale acute (DSS-Akute) [in German]  Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 2003;53287- 294PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Boynton  GMEngel  SAGlover  GHHeeger  DJ Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1.  J Neurosci 1996;164207- 4221PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Benjamini  YHochberg  Y Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.  J R Stat Soc B 1995;57289- 300Google Scholar
27.
Genovese  CRLazar  NANichols  T Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate.  Neuroimage 2002;15870- 878PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Friston  KJHolmes  APWorsley  KJ How many subjects constitute a study?  Neuroimage 1999;101- 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Price  DDBush  FMLong  SHarkins  SW A comparison of pain measurement characteristics of mechanical visual analogue and simple numerical rating scales.  Pain 1994;56217- 226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Price  DDMcGrath  PARafii  ABuckingham  B The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain.  Pain 1983;1745- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Coghill  RCMayer  DJPrice  DD Wide dynamic range but not nociceptive-specific neurons encode multidimensional features of prolonged repetitive heat pain.  J Neurophysiol 1993;69703- 716PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Neugebauer  VLi  WBird  GCHan  JS The amygdala and persistent pain.  Neuroscientist 2004;10221- 234PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Manning  BH A lateralized deficit in morphine antinociception after unilateral inactivation of the central amygdala.  J Neurosci 1998;189453- 9470PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Simpson  JR  JrDrevets  WCSnyder  AZGusnard  DARaichle  ME Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex, II: during anticipatory anxiety.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98688- 693PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Petrovic  PCarlsson  KPetersson  KMHansson  PIngvar  M Context-dependent deactivation of the amygdala during pain.  J Cogn Neurosci 2004;161289- 1301PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Bremner  JD Neurological consequences of stress as a biological basis for a common spectrum of psychiatric disorders.  Does Stress Damage the Brain? New York, NY WW Norton Co Inc2002;32- 37Google Scholar
37.
Schmahl  CBremner  JD Neuroimaging in borderline personality disorder.  J Psychiatr Res 2006;40419- 427PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Nemeroff  CBBremner  JDFoa  EBMayberg  HSNorth  CSStein  MB Posttraumatic stress disorder: a state-of-the-science review.  J Psychiatr Res 2006;401- 21PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Golier  JAYehuda  RBierer  LMMitropoulou  VNew  ASSchmeidler  JSilverman  JMSiever  LJ The relationship of borderline personality disorder to posttraumatic stress disorder and traumatic events.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;1602018- 2024PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Hutchinson  MSchiffer  WJoseffer  SLiu  ASchlosser  RDikshit  SGoldberg  EBrodie  JD Task-specific deactivation patterns in functional magnetic resonance imaging.  Magn Reson Imaging 1999;171427- 1436PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Buckner  RLKoutstaal  WSchacter  DLRosen  BR Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming.  Brain 2000;123620- 640PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Harel  NLee  SPNagaoka  TKim  DSKim  SG Origin of negative blood oxygenation level–dependent fMRI signals.  J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2002;22908- 917PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Woolsey  TARovainen  CMCox  SBHenegar  MHLiang  GELiu  DMoskalenko  YESui  JWei  L Neuronal units linked to microvascular modules in cerebral cortex: response elements for imaging the brain.  Cereb Cortex 1996;6647- 660PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Coghill  RCTalbot  JDEvans  ACMeyer  EGjedde  ABushnell  MCDuncan  GH Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain.  J Neurosci 1994;144095- 4108PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Becerra  LRBreiter  HCStojanovic  MFishman  SEdwards  AComite  ARGonzalez  RGBorsook  D Human brain activation under controlled thermal stimulation and habituation to noxious heat: an fMRI study.  Magn Reson Med 1999;411044- 1057PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Derbyshire  SWJones  AKGyulai  FClark  STownsend  DFirestone  LL Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity.  Pain 1997;73431- 445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Peyron  RLaurent  BGarcia-Larrea  L Functional imaging of brain responses to pain: a review and meta-analysis.  Neurophysiol Clin 2000;30263- 288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Phillips  MLYoung  AWSenior  CBrammer  MAndrew  CCalder  AJBullmore  ETPerrett  DIRowland  DWilliams  SCGray  JADavid  AS A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust.  Nature 1997;389495- 498PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Craig  ADChen  KBandy  DReiman  EM Thermosensory activation of insular cortex.  Nat Neurosci 2000;3184- 190PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Craig  AD How do you feel? interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3655- 666PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Phan  KLWager  TDTaylor  SFLiberzon  I Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions.  CNS Spectr 2004;9258- 266PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Kong  JWhite  NSKwong  KKVangel  MGRosman  ISGracely  RHGollub  RL Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity.  Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27715- 721PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Peyron  RGarcia-Larrea  LGregoire  MCCostes  NConvers  PLavenne  FMauguiere  FMichel  DLaurent  B Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans: sensory and attentional networks.  Brain 1999;1221765- 1780PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Liberzon  IBritton  JCPhan  KL Neural correlates of traumatic recall in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Stress 2003;6151- 156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Williams  LMKemp  AHFelmingham  KBarton  MOlivieri  GPeduto  AGordon  EBryant  RA Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear.  Neuroimage 2006;29347- 357PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Lanius  RAWilliamson  PCHopper  JDensmore  MBoksman  KGupta  MANeufeld  RWGati  JSMenon  RS Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53204- 210PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Bremner  JDVermetten  ESchmahl  CVaccarino  VVythilingam  MAfzal  NGrillon  CCharney  DS Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse–related post-traumatic stress disorder.  Psychol Med 2005;35791- 806PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Bremner  JDVythilingam  MVermetten  ESouthwick  SMMcGlashan  TStaib  LHSoufer  RCharney  DS Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53879- 889PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Shin  LMWright  CICannistraro  PAWedig  MMMcMullin  KMartis  BMacklin  MLLasko  NBCavanagh  SRKrangel  TSOrr  SPPitman  RKWhalen  PJRauch  SL A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62273- 281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Farrow  TFHunter  MDWilkinson  IDGouneea  CFawbert  DSmith  RLee  KHMason  SSpence  SAWoodruff  PW Quantifiable change in functional brain response to empathic and forgivability judgments with resolution of posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatry Res 2005;14045- 53PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Strigo  IADuncan  GHBoivin  MBushnell  MC Differentiation of visceral and cutaneous pain in the human brain.  J Neurophysiol 2003;893294- 3303PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Geuze  EVermetten  EBremner  JD MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics, 2: findings in neuropsychiatric disorders.  Mol Psychiatry 2005;10160- 184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Neylan  TCLenoci  MRothlind  JMetzler  TJSchuff  NDu  ATFranklin  KWWeiss  DSWeiner  MWMarmar  CR Attention, learning, and memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Trauma Stress 2004;1741- 46PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Schuff  NNeylan  TCLenoci  MADu  ATWeiss  DSMarmar  CRWeiner  MW Decreased hippocampal N-acetylaspartate in the absence of atrophy in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry 2001;50952- 959PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Sakamoto  HFukuda  ROkuaki  TRogers  MKasai  KMachida  TShirouzu  IYamasue  HAkiyama  TKato  N Parahippocampal activation evoked by masked traumatic images in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study.  Neuroimage 2005;26813- 821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Delay-Goyet  PZajac  JMJavoy-Agid  FAgid  YRoques  BP Regional distribution of μ, δ and κ opioid receptors in human brains from controls and parkinsonian subjects.  Brain Res 1987;4148- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Jones  AKQi  LYFujirawa  TLuthra  SKAshburner  JBloomfield  PCunningham  VJItoh  MFukuda  HJones  T In vivo distribution of opioid receptors in man in relation to the cortical projections of the medial and lateral pain systems measured with positron emission tomography.  Neurosci Lett 1991;12625- 28PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Jones  AKWatabe  HCunningham  VJJones  T Cerebral decreases in opioid receptor binding in patients with central neuropathic pain measured by [11C]diprenorphine binding and PET.  Eur J Pain 2004;8479- 485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Pilapil  CWelner  SMagnan  JZamir  NQuirion  R μ Opioid receptor binding sites in human brain.  NIDA Res Monogr 1986;75319- 322PubMedGoogle Scholar
Original Article
January 2007

