[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Differences in verbal IQ associated with NOS1 rs6490121 genotype in the Irish and German cases and controls. In addition to a main effect of genotype in both samples, a genotype × diagnosis interaction was observed in the German samples such that the effect of NOS1 was greater in cases than in controls.

Differences in verbal IQ associated with NOS1 rs6490121 genotype in the Irish and German cases and controls. In addition to a main effect of genotype in both samples, a genotype × diagnosis interaction was observed in the German samples such that the effect of NOS1 was greater in cases than in controls.

Figure 2. 
Differences in working memory associated with NOS1 rs6490121 genotype assessed by the letter-number sequencing task from the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition, in the Irish samples (A) and by the composite digit span and spatial span score from the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised in the German samples (B). In addition to a main effect of NOS1 genotype, an interaction effect was observed in both groups with NOS1 showing a larger effect in controls in the Irish samples and a larger effect in cases in the German samples.

Differences in working memory associated with NOS1 rs6490121 genotype assessed by the letter-number sequencing task from the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition,53 in the Irish samples (A) and by the composite digit span and spatial span score from the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised54,55 in the German samples (B). In addition to a main effect of NOS1 genotype, an interaction effect was observed in both groups with NOS1 showing a larger effect in controls in the Irish samples and a larger effect in cases in the German samples.