Altered Pain Processing in Veterans With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Military Psychiatry, Central Military Hospital (Drs Geuze, de Kloet, and Vermetten), and Department of Psychiatry, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience (Drs Geuze, Westenberg, and Vermetten), Utrecht, the Netherlands; and Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Central Institute of Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg (Ms Jochims and Drs Bohus and Schmahl), Mannheim, Germany.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(1):76-85. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.76
Abstract

Context  Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a chronic and debilitating anxiety disorder. Several brain areas related to pain processing are implicated in PTSD. To our knowledge, no functional imaging study has discussed whether patients with PTSD experience and process pain in a different way than control subjects.

Objective  To examine neural correlates of pain processing in patients with PTSD.

Design  The experimental procedure consisted of psychophysical assessment and neuroimaging with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Two conditions were assessed during functional magnetic resonance imaging in both experimental groups, one condition with administration of a fixed temperature of 43°C (fixed-temperature condition) and the other condition with an individual temperature for each subject but with a similar affective label equaling 40% of the subjective pain intensity (individual temperature condition).

Setting  Academic outpatient unit in a department of military psychiatry in collaboration with an imaging center at a psychiatric hospital.

Participants  Twelve male veterans with PTSD and 12 male veterans without PTSD were recruited and matched for age, region of deployment, and year of deployment.

Main Outcome Measures  Changes in functional magnetic resonance imaging blood oxygenation level–dependent response to heat stimuli, reflecting increased and decreased activity of brain areas involved in pain processing.

Results  Patients with PTSD rated temperatures in the fixed-temperature assessment as less painful compared with controls. In the fixed-temperature condition, patients with PTSD revealed increased activation in the left hippocampus and decreased activation in the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the right amygdala. In the individual temperature condition, patients with PTSD showed increased activation in the right putamen and bilateral insula, as well as decreased activity in the right precentral gyrus and the right amygdala.

Conclusions  These data provide evidence for reduced pain sensitivity in PTSD. The witnessed neural activation pattern is proposed to be related to altered pain processing in patients with PTSD.

Pain experience consists of a sensory-discriminative, an affective, and a cognitive component, which are mediated by different parts of the central nervous system. From a neuroanatomical viewpoint, the sensory-discriminative pathway of pain is localized in the lateral nociceptive system (lateral thalamic nuclei and primary and secondary somatosensory cortex). The affective component of pain is anatomically connected with the medial nociceptive system (insula, medial thalamic nuclei, and anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]), whereas the cognitive component is localized in the prefrontal cortex (PFC).1 Imaging studies2-8 using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) or positron emission tomography have confirmed the ACC, PFC, limbic cortex, insular cortex, and somatosensory cortex as part of the neural circuitry of pain.

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is an anxiety disorder that may occur in individuals exposed to a traumatic event and is characterized by chronic arousal, reexperience of the event, and avoidance of stimuli related to the event. Clinical studies9-11 report that pain experience in persons with PTSD is significantly increased compared with control subjects and that chronic pain is a commonly reported symptom of patients with PTSD. However, previous empirical research also indicates that patients with PTSD report a decrease in pain intensity ratings after being exposed to traumatic reminders.12 This has been purported to be related to opioid-mediated stress-induced analgesia.13 Activation of the μ-opioid receptor system by endogenous opioid peptides is associated with reductions in sensory and affective ratings of pain experience.14,15

Despite the increase in the numbers of neuroimaging studies of pain, no functional imaging study (to our knowledge) has explored neural correlates of pain processing in patients with PTSD. Recently, a functional neuroimaging study16 in borderline personality disorder (BPD), a related psychiatric condition, revealed an antinociceptive neural network that includes the right amygdala, rostral ACC, and PFC. The present study used the same design (fMRI in combination with painful tonic-phasic heat stimuli) to compare the brain activity in patients with PTSD and in controls. We used fixed-temperature heat stimuli that were the same for all subjects and individual temperature heat stimuli that were adjusted for equal subjective pain in all subjects. We predicted that patients with PTSD would display altered activity in brain areas related to pain processing.

Methods
Subjects

Twelve male Dutch veterans with PTSD and 12 male Dutch veterans without PTSD were recruited. Patients with PTSD were recruited from the Department of Military Psychiatry, Central Military Hospital, Utrecht, the Netherlands. Control subjects were recruited via direct mail to veterans who were registered at the Veterans Institute, Doorn, the Netherlands. All participants were veterans who had served in United Nations peacekeeping missions in Lebanon, Cambodia, and Bosnia. Control veterans were matched to the patient group for age, handedness, year of deployment, and country of deployment.

Posttraumatic stress disorder was diagnosed using DSM-IV criteria and was confirmed using the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale17 and by consensus with 3 clinicians (E.G., C.S.K., and E.V.). Only patients with Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale scores exceeding 50 were included in the study. Comorbid disorders were examined using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID).18 Control subjects were assessed using the SCID and the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. All control subjects fulfilled the DSM-IV criterion A1 (ie, they had all experienced a traumatic event). All subjects received a physical examination by a physician (C.S.K.). Subjects were excluded if they had claustrophobia, any clinical significant abnormality of a clinical laboratory test result, a history of psychiatric illness (controls only) or neurological dysfunction (all subjects), or a history of alcohol or other drug abuse (DSM-IV criteria) within 6 months before the study. None of the veterans included were physically injured at the time of deployment. All of the participants had been free of psychotropic and analgesic medication for 4 weeks before entering the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects who participated in the study after receiving a complete written and verbal description of the study. This study was approved by the institutional review boards of the University Medical Centre of Utrecht and the Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim.

Experimental procedure

The experimental procedure followed a protocol first used by Schmahl et al.16 It consisted of a psychophysical assessment performed before the imaging procedure, followed by a neuroimaging session.

Psychophysical Assessment

After a verbal introductory instruction phase, the subjective pain intensity of various painful stimuli was assessed using the Thermal Sensory Analyzer II (Medoc Advanced Medical Systems Ltd, Ramat Yishay, Israel), a device to induce thermal stimuli.19-22 Temperatures between 40°C and 48°C were applied in blocks of 30 seconds in length with 1-minute intervals on the dorsal right hand of each subject. These temperatures oscillated with an amplitude of 2°C to avoid adaptation. Five stimulus temperatures (40°C, 42°C, 44°C, 46°C, and 48°C) were applied consecutively in increasing and decreasing order. The full protocol consisted of 20 heat stimuli (5 stimuli and 4 repetitions). Subjective pain estimates were given by the participants immediately after the temperature blocks, using a numerical rating scale (NRS) ranging from 0 (no pain at all) to 100 (worst imaginable pain). These values were used to calculate the temperature equaling a subjective pain intensity of 40, using regression analysis.

Neuroimaging Using fMRI

The second part of the experiment was performed on a 1.5-T scanner (Magnetom Vision; Siemens Medical Solution, Erlangen, Germany). Functional data were assessed using a functional echo-planar imaging series (number of contiguous transversal sections, 25; section thickness, 5 mm; field of view, 220 × 220 mm2; matrix, 64 × 64 pixels; section acquisition time, 107 milliseconds; volume acquisition time, 2675 milliseconds; and repetition time, 4175 milliseconds). During acquisition of the functional data sets, 10 blocks of 30 seconds in length were applied with the temperature equaling the subjective pain intensity of 40 on the NRS (individual temperature) or with a fixed temperature of 43°C. Interspersed between these blocks were 1-minute intervals with a baseline temperature of 35°C. Subjects were asked for a pain rating on an NRS of 0 to 100 after the first poststimulation image and before the baseline image. Thermal stimuli were applied to the dorsal right hand using the Thermal Sensory Analyzer II with a thermode designed for use in the MR imaging room.

After the functional data were acquired, a high-resolution anatomical series using MP-RAGE (3-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo) with a voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 was performed. This series was used as an individual template for coregistration of functional and anatomical data and for spatial standardization into the stereotactic system of Talairach and Tournoux.23 Immediately before and after the fMRI data acquisition, a Dissociative State Questionnaire24 was given to all participants. This questionnaire consists of 20 items measuring dissociation and 1 item measuring aversive inner tension.

Data analysis

All the image data preparation and preprocessing steps, as well as the statistical analyses and the map volumetric projection, were performed using the cross-platform neuroimaging tool Brain Voyager QX 1.6 (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, the Netherlands). The first 2 images were excluded from the data analysis. Three-dimensional data preprocessing included section imaging time correction (using sinc interpolation), linear trend removal, temporal high-pass filtering to remove low-frequency nonlinear drifts of 3 cycles or fewer per time course, and 3-dimensional motion correction to detect and correct for small head movements by spatial alignment of all volumes to the first volume by rigid body transformations. Estimated translation and rotation parameters were inspected and never exceeded 3 mm. Coregistration of functional and 3-dimensional structural measurements was computed by relating T2-weighted images and the T1-weighted 3-dimensional MP-RAGE measurement, which yields a 4-dimensional functional data set. Structural 3-dimensional and functional 4-dimensional data sets were transformed into the standard space corresponding to the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux.23

The stimulation protocol was convoluted using a hemodynamic response function to account for the expected delay and generic shape of the blood oxygenation level–dependent signal.25 To correct for multiple comparisons, the false discovery rate (FDR) controlling procedure was applied on the resulting P values for all voxels. The value of q specifying the mean maximum FDR tolerated was set to 0.01. With this value, a single-voxel threshold is chosen by the FDR procedure that ensures that, from all voxels shown as active, only 1% or less are false-positive findings.26,27 Brain areas responding to the fixed and the individual temperature conditions were identified by the temperature-specific main effects in the general linear model (heat vs baseline) and were corrected for serial correlations.16 Voxel level and region of interest–level intergroup linear contrasts were computed using 2-tailed t tests. Three-dimensional statistical maps were overlaid on the Talairach-transformed Montreal Neurological Institute T1-weighted brain template (http://www.bic.mni.mcgill.ca).