Table 1. 
Demographic and Clinical Data for Irish and German Samples
Demographic and Clinical Data for Irish and German Samples
Table 2. 
Cognitive Performance According to NOS1 Genotype in Both Discovery and Replication Samples
Cognitive Performance According to NOS1 Genotype in Both Discovery and Replication Samples
1.
Cardno  AGGottesman  II Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics.  Am J Med Genet 2000;97 (1) 12- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Sullivan  PF Schizophrenia genetics: the search for a hard lead.  Curr Opin Psychiatry 2008;21 (2) 157- 160PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
International Schizophrenia Consortium, Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia.  Nature 2008;455 (7210) 237- 241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
O'Donovan  MCCraddock  NNorton  NWilliams  HPeirce  TMoskvina  VNikolov  IHamshere  MCarroll  LGeorgieva  LDwyer  SHolmans  PMarchini  JLSpencer  CCHowie  BLeung  HTHartmann  AMMöller  HJMorris  DWShi  YFeng  GHoffmann  PPropping  PVasilescu  CMaier  WRietschel  MZammit  SSchumacher  JQuinn  EMSchulze  TGWilliams  NMGiegling  IIwata  NIkeda  MDarvasi  AShifman  SHe  LDuan  JSanders  ARLevinson  DFGejman  PVCichon  SNöthen  MMGill  MCorvin  ARujescu  DKirov  GOwen  MJBuccola  NGMowry  BJFreedman  RAmin  FBlack  DWSilverman  JMByerley  WFCloninger  CRMolecular Genetics of Schizophrenia Collaboration, Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up.  Nat Genet 2008;40 (9) 1053- 1055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Stefansson  HRujescu  DCichon  SPietiläinen  OPIngason  ASteinberg  SFossdal  RSigurdsson  ESigmundsson  TBuizer-Voskamp  JEHansen  TJakobsen  KDMuglia  PFrancks  CMatthews  PMGylfason  AHalldorsson  BVGudbjartsson  DThorgeirsson  TESigurdsson  AJonasdottir  AJonasdottir  ABjornsson  AMattiasdottir  SBlondal  THaraldsson  MMagnusdottir  BBGiegling  IMöller  HJHartmann  AShianna  KVGe  DNeed  ACCrombie  CFraser  GWalker  NLonnqvist  JSuvisaari  JTuulio-Henriksson  APaunio  TToulopoulou  TBramon  EDi Forti  MMurray  RRuggeri  MVassos  ETosato  SWalshe  MLi  TVasilescu  CMühleisen  TWWang  AGUllum  HDjurovic  SMelle  IOlesen  JKiemeney  LAFranke  BSabatti  CFreimer  NBGulcher  JRThorsteinsdottir  UKong  AAndreassen  OAOphoff  RAGeorgi  ARietschel  MWerge  TPetursson  HGoldstein  DBNöthen  MMPeltonen  LCollier  DASt Clair  DStefansson  KGROUP, Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia.  Nature 2008;455 (7210) 232- 236PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Fanous  AHKendler  KS Genetics of clinical features and subtypes of schizophrenia: a review of the recent literature.  Curr Psychiatry Rep 2008;10 (2) 164- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Mefford  HCSharp  AJBaker  CItsara  AJiang  ZBuysse  KHuang  SMaloney  VKCrolla  JABaralle  DCollins  AMercer  CNorga  Kde Ravel  TDevriendt  KBongers  EMde Leeuw  NReardon  WGimelli  SBena  FHennekam  RCMale  AGaunt  LClayton-Smith  JSimonic  IPark  SMMehta  SGNik-Zainal  SWoods  CGFirth  HVParkin  GFichera  MReitano  SGiudice  MLLi  KECasuga  IBroomer  AConrad  BSchwerzmann  MRäber  LGallati  SStriano  PCoppola  ATolmie  JLTobias  ESLilley  CArmengol  LSpysschaert  YVerloo  PDe Coene  AGoossens  LMortier  GSpeleman  Fvan Binsbergen  ENelen  MRHochstenbach  RPoot  MGallagher  LGill  M McClellan  JKing  MCRegan  RSkinner  CStevenson  REAntonarakis  SEChen  CEstivill  XMenten  BGimelli  GGribble  SSchwartz  SSutcliffe  JSWalsh  TKnight  SJSebat  JRomano  CSchwartz  CEVeltman  JAde Vries  BBVermeesch  JRBarber  JCWillatt  LTassabehji  MEichler  EE Recurrent rearrangements of chromosome 1q21.1 and variable pediatric phenotypes.  N Engl J Med 2008;359 (16) 1685- 1699PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Abkevich  VCamp  NJHensel  CHNeff  CDRussell  DLHughes  DCPlenk  AMLowry  MRRichards  RLCarter  CFrech  GCStone  SRowe  KChau  CACortado  KHunt  ALuce  KO'Neil  GPoarch  JPotter  JPoulsen  GHSaxton  HBernat-Sestak  MThompson  VGutin  ASkolnick  MHShattuck  DCannon-Albright  L Predisposition locus for major depression at chromosome 12q22-12q23.2.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;73 (6) 1271- 1281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Bailer  ULeisch  FMeszaros  KLenzinger  EWillinger  UStrobl  RGebhardt  CGerhard  EFuchs  KSieghart  WKasper  SHornik  KAschauer  HN Genome scan for susceptibility loci for schizophrenia.  Neuropsychobiology 2000;42 (4) 175- 182PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Bailer  ULeisch  FMeszaros  KLenzinger  EWillinger  UStrobl  RHeiden  A Gebhardt  CDöge  EFuchs  KSieghart  WKasper  SHornik  KAschauer  HN Genome scan for susceptibility loci for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.   Biol Psychiatry 2002;52 (1) 40- 52PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