Because of the sample size and the high variability of the population compared with the signal-to-noise ratio of the measurements, interindividual variance was not accounted for in the voxel-level group analysis, and all contrast maps were computed using a fixed-effects model.28 To extend our inferences and results to the clinical population, significantly activated clusters of 200 voxels in the first-level intergroup voxel-level analysis were selected for a more sensitive second-level region of interest analysis using a random-effects model in the 2-sample t tests. Data are given as mean ± SD.

Results
Psychometric data

Patients with PTSD and control veterans were matched for age (34.50 ± 6.02 and 33.26 ± 4.24 years, respectively) (Table 1). Patients with PTSD had significantly greater Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale, Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale, and Hamilton Depression scores. According to the SCID, the PTSD group met lifetime (past) DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder (n = 2), bipolar disorder (n = 2), alcohol abuse (n = 4), alcohol dependence (n = 2), substance abuse (n = 2), substance dependence (n = 1), and panic disorder without agoraphobia (n = 2). Only 1 subject with PTSD met current diagnostic criteria for panic disorder. Among the control subjects, the SCID did not reveal any current or lifetime psychiatric disorders. None of the subjects had a pain disorder or somatization disorder as determined by the SCID. In addition, none of the subjects reported the presence of current or chronic pain.

Psychophysics

When plotted in linear coordinates (Figure 1A), the mean ratings given were positively accelerating functions of heat temperature. Patients with PTSD rated the objectively equal temperatures as subjectively less painful. The fixed temperature of 43°C was rated as 10.5 ± 7.0 by patients with PTSD compared with 23.9 ± 12.7 by controls (t22 = 3.20, P<.005; 2-tailed t test). Consequently, the temperature that corresponded to an NRS rating of 40 was 46.82°C ± 1.03°C for patients with PTSD compared with 45.35°C ± 1.73°C for controls (t22 = 2.53, P<.05; 2-tailed t test). Patients with PTSD showed a significantly reduced offset of the stimulus-response function (ie, the stimulus-response function of the patients is shifted to the right with respect to the controls; y-intercept at the mean stimulus temperature of 44°C, 16.77 ± 10.07 in patients with PTSD and 32.06 ± 15.42 in controls; t22 = 2.87, P<.01; 2-tailed t test). When plotted in log-log coordinates (Figure 1B), the functions were fit by linear regression lines. Correlation coefficients (Pearson product moment correlation r) computed for these regressions of mean ratings against stimulus temperatures revealed a significant effect of temperature on pain ratings in both groups (r = 0.832, P<.05 in patients with PTSD; and r = 0.978, P<.01 in controls). The linearity of these functions in double logarithmic coordinates indicates that the relationships between the scaling responses and stimulus temperature intensity (minus the baseline temperature of 34°C) can be described by power functions as follows: f(x) = k(t − 34)x, where x is the heat pain exponent; k, a constant; and t, the stimulus temperature in degrees Celsius.29,30 Heat pain exponents were obtained by computing the slope of the regression lines (in double logarithmic coordinates), and the values were not significantly different (4.03 ± 1.37 in patients with PTSD and 3.07 ± 0.92 in controls; t22 = 1.40, P>.05). These exponents are similar to those found in other heat pain studies29,31 and indicate that the rate of change in the pain rating is not significantly different between the groups.

During imaging, the subjects were also asked to give pain ratings after each 30-second block. Again, the fixed temperature of 43°C during imaging was rated as 5.0 ± 5.5 on the NRS by patients with PTSD compared with 30.8 ± 20.9 by controls, which was significantly different (t22 = 4.13, P<.001; 2-tailed t test). During fMRI, the pain intensity for the individual temperature was rated as 39.9 ± 19.6 by patients with PTSD and as 49.9 ± 18.1 by controls (t22 = 1.31, P>.05, 2-tailed t test)

fMRI PSYCHOMETRICS

Patients with PTSD experienced significantly more aversive inner tension (2.4 ± 1.9 in patients vs 0.7 ± 0.9 in controls) before imaging (t22 = 2.86, P<.05) (score range, 0-9). After imaging, aversive inner tension was not significantly different. Patients with PTSD experienced significantly more dissociative symptoms before fMRI (10.6 ± 11.6 in patients vs 1.3 ± 3.0 in controls; t22 = 2.70, P<.05) and after fMRI (14.1 ± 14.6 in patients vs 0.6 ± 1.4 in controls; t22 = 3.19, P<.01). However, the difference between levels of dissociative symptoms after fMRI compared with levels of dissociative symptoms before imaging was not significantly different between patients with PTSD (3.5 ± 16.9) and controls (−0.7 ± 1.6) (t22 = 0.85, P>.05).

fMRI MAIN EFFECTS

In a first analysis step, activations for the main effects of the 2 conditions were investigated. Activations were thresholded at q≤0.01 (Table 2). In the patient group, solid activations were seen in the ACC, left claustrum, right putamen, left hippocampus, parietal cortex, medial PFC, dorsolateral PFC, and bilateral ventrolateral PFC in the fixed-temperature condition, as well as increased activity in the bilateral insular cortex, left and right superior temporal gyri, and a region of reduced activity in the right amygdala, bilateral precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, and right somatosensory cortex. A similar pattern of activity was seen in the individual temperature condition. In the control group in the fixed-temperature condition, significant activations were seen in the ACC, left claustrum, right amygdala, insular cortex, parietal cortex, and bilateral PFC, as well as regions of signal decrease in the left amygdala, left precuneus, right precentral and postcentral gyri, and bilateral posterior cingulate cortex. In the individual temperature condition, this pattern of activity was more pronounced. In this condition, the right medial thalamus and the left putamen were also significantly activated in controls.

fMRI GROUP ANALYSIS

In the group comparison, patients with PTSD displayed altered activity in the insula, putamen, amygdala, hippocampus, precentral gyrus, and ventrolateral PFC (Table 3). In the fixed-temperature condition, patients with PTSD revealed more activity in the left hippocampus (t<0 in 2 patients and 9 controls; t>0 in 10 patients and 3 controls; tpatients, 2.08 ± 2.21, and tcontrols, −0.91 ± 1.78; random-effects analysis, P<.001) (Figure 2A). In addition, patients with PTSD displayed less activity in the bilateral ventrolateral PFC (left ventrolateral PFC: t<0 in 8 patients and 2 controls; t>0 in 4 patients and 10 controls; tpatients, −0.96 ± 2.25, and tcontrols, 1.76 ± 2.30; random-effects analysis, P<.01; and right ventrolateral PFC: t<0 in 8 patients and 2 controls; t>0 in 4 patients and 10 controls; tpatients, −0.71 ± 2.63, and tcontrols, 1.95 ± 1.99; random-effects analysis, P<.01) (Figure 2B). Compared with controls, patients with PTSD also displayed a signal decrease in the right amygdala (t<0 in 9 patients and 2 controls; t>0 in 3 patients and 10 controls; tpatients, −1.18 ± 1.81, and tcontrols, 0.83 ± 1.54; random-effects analysis, P<.01). In the individual temperature condition, patients with PTSD revealed more activity in the right putamen (t<0 in 1 patient and 5 controls; t>0 in 11 patients and 7 controls; tpatients, 2.33 ± 1.80, and tcontrols, −0.00 ± 2.29; random-effects analysis, P<.01). A similar pattern of more activity in patients with PTSD was found in the bilateral anterior insula (left anterior insula: t<0 in 1 patient and 4 controls; t>0 in 11 patients and 8 controls; tpatients, 2.77 ± 2.15, and tcontrols, 0.72 ± 1.17; random-effects analysis, P<.01; and right anterior insula: t<0 in 2 controls; t>0 in all patients and 10 controls; tpatients, 3.80 ± 2.04, and tcontrols, 1.62 ± 1.76; random-effects analysis, P<.01) (Figure 3). In the right precentral gyrus, patients with PTSD also displayed more activity (t<0 in 2 patients and 7 controls; t>0 in 10 patients and 5 controls; tpatients, 1.40 ± 1.47, and tcontrols, −0.60 ± 1.61; random-effects analysis, P<.01). Patients with PTSD continued to display significantly less right amygdala activity in the individual temperature condition (t<0 in 9 patients and 4 controls; t>0 in 3 patients and 8 controls; tpatients, −1.37 ± 1.65, and tcontrols, 0.51 ± 1.42; random-effects analysis, P<.01).

Comment

To our knowledge, this is the first fMRI study that investigates pain processing in patients with PTSD. Patients with PTSD showed a shifted stimulus response function to heat stimuli. Before fMRI, patients with PTSD already showed a significant reduction in pain sensitivity. During imaging, patients with PTSD rated a fixed temperature as significantly less painful than control veterans. This contrasts with previous reports of increased pain sensitivity and more subjective complaints in patients with PTSD.9-11 In the fixed-temperature condition, patients with PTSD revealed increased activation in the left hippocampus and decreased activation in the bilateral ventrolateral PFC and the right amygdala compared with controls. During stimulation with an individually adjusted temperature, patients with PTSD showed increased activation in the right putamen and bilateral insula, as well as decreased activity in the right precentral gyrus and the right amygdala.

The reduced right amygdala activation is a prominent finding that was displayed by veterans with PTSD in the fixed-temperature and the individual temperature conditions. The amygdala integrates nociceptive information and plays a dual facilitatory and inhibitory role in the modulation of emotional pain behavior.32 The amygdala is an important brain structure involved in the processing of analgesia information and has been hypothesized to play a role in opioid antinociception.33 Prolonging the anticipated duration of a nociceptive stimulus also results in deactivation of the amygdala and may reflect cognitive modulation of the aversiveness of the heat stimulus.34,35 Reduced right amygdala activity was displayed by patients with BPD in an earlier study on neural correlates of pain processing in BPD by Schmahl et al.16 Both PTSD and BPD may be seen along a trauma spectrum perspective, because they show similar underlying etiology and several overlapping symptoms.36 The symptoms of BPD and PTSD tap into neural circuitry involved in emotional regulation such as the amygdala, hippocampus, and orbitofrontal cortex.37,38 Although traumatic experience is involved in both disorders, BPD involves developmental or acquired brain dysfunction, possibly associated with early traumatic experience in most patients, whereas trauma usually occurs later in life in patients with PTSD (especially in veterans).39

Negative blood oxygenation level–dependent responses should be interpreted with caution, as this may reflect a reduction or suppression of neuronal activity resulting in decreased cerebral blood flow40,41 or a hemodynamic effect in which blood is diverted or allocated to the most active areas, while adjacent areas reveal reduced blood flow.42,43 However, it was not likely in our study that this negative blood oxygenation level–dependent response was induced by a reallocation of blood flow from the posterior cingulate cortex or the amygdala to adjacent areas (there were no adjacent areas that exhibited a significantly increased cerebral blood flow); therefore, a reduction of neuronal activity in these areas (compared with baseline activity) is a more plausible explanation. Other studies16,44-46 found a decrease in these areas after application of noxious stimuli, and these signal decreases were not unexpected.

In the individual temperature condition, each subject received a stimulus that corresponded to a score of 40 on the NRS. In this condition, the affective value given to the stimulus was similar in both groups. Pain ratings that were taken during the imaging session for the individual condition were slightly higher than those before fMRI for patients with PTSD and controls. However, there were no significant differences between the affective ratings given by patients with PTSD compared with controls during fMRI, showing that both groups attributed the same affective label to stimuli in this condition. In the individual temperature condition, patients with PTSD revealed increased activity in the bilateral insula compared with controls. Activity of the insula in pain research is common and is usually related to discrimination of stimulus intensity and to emotional processing.47-51 Bilateral anterior insular cortex activation is also related to cognitive evaluation of pain intensity.52 The observed increase in activity in patients is perhaps related to a similar mechanism.