DeLisi  LEShaw  SHCrow  TJShields  GSmith  ABLarach  VWWellman  NLoftus  JNanthakumar  BRazi  KStewart  JComazzi  MVita  AHeffner  TSherrington  R A genome-wide scan for linkage to chromosomal regions in 382 sibling pairs with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder.  Am J Psychiatry 2002;159 (5) 803- 812PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Shinkai  TOhmori  OHori  HNakamura  J Allelic association of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS1) gene with schizophrenia.  Mol Psychiatry 2002;7 (6) 560- 563PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Reif  AHerterich  SStrobel  AEhlis  ACSaur  DJacob  CPWienker  TTöpner  TFritzen  SWalter  USchmitt  AFallgatter  AJLesch  KP A neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) haplotype associated with schizophrenia modifies prefrontal cortex function.  Mol Psychiatry 2006;11 (3) 286- 300PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Fallin  MDLasseter  VKAvramopoulos  DNicodemus  KKWolyniec  PS McGrath  JASteel  GNestadt  GLiang  KYHuganir  RLValle  DPulver  AE Bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia: a 440-single-nucleotide polymorphism screen of 64 candidate genes among Ashkenazi Jewish case-parent trios.  Am J Hum Genet 2005;77 (6) 918- 936PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Tang  WHuang  KTang  RZhou  GFang  CZhang  JDu  LFeng  GHe  LShi  Y Evidence for association between the 5′ flank of the NOS1 gene and schizophrenia in the Chinese population.  Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;11 (8) 1063- 1071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Liou  YJTsai  SJHong  CJLiao  DL Association analysis for the CA repeat polymorphism of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS1) gene and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2003;65 (1) 57- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Akbarian  SViñuela  AKim  JJPotkin  SGBunney  WE  JrJones  EG Distorted distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase neurons in temporal lobe of schizophrenics implies anomalous cortical development.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50 (3) 178- 187PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Akbarian  SBunney  WE  JrPotkin  SGWigal  SBHagman  JOSandman  CAJones  EG Altered distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase cells in frontal lobe of schizophrenics implies disturbances of cortical development.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50 (3) 169- 177PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Taneli  FPirildar  SAkdeniz  FUyanik  BSAri  Z Serum nitric oxide metabolite levels and the effect of antipsychotic therapy in schizophrenia.  Arch Med Res 2004;35 (5) 401- 405PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Das  IKhan  NSPuri  BKSooranna  SRde Belleroche  JHirsch  SR Elevated platelet calcium mobilization and nitric oxide synthase activity may reflect abnormalities in schizophrenic brain.  Biochem Biophys Res Commun 1995;212 (2) 375- 380PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Yilmaz  NHerken  HCicek  HKCelik  AYürekli  MAkyol  O Increased levels of nitric oxide, cortisol and adrenomedullin in patients with chronic schizophrenia.  Med Princ Pract 2007;16 (2) 137- 141PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Baba  HSuzuki  TArai  HEmson  PC Expression of nNOS and soluble guanylate cyclase in schizophrenic brain.  Neuroreport 2004;15 (4) 677- 680PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Harrison  PJWeinberger  DR Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence.  Mol Psychiatry 2005;10 (1) 40- 68, 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ross  CAMargolis  RLReading  SAPletnikov  MCoyle  JT Neurobiology of schizophrenia.  Neuron 2006;52 (1) 139- 153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Brzustowicz  LMSimone  JMohseni  PHayter  JEHodgkinson  KAChow  EWBassett  AS Linkage disequilibrium mapping of schizophrenia susceptibility to the CAPON region of chromosome 1q22.  Am J Hum Genet 2004;74 (5) 1057- 1063PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Xu  SGuo  SJiang  XUmezawa  THisamitsu  T The role of norepinephrine and nitric oxide in activities of rat arginine vasopressin neurons in response to immune challenge.  Neurosci Lett 2005;383 (3) 231- 235PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Lencz  TLambert  CDeRosse  PBurdick  KEMorgan  TVKane  JMKucherlapati  RMalhotra  AK Runs of homozygosity reveal highly penetrant recessive loci in schizophrenia.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104 (50) 19942- 19947PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Lee  HJSong  JYKim  JWJin  SYHong  MSPark  JKChung  JHShibata  HFukumaki  Y Association study of polymorphisms in synaptic vesicle-associated genes, SYN2 and CPLX2, with schizophrenia.  Behav Brain Funct 2005;1- 15PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Saviouk  VMoreau  MPTereshchenko  IVBrzustowicz  LM Association of synapsin 2 with schizophrenia in families of Northern European ancestry.  Schizophr Res 2007;96 (1-3) 100- 111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Walsh  T McClellan  JM McCarthy  SEAddington  AMPierce  SBCooper  GMNord  ASKusenda  MMalhotra  DBhandari  AStray  SMRippey  CFRoccanova  PMakarov  VLakshmi  BFindling  RLSikich  LStromberg  TMerriman  BGogtay  NButler  PEckstrand  KNoory  LGochman  PLong  RChen  ZDavis  SBaker  CEichler  EEMeltzer  PSNelson  SFSingleton  ABLee  MKRapoport  JLKing  MCSebat  J Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia.  