We had expected that the rostral ACC, which plays an important role in affective pain processing,6,53 would be less active in the PTSD group because patients consistently rated the fixed temperature as less painful. However, it may be that we had insufficient power to differentiate between patients and controls in this area, because in both groups the ACC was highly active compared with baseline activity. Cognitive attention given to other stimuli (eg, other sounds in the MR imaging room) by patients with PTSD could lead to reduced pain perception, while ACC activity would still be present.6

In the fixed-temperature condition (43°C), veterans with PTSD displayed reduced activation in the bilateral ventrolateral PFC and the right amygdala, as well as increased activation in the hippocampus. Reduced activation of the ventrolateral PFC is consistent with previous functional imaging research that found decreased neural activity in the PFC of patients with PTSD.54-60 Activation of prefrontal regions is usually attributed to mediation of the cognitive dimension of pain processing associated with localization and encoding of the attended stimulus.47,53 Previous research indicates that the ventrolateral PFC is activated during cutaneous painful heat stimulation.61 The reduced activity in the ventrolateral PFC found in patients with PTSD may be related to the reduced cognitive pain processing during the fixed-temperature condition. However, the reduced activity of the ventrolateral PFC is observed in the context of reports of lower pain intensity by veterans with PTSD, which may also explain this finding.

In patients with PTSD, the hippocampus (which plays an important role in the regulation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis) is structurally and functionally abnormal. Patients have shown reduced hippocampal volume and have displayed altered activity in the hippocampal area in several paradigms.58,62-65 The hippocampal role in pain processing is usually attributed to the intensity of nociceptive stimulation.6 That patients with PTSD show a net signal increase in this area during the fixed-temperature condition cannot be related to stimulus intensity, as this was the same in both groups. Other hippocampal functions such as memory encoding, memory retrieval, novelty detection, or contextual conditioning are a more probable explanation for the increased activity observed in patients with PTSD. Glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus are activated by rising glucocorticoid levels during stress to mediate fast feedback inhibition of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. It is also plausible that the increased blood oxygenation level–dependent response in the left hippocampus (which extended into the parahippocampal gyrus) witnessed in our patients reflects this activity. The net signal decrease in the left hippocampus displayed by controls has been previously reported and is thought to pertain to the affective response to pain.46,53

The fMRI results need to be interpreted in light of an altered stimulus response to heat stimuli. A mechanism that may be involved in this shift is stress-induced analgesia. Earlier studies12,13 demonstrate that patients with PTSD reveal opioid-mediated stress-induced analgesia after watching a stressful combat video. Activation of the μ-opioid receptor system by endogenous opioid peptides is associated with reductions in sensory and affective ratings of pain experience.14,15 High levels of opioid receptor binding are seen in the ACC, PFC, putamen, amygdala, caudate nucleus, and insular cortex.66-69 Several pain regions that showed an altered response in PTSD in our study, most notably the amygdala, insular cortex, and ventrolateral PFC, are involved in the endogenous opioid modulation of sensory and affective pain elements.14,15 In our study, patients with PTSD experienced significantly more aversive inner tension during the fMRI session than controls. However, whether the stress of the psychophysical examination before fMRI and the stress experienced during fMRI was sufficient to induce opioid-mediated analgesia needs to be investigated.

The finding of a signal decrease in the precentral gyrus ipsilateral to the stimulus presentation in both groups is more difficult to interpret, although this phenomenon has been observed before.53 It may be related to an inhibition of movement or to a contrast-enhancing mechanism such as anticipation or stimulus repetition.53 In addition, both groups showed reduced activity in the precuneus and posterior cingulate cortex compared with baseline in both conditions.

The pain model used in this study is phasic thermal heat pain and may not necessarily be related to clinical pain conditions. Nevertheless, it is a useful probe for the examination of pain regulation mechanisms in PTSD. Future research should extend these findings to patients with PTSD related to trauma among civilians. The use of trauma controls in this study allows us to differentiate between these groups based on the presence and absence of PTSD only. The addition of a third control group consisting of healthy subjects who have not been subjected to trauma would allow us to observe differences in pain processing related to the effect of trauma or (former) occupation (all subjects were veterans). Although the number of subjects in this study was sufficient to permit random-effects analysis and to allow generalization of these results to the clinical population, future research should include more subjects to enable subgroup analyses.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to use fMRI to differentiate pain processing in patients with PTSD compared with trauma controls. Compared with controls, veterans with PTSD revealed an analgesic response when subjected to heat stimuli. Patients with PTSD showed altered pain processing in brain areas associated with affective and cognitive pain processing, such as the insula, hippocampus, amygdala, and ventrolateral PFC. We propose that the neural pattern with decreased activity in the right amygdala and the bilateral ventrolateral PFC reflects altered pain regulation mechanisms in patients with PTSD.

Correspondence: Elbert Geuze, PhD, Department of Military Psychiatry T2, Central Military Hospital, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX Utrecht, the Netherlands (s.g.geuze@umcutrecht.nl).

Submitted for Publication: February 23, 2006; final revision received April 25, 2006; accepted April 25, 2006.

Author Contributions: Drs Vermetten and Schmahl contributed equally to this work.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Dutch Ministry of Defense.

Acknowledgment: We thank Arthur Rademaker, MSc, for clinical assessments; Valerio Francati, MSc, and Christian Vrijlandt for help with data acquisition; Matthias Ruf, MD, for technical assistance; and Wolfgang Greffrath, PhD, for statistical advice in the psychophysical assessments.

References
1.
Treede  RDKenshalo  DRGracely  RHJones  AK The cortical representation of pain.  Pain 1999;79105- 111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Brooks  JCZambreanu  LGodinez  ACraig  ADTracey  I Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging.  Neuroimage 2005;27201- 209PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Casey  KLMinoshima  S Can pain be imaged? In: Jensen TS, Turner JA, Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z, eds. Proceedings of the 8th World Congress on Pain: Progress in Pain Research and Management8 Seattle, Wash IASP Press1997;855- 866
4.
Davis  KD The neural circuitry of pain as explored with functional MRI.  Neurol Res 2000;22313- 317PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Ingvar  M Pain and functional imaging.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1999;3541347- 1358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Bantick  SJWise  RGPloghaus  AClare  SSmith  SMTracey  I Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI.  Brain 2002;125310- 319PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Singer  TSeymour  BO’Doherty  JKaube  HDolan  RJFrith  CD Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.  Science 2004;3031157- 1162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Talbot  JDMarrett  SEvans  ACMeyer  EBushnell  MCDuncan  GH Multiple representations of pain in human cerebral cortex.  Science 1991;2511355- 1358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Smith  MYEgert  JWinkel  GJacobson  J The impact of PTSD on pain experience in persons with HIV/AIDS.  Pain 2002;989- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Asmundson  GJCoons  MJTaylor  SKatz  J PTSD and the experience of pain: research and clinical implications of shared vulnerability and mutual maintenance models.  Can J Psychiatry 2002;47930- 937PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Beckham  JCCrawford  ALFeldman  MEKirby  ACHertzberg  MADavidson  JRMoore  SD Chronic posttraumatic stress disorder and chronic pain in Vietnam combat veterans.  J Psychosom Res 1997;43379- 389PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Pitman  RKvan der Kolk  BAOrr  SPGreenberg  MS Naloxone-reversible analgesic response to combat-related stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot study.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990;47541- 544PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
van der Kolk  BAGreenberg  MSOrr  SPPitman  RK Endogenous opioids, stress induced analgesia, and posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychopharmacol Bull 1989;25417- 421PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Zubieta  JKKetter  TABueller  JAXu  YKilbourn  MRYoung  EAKoeppe  RA Regulation of human affective responses by anterior cingulate and limbic μ-opioid neurotransmission.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;601145- 1153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Zubieta  JKSmith  YRBueller  JAXu  YKilbourn  MRJewett  DMMeyer  CRKoeppe  RAStohler  CS Regional μ opioid receptor regulation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain.  Science 2001;293311- 315PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Schmahl  CBohus  MEsposito  FTreede  RDDi Salle  FGreffrath  WLudaescher  PJochims  ALieb  KScheffler  KHennig  JSeifritz  E Neural correlates of antinociception in borderline personality disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63659- 667PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Blake  DDWeathers  FWNagy  LMKaloupek  DGGusman  FDCharney  DSKeane  TM The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale.  J Trauma Stress 1995;875- 90PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: SCID-I/P  New York Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute July1997;
19.
Davis  KDPope  GECrawley  APMikulis  DJ Neural correlates of prickle sensation: a percept-related fMRI study.  Nat Neurosci 2002;51121- 1122PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Koyama  YKoyama  TKroncke  APCoghill  RC Effects of stimulus duration on heat induced pain: the relationship between real-time and post-stimulus pain ratings.  Pain 2004;107256- 266PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Koyama  TMcHaffie  JGLaurienti  PJCoghill  RC The single-epoch fMRI design: validation of a simplified paradigm for the collection of subjective ratings.  Neuroimage 2003;19976- 987PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Ness  TJFillingim  RBRandich  ABackensto  EMFaught  E Low intensity vagal nerve stimulation lowers human thermal pain thresholds.  Pain 2000;8681- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Talairach  JTournoux  P Coplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging.  New York, NY Thieme Medical1988;
24.
Stiglmayr  CEBraakmann  DHaaf  BStieglitz  RBohus  M Development and characteristics of Dissociation-Tension-Scale acute (DSS-Akute) [in German]  Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 2003;53287- 294PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Boynton  GMEngel  SAGlover  GHHeeger  DJ Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1.  J Neurosci 1996;164207- 4221PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Benjamini  YHochberg  Y Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.  J R Stat Soc B 1995;57289- 300Google Scholar
27.
Genovese  CRLazar  NANichols  T Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate.  Neuroimage 2002;15870- 878PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Friston  KJHolmes  APWorsley  KJ How many subjects constitute a study?  Neuroimage 1999;101- 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Price  DDBush  FMLong  SHarkins  SW A comparison of pain measurement characteristics of mechanical visual analogue and simple numerical rating scales.  Pain 1994;56217- 226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Price  DDMcGrath  PARafii  ABuckingham  B The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain.  Pain 1983;1745- 56PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Coghill  RCMayer  DJPrice  DD Wide dynamic range but not nociceptive-specific neurons encode multidimensional features of prolonged repetitive heat pain.  J Neurophysiol 1993;69703- 716PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Neugebauer  VLi  WBird  GCHan  JS The amygdala and persistent pain.  Neuroscientist 2004;10221- 234PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Manning  BH A lateralized deficit in morphine antinociception after unilateral inactivation of the central amygdala.  J Neurosci 1998;189453- 9470PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Simpson  JR  JrDrevets  WCSnyder  AZGusnard  DARaichle  ME Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex, II: during anticipatory anxiety.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98688- 693PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Petrovic  PCarlsson  KPetersson  KMHansson  PIngvar  M Context-dependent deactivation of the amygdala during pain.  J Cogn Neurosci 2004;161289- 1301PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Bremner  JD Neurological consequences of stress as a biological basis for a common spectrum of psychiatric disorders.  Does Stress Damage the Brain? New York, NY WW Norton Co Inc2002;32- 37Google Scholar
37.