Science 2008;320 (5875) 539- 543PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Kirchner  LWeitzdoerfer  RHoeger  HUrl  ASchmidt  PEngelmann  MVillar  SRFountoulakis  MLubec  GLubec  B Impaired cognitive performance in neuronal nitric oxide synthase knockout mice is associated with hippocampal protein derangements.  Nitric Oxide 2004;11 (4) 316- 330PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Weitzdoerfer  RHoeger  HEngidawork  EEngelmann  MSingewald  NLubec  GLubec  B Neuronal nitric oxide synthase knock-out mice show impaired cognitive performance.  Nitric Oxide 2004;10 (3) 130- 140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Johansson  CJackson  DMSvensson  L Nitric oxide synthase inhibition blocks phencyclidine-induced behavioural effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotor activity in the rat.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;131 (2) 167- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Johansson  CMagnusson  ODeveney  AMJackson  DMZhang  JEngel  JASvensson  L The nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, L-NAME, blocks certain phencyclidine-induced but not amphetamine-induced effects on behaviour and brain biochemistry in the rat.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1998;22 (8) 1341- 1360Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L The nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, L-NAME, block phencyclidine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition in mice.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;156 (2-3) 182- 186PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Phencyclidine-induced behaviour in mice prevented by methylene blue.  Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2004;94 (2) 65- 72PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Klamer  DPålsson  ERevesz  AEngel  JASvensson  L Habituation of acoustic startle is disrupted by psychotomimetic drugs: differential dependence on dopaminergic and nitric oxide modulatory mechanisms.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176 (3-4) 440- 450PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L The neuronal selective nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, Nomega-propyl-L-arginine, blocks the effects of phencyclidine on prepulse inhibition and locomotor activity in mice.  Eur J Pharmacol 2004;503 (1-3) 103- 107PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Effects of phencyclidine on acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in neuronal nitric oxide synthase deficient mice.  Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2005;15 (5) 587- 590PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Wass  CArcher  TPålsson  EFejgin  KAlexandersson  AKlamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Phencyclidine affects memory in a nitric oxide-dependent manner: working and reference memory.  Behav Brain Res 2006;174 (1) 49- 55PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Wass  CArcher  TPålsson  EFejgin  KKlamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Effects of phencyclidine on spatial learning and memory: nitric oxide-dependent mechanisms.  Behav Brain Res 2006;171 (1) 147- 153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Pålsson  EFejgin  KWass  CEngel  JASvensson  LKlamer  D The amino acid L-lysine blocks the disruptive effect of phencyclidine on prepulse inhibition in mice.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;192 (1) 9- 15PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Heinrichs  RWZakzanis  KK Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence.  Neuropsychology 1998;12 (3) 426- 445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–Patient Edition.  New York Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute1995;
45.
Andreasen  NC Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (Manual).  Iowa City University of Iowa Press1984;
46.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS).  Iowa City University of Iowa Press1984;
47.
Corvin  ADonohoe  GNangle  JMSchwaiger  SMorris  DGill  M A dysbindin risk haplotype associated with less severe manic-type symptoms in psychosis.  Neurosci Lett 2008;431 (2) 146- 149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  BWBenjamin  L Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II).  New York Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute1990;
49.
Tewes  U Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (HAWIE-R).  Göttingen, Germany Hogrefe1991;
50.
Rice  JPReich  TBucholz  KKNeuman  RJFishman  RRochberg  NHesselbrock  VMNurnberger  JI  JrSchuckit  MABegleiter  H Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19 (4) 1018- 1023PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Kessler  JFolstein  SEDenzler  P Mini-Mental Status Test, German Version.  Weinheim, Germany Beltz1990;
52.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III).  New York, NY Psychological Corp1997;
53.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition (WMS-III).  New York, NY Psychological Corp1998;
54.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised.  San Antonio, TX Psychological Corp1987;
55.
Härting  CMarkowitsch  HJNeufeld  HCalabrese  PDeisinger  KKessler  J Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised, German Version: Wechsler Gedächtnistest; Revidierte Fassung.  Bern, Switzerland Huber2000;
56.
Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, Expedio Version (CANTABexpedio).  Cambridge, England Cambridge Cognition Ltd2003;
57.
Egan  MFGoldberg  TEKolachana  BSCallicott  JHMazzanti  CMStraub  REGoldman  DWeinberger  DR Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;98 (12) 6917- 6922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Cornblatt  BARisch  NJFaris  GFriedman  DErlenmeyer-Kimling  L The continuous performance test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): new findings about sustained attention in normal families.  Psychiatry Res 1988;26 (2) 223- 238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Nuechterlein  KAsarnow  R 3-7 Continuous Performance Test.  Los Angeles University of California2004;
60.
Kay  SRFiszbein  AOpler  LA The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull 1987;13 (2) 261- 276PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Feil  RKleppisch  T NO/cGMP-dependent modulation of synaptic transmission.  Handb Exp Pharmacol 2008; (184) 529- 560PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Gottesman  IIGould  TD The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;160 (4) 636- 645PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Burdick  KELencz  TFunke  BFinn  CTSzeszko  PRKane  JMKucherlapati  RMalhotra  AK Genetic variation in DTNBP1 influences general cognitive ability.  Hum Mol Genet 2006;15 (10) 1563- 1568PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Donohoe  GMorris  DWClarke  S McGhee  KASchwaiger  SNangle  JMGaravan  HRobertson  IHGill  MCorvin  A Variance in neurocognitive performance is associated with dysbindin-1 in schizophrenia: a preliminary study.  Neuropsychologia 2007;45 (2) 454- 458PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Plomin  R Genetics, genes, genomics and g.  Mol Psychiatry 2003;8 (1) 1- 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Barnett  JHScoriels  LMunafò  MR Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;64 (2) 137- 144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Donohoe  GClarke  SMorris  DNangle  JMSchwaiger  SGill  MCorvin  ARobertson  IH Are deficits in executive sub-processes simply reflecting more general cognitive decline in schizophrenia?  Schizophr Res 2006;85 (1-3) 168- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Plomin  RSpinath  FM Genetics and general cognitive ability (g).  Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6 (4) 169- 176PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Engle  RWTuholski  SWLaughlin  JEConway  AR Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach.  J Exp Psychol Gen 1999;128 (3) 309- 331PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Kyllonen  PC Is working memory capacity Spearman's g? Dennis  ITapsfield  P Human Abilities Their Nature and Measurement. Philadelphia, PA Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1996;Google Scholar
71.
Stauffer  JM Cognitive-components tests are not much more than g: an extension of Kyllonen's analyses.  J Gen Psychol 1996;123193- 205Google ScholarCrossref
72.
Boissel  JPZelenka  MGödtel-Armbrust  UFeuerstein  TJFörstermann  U Transcription of different exons 1 of the human neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene is dynamically regulated in a cell- and stimulus-specific manner.  Biol Chem 2003;384 (3) 351- 362PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Bros  MBoissel  JPGödtel-Armbrust  UFörstermann  U Transcription of human neuronal nitric oxide synthase mRNAs derived from different first exons is partly controlled by exon 1-specific promoter sequences.  Genomics 2006;87 (4) 463- 473PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Hall  AVAntoniou  HWang  YCheung  AHArbus  AMOlson  SLLu  WCKau  CLMarsden  PA Structural organization of the human neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene (NOS1).  J Biol Chem 1994;269 (52) 33082- 33090PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Reif  AJacob  CPRujescu  DHerterich  SLang  SGutknecht  LBaehne  CGStrobel  AFreitag  CMGiegling  IRomanos  MHartmann  ARösler  MRenner  TJFallgatter  AJRetz  WEhlis  ACLesch  KP Influence of functional variant of neuronal nitric oxide synthase on impulsive behaviors in humans.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66 (1) 41- 50PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Jensen  LJKuhn  MStark  MChaffron  SCreevey  CMuller  JDoerks  TJulien  PRoth  ASimonovic  MBork  Pvon Mering  C STRING 8: a global view on proteins and their functional interactions in 630 organisms.  Nucleic Acids Res 2009;37 (database issue) D412- D416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Salwinski  LMiller  CSSmith  AJPettit  FKBowie  JUEisenberg  D The Database of Interacting Proteins: 2004 update.  Nucleic Acids Res 2004;32 (database issue) D449- D451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Hermjakob  HMontecchi-Palazzi  LLewington  CMudali  SKerrien  SOrchard  SVingron  MRoechert  BRoepstorff  PValencia  AMargalit  HArmstrong  JBairoch  ACesareni  GSherman  DApweiler  R IntAct: an open source molecular interaction database.  Nucleic Acids Res 2004;32 (database issue) D452- D455PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Bader  GDDonaldson  IWolting  COuellette  BFPawson  THogue  CW BIND: the Biomolecular Interaction Network Database.  Nucleic Acids Res 2001;29 (1) 242- 245PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Kawashima  ROhnuma  TShibata  NArai  H No genetic association between postsynaptic density-95 gene polymorphisms and schizophrenia.  Neurosci Lett 2006;400 (1-2) 168- 171PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Tsai  SJHong  CJCheng  CYLiao  DLLiou  YJ Association study of polymorphisms in post-synaptic density protein 95 (PSD-95) with schizophrenia.  J Neural Transm 2007;114 (4) 423- 426PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Zheng  YLi  HQin  WChen  WDuan  YXiao  YLi  CZhang  JLi  XFeng  GHe  L Association of the carboxyl-terminal PDZ ligand of neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene with schizophrenia in the Chinese Han population.  Biochem Biophys Res Commun 2005;328 (4) 809- 815PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Puri  V McQuillin  AThirumalai  SLawrence  JKrasucki  RChoudhury  KDatta  SKerwin  SQuested  DBass  NPimm  JLamb  GMoorey  HKandasami  GBadacsonyi  AKelly  KMorgan  JPunukollu  BNadeem  HCurtis  DGurling  HM Failure to confirm allelic association between markers at the CAPON gene locus and schizophrenia in a British sample.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (2) 195- 197PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
Fang  CTang  WTang  RQWang  LZhou  GQHuang  KLi  XWFeng  GYHe  MDu  LZZhu  SMXing  YLSang  HHe  LShi  YY Family-based association studies of CAPON and schizophrenia in the Chinese Han population.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008;32 (5) 1210- 1213PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
de Quervain  DJPapassotiropoulos  A Identification of a genetic cluster influencing memory performance and hippocampal activity in humans.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103 (11) 4270- 4274PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
86.
Huentelman  MJPapassotiropoulos  ACraig  DWHoerndli  FJPearson  JVHuynh  KDCorneveaux  JHänggi  JMondadori  CRBuchmann  AReiman  EMHenke  Kde Quervain  DJStephan  DA Calmodulin-binding transcription activator 1 (CAMTA1) alleles predispose human episodic memory performance.  Hum Mol Genet 2007;16 (12) 1469- 1477PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Meyer-Lindenberg  AWeinberger  DR Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7 (10) 818- 827PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Barnett  JHSalmond  CHJones  PBSahakian  BJ Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (8) 1053- 1064PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Crow  TJDone  DJSacker  A Childhood precursors of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;245 (2) 61- 69PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Kremen  WSBuka  SLSeidman  LJGoldstein  JMKoren  DTsuang  MT IQ decline during childhood and adult psychotic symptoms in a community sample: a 19-year longitudinal study.  Am J Psychiatry 1998;155 (5) 672- 677PubMedGoogle Scholar
91.
Cannon  MCaspi  AMoffitt  TEHarrington  HTaylor  AMurray  RMPoulton  R Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (5) 449- 456PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
David  ASMalmberg  ABrandt  LAllebeck  PLewis  G IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study.  Psychol Med 1997;27 (6) 1311- 1323PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Davidson  MReichenberg  ARabinowitz  JWeiser  MKaplan  ZMark  M Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents.  Am J Psychiatry 1999;156 (9) 1328- 1335PubMedGoogle Scholar
94.
Gunnell  DHarrison  GRasmussen  FFouskakis  DTynelius  P Associations between premorbid intellectual performance, early-life exposures and early-onset schizophrenia: cohort study.  Br J Psychiatry 2002;181298- 305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Reichenberg  AWeiser  MRabinowitz  JCaspi  ASchmeidler  JMark  MKaplan  ZDavidson  M A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry 2002;159 (12) 2027- 2035PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Reichenberg  AWeiser  MRapp  MARabinowitz  JCaspi  ASchmeidler  JKnobler  HYLubin  GNahon  DHarvey  PDDavidson  M Elaboration on premorbid intellectual performance in schizophrenia: premorbid intellectual decline and risk for schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (12) 1297- 1304PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
October 2009

Influence of NOS1 on Verbal Intelligence and Working Memory in Both Patients With Schizophrenia and Healthy Control Subjects

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Institute of Molecular Medicine (Drs Donohoe, Morris, Gill, and Corvin and Mss Quinn, Judge, Peel, and O’Donoghue) and Trinity Institute of Neuroscience (Drs Donohoe, Gill, and Corvin and Ms O’Donoghue), Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland; Department of Psychological Medicine, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Cardiff, Wales (Drs Walters, Norton, Williams, Moskvina, Owen, and O’Donovan); Division of Molecular and Clinical Neurobiology, Department of Psychiatry (Drs Giegling, Hartmann, and Rujescu) and Department of Psychiatry (Dr Möller), Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Munich, Germany; and Medical Genetics, Research & Development, GlaxoSmithKline, Verona, Italy (Dr Muglia).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(10):1045-1054. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.139
Abstract

Context  Human and animal studies have implicated the gene NOS1 in both cognition and schizophrenia susceptibility.

Objective  To investigate whether a potential schizophrenia risk single-nucleotide polymorphism (rs6490121) identified in a recent genome-wide association study negatively influences cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy control subjects.

Design  A comparison of both cases and controls grouped according to NOS1 genotype (GG vs AG vs AA) on selected measures of cognition in 2 independent samples. We tested for association between NOS1 rs6490121 and cognitive functions known to be impaired in schizophrenia (IQ, episodic memory, working memory, and attentional control) in an Irish sample. We then sought to replicate the significant results in a German sample.

Setting  Unrelated patients from general adult psychiatric inpatient and outpatient services and unrelated healthy volunteers from the general population were ascertained.

Participants  Patients with DSM-IV–diagnosed schizophrenia and healthy control subjects from independent samples of Irish (cases, n = 349; controls, n = 230) and German (cases, n = 232; controls, n = 1344) nationality.

Results  A main effect of NOS1 genotype on verbal IQ and working memory was observed in the Irish sample where the homozygous carriers of the schizophrenia risk G allele performed poorly compared with the other genotype groups. These findings were replicated in the German sample, again with the GG genotype carriers performing below other genotype groups. Post hoc analysis of additional IQ measures (full-scale and performance IQ) in the German sample revealed that NOS1 GG carriers underperformed on these measures also.

Conclusions  NOS1 is associated with clinically significant variation in cognition. Whether this is a mechanism by which schizophrenia risk is increased (eg, via an influence on cognitive reserve) is yet to be confirmed.

Schizophrenia (SZ) is a disorder of substantial heritability (approximately 80%) but uncertain underlying pathophysiology.1 Despite some progress, most of the variance in SZ susceptibility attributable to genetic factors remains to be identified.2 A number of recent studies using high-throughput technologies investigating structural and genomic sequence variation in large SZ samples have identified more consistent support for several susceptibility loci.3-5 As genomic data emerge, important questions are how these variants increase disease liability and whether they are relevant to only the neurobiology of SZ or to neurodevelopment more generally.6,7

One putative SZ risk gene is the nitric oxide synthase 1 gene (NOS1 [OMIM 163731]) (also known as neuronal NOS or nNOS), which maps to chromosome 12q24 and synthesizes nitric oxide (NO) in both the central and peripheral nervous system. Nitric oxide is a highly reactive messenger molecule that diffuses freely across membranes, stimulating guanylyl cyclase and modifying protein structure with multiple roles in immune, cardiac, and neurological function. It is produced by different NO synthetase (NOS) enzymes, including neuronal NOS, and is transported to different cellular compartments by adaptor proteins to minimize nonspecific interactions.

Transcription of NOS1 involves 12 alternate untranslated first exons and coding regions consisting of 28 exons covering 240 kilobases. Following modest linkage support to the NOS1 region,8-11 4 of 5 published NOS1 candidate gene association studies suggest evidence of association with SZ,12-15 with the exception being the study by Liou et al.16 Moreover, in their SZ genome-wide association study of 479 cases and 2937 controls, O’Donovan et al4 identified a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) at the NOS1 locus (rs6490121) as being 1 of 12 SNPs with strong initial statistical evidence for association (P = 9.82 × 10−6). The same allele at this SNP was significantly associated in a replication sample of 1664 cases and 3541 controls of European ancestry but not in a sample of mixed European and Asian ancestry.4

Functional evidence for involvement of NO in SZ comes from studies showing abnormal distribution of nitrinergic neurons in frontal and temporal lobes in SZ,17,18 increased NO metabolites in the serum of patients with SZ,19-21 and postmortem increased NOS1 messenger RNA in prefrontal cortex of patients.22 Neurally generated NO displays many of the properties of a neurotransmitter and is involved in processes of potential relevance to SZ, including synaptogenesis, long-term potentiation, neurotransmitter release, synaptic plasticity, blood flow, and cell death.23,24 These functions are dependent on the delivery and localization of NOS1 to multiple intracellular locations by specific adaptor proteins. Presynaptically, NOS1 is linked to the NOS1 (neuronal) adaptor protein gene (NOS1AP [OMIM 605551]) and forms a complex with synapsin, which is involved in the assembly of synaptic vesicle clusters and synaptogenesis. Both NOS1AP and the synapsin II gene (SYN2 [OMIM 600755]) have been implicated in SZ by genetic association studies.25-29 Postsynaptically, NOS1 is coupled to N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors through postsynaptic density 95, representing another potential mechanism for involvement in SZ susceptibility. Molecular pathway analysis of structural variants implicated in SZ by Walsh et al30 identified a significant excess of disrupted genes involving the NO signaling pathway. Taken together, these studies provide support for the involvement of NO signaling in SZ.

In mouse models, NOS1 knockouts have repeatedly been associated with variance in cognition.31,32 Notably, phencyclidine hydrochloride–induced cognitive and behavioral deficits that model SZ symptoms (including prepulse inhibition, habituation of acoustic startle, latent inhibition, spatial learning, spatial reference memory, and working memory) can all be prevented by interfering with the production of NO.33-42 In patients with SZ, Reif et al13 reported that 2 of 4 genetic markers tested at the NOS1 loci were associated with variance in performance on a measure of prefrontal function (the continuous performance task) in terms of behavioral performance, P300 peak amplitude, and response latency. Whether this represents a specific or more general effect on cognition or affects cognition in the general population is unknown.

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of NOS1 on cognition in SZ and control populations. We selected a single SNP at NOS1 (rs6490121) from the initial genome-wide association study by O’Donovan et al4 to which our cases had contributed as replication samples. We chose this SNP alone as it was the only SNP in the study by O’Donovan et al4 that surpassed a statistical significance threshold of P < 1.0 × 10−5 and was replicated with the same allele G in the European sample. We hypothesized that the genotypes carrying the risk allele would be associated with poorer performance on a selected set of cognitive domains typically impaired in SZ, namely IQ, episodic and working memory, and attentional control.43

Methods
Sample characteristics
Irish Patient and Control Samples (Discovery Samples)

The Irish sample consisted of 349 cases and 230 controls. Ninety-one of the patients were genotyped as part of the prior genome-wide association study4; the remaining patients and all of the control subjects were independent of that study. Cases consisted of clinically stable patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of SZ (n = 294) or schizoaffective disorder (n = 55) recruited from 5 sites across Ireland. Inclusion criteria required that participants were aged 18 to 65 years, had no history of comorbid psychiatric disorder, had no substance abuse in the preceding 6 months, had no prior head injury with loss of consciousness, and had no history of seizures. Diagnosis was confirmed by trained psychiatrists using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–Patient Edition.44 Additional diagnostic details and clinical sample characteristics ascertained at the time of the interview, including symptom severity (scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms45,46) and medication dosage, are detailed elsewhere.47

The healthy control sample was recruited on the basis of responses to local media advertisements. Control participants were included only if they were aged between 18 and 65 years and, based on clinical interview, satisfied the criteria of having no history of major mental health problems, intellectual disability, acquired brain injury, or substance misuse in the preceding 6 months based on self-report. Control participants were also excluded from the study if they reported having a first-degree relative with a history of psychosis. All patients and control assessments were conducted in accordance with the relevant ethics committees' approval from each participating site. All patients and control subjects were of Irish ancestry (ie, had 4 grandparents born in Ireland) and all provided written informed consent.

German Patient and Control Samples (Replication Samples)

The German sample consisted of 240 clinically stable patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of SZ and 1344 healthy control subjects, all of whom were genotyped as part of the previous study.4 Patients were ascertained from mental health services in the Munich area and all participants provided written informed consent. Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of SZ (symptom duration >6 months) and being aged 18 to 65 years. Exclusion criteria included a history of head injury or neurological diseases. Detailed medical and psychiatric histories were collected, including a clinical interview using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders44 and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders48 to evaluate lifetime Axis I and II diagnoses. Four physicians and 1 psychologist rated the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders responses, and all measurements were double-rated by a senior researcher (A.C.). Among the patients, 68% were of strict German descent (ie, all 4 grandparents were born in Germany) and the other 32% were German Caucasian. Of the 240 patients, 216 completed the full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised49 assessment and 232 completed a further comprehensive neuropsychological battery.

Healthy control participants of German descent (ie, all 4 grandparents being German) were randomly selected from the general population of Munich, Germany, and contacted by mail. Control subjects for this study were included only if they were aged between 18 and 65 years. To exclude subjects with central neurological diseases and