Schmahl  CBremner  JD Neuroimaging in borderline personality disorder.  J Psychiatr Res 2006;40419- 427PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Nemeroff  CBBremner  JDFoa  EBMayberg  HSNorth  CSStein  MB Posttraumatic stress disorder: a state-of-the-science review.  J Psychiatr Res 2006;401- 21PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Golier  JAYehuda  RBierer  LMMitropoulou  VNew  ASSchmeidler  JSilverman  JMSiever  LJ The relationship of borderline personality disorder to posttraumatic stress disorder and traumatic events.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;1602018- 2024PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Hutchinson  MSchiffer  WJoseffer  SLiu  ASchlosser  RDikshit  SGoldberg  EBrodie  JD Task-specific deactivation patterns in functional magnetic resonance imaging.  Magn Reson Imaging 1999;171427- 1436PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Buckner  RLKoutstaal  WSchacter  DLRosen  BR Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming.  Brain 2000;123620- 640PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Harel  NLee  SPNagaoka  TKim  DSKim  SG Origin of negative blood oxygenation level–dependent fMRI signals.  J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2002;22908- 917PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Woolsey  TARovainen  CMCox  SBHenegar  MHLiang  GELiu  DMoskalenko  YESui  JWei  L Neuronal units linked to microvascular modules in cerebral cortex: response elements for imaging the brain.  Cereb Cortex 1996;6647- 660PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Coghill  RCTalbot  JDEvans  ACMeyer  EGjedde  ABushnell  MCDuncan  GH Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain.  J Neurosci 1994;144095- 4108PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Becerra  LRBreiter  HCStojanovic  MFishman  SEdwards  AComite  ARGonzalez  RGBorsook  D Human brain activation under controlled thermal stimulation and habituation to noxious heat: an fMRI study.  Magn Reson Med 1999;411044- 1057PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Derbyshire  SWJones  AKGyulai  FClark  STownsend  DFirestone  LL Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity.  Pain 1997;73431- 445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Peyron  RLaurent  BGarcia-Larrea  L Functional imaging of brain responses to pain: a review and meta-analysis.  Neurophysiol Clin 2000;30263- 288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Phillips  MLYoung  AWSenior  CBrammer  MAndrew  CCalder  AJBullmore  ETPerrett  DIRowland  DWilliams  SCGray  JADavid  AS A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust.  Nature 1997;389495- 498PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Craig  ADChen  KBandy  DReiman  EM Thermosensory activation of insular cortex.  Nat Neurosci 2000;3184- 190PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Craig  AD How do you feel? interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3655- 666PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Phan  KLWager  TDTaylor  SFLiberzon  I Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions.  CNS Spectr 2004;9258- 266PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Kong  JWhite  NSKwong  KKVangel  MGRosman  ISGracely  RHGollub  RL Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity.  Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27715- 721PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Peyron  RGarcia-Larrea  LGregoire  MCCostes  NConvers  PLavenne  FMauguiere  FMichel  DLaurent  B Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans: sensory and attentional networks.  Brain 1999;1221765- 1780PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Liberzon  IBritton  JCPhan  KL Neural correlates of traumatic recall in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Stress 2003;6151- 156PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Williams  LMKemp  AHFelmingham  KBarton  MOlivieri  GPeduto  AGordon  EBryant  RA Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear.  Neuroimage 2006;29347- 357PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Lanius  RAWilliamson  PCHopper  JDensmore  MBoksman  KGupta  MANeufeld  RWGati  JSMenon  RS Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53204- 210PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Bremner  JDVermetten  ESchmahl  CVaccarino  VVythilingam  MAfzal  NGrillon  CCharney  DS Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse–related post-traumatic stress disorder.  Psychol Med 2005;35791- 806PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Bremner  JDVythilingam  MVermetten  ESouthwick  SMMcGlashan  TStaib  LHSoufer  RCharney  DS Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53879- 889PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Shin  LMWright  CICannistraro  PAWedig  MMMcMullin  KMartis  BMacklin  MLLasko  NBCavanagh  SRKrangel  TSOrr  SPPitman  RKWhalen  PJRauch  SL A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62273- 281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Farrow  TFHunter  MDWilkinson  IDGouneea  CFawbert  DSmith  RLee  KHMason  SSpence  SAWoodruff  PW Quantifiable change in functional brain response to empathic and forgivability judgments with resolution of posttraumatic stress disorder.  Psychiatry Res 2005;14045- 53PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Strigo  IADuncan  GHBoivin  MBushnell  MC Differentiation of visceral and cutaneous pain in the human brain.  J Neurophysiol 2003;893294- 3303PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Geuze  EVermetten  EBremner  JD MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics, 2: findings in neuropsychiatric disorders.  Mol Psychiatry 2005;10160- 184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Neylan  TCLenoci  MRothlind  JMetzler  TJSchuff  NDu  ATFranklin  KWWeiss  DSWeiner  MWMarmar  CR Attention, learning, and memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Trauma Stress 2004;1741- 46PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Schuff  NNeylan  TCLenoci  MADu  ATWeiss  DSMarmar  CRWeiner  MW Decreased hippocampal N-acetylaspartate in the absence of atrophy in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biol Psychiatry 2001;50952- 959PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Sakamoto  HFukuda  ROkuaki  TRogers  MKasai  KMachida  TShirouzu  IYamasue  HAkiyama  TKato  N Parahippocampal activation evoked by masked traumatic images in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study.  Neuroimage 2005;26813- 821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Delay-Goyet  PZajac  JMJavoy-Agid  FAgid  YRoques  BP Regional distribution of μ, δ and κ opioid receptors in human brains from controls and parkinsonian subjects.  Brain Res 1987;4148- 14PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Jones  AKQi  LYFujirawa  TLuthra  SKAshburner  JBloomfield  PCunningham  VJItoh  MFukuda  HJones  T In vivo distribution of opioid receptors in man in relation to the cortical projections of the medial and lateral pain systems measured with positron emission tomography.  Neurosci Lett 1991;12625- 28PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Jones  AKWatabe  HCunningham  VJJones  T Cerebral decreases in opioid receptor binding in patients with central neuropathic pain measured by [11C]diprenorphine binding and PET.  Eur J Pain 2004;8479- 485PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Pilapil  CWelner  SMagnan  JZamir  NQuirion  R μ Opioid receptor binding sites in human brain.  NIDA Res Monogr 1986;75319- 322PubMedGoogle Scholar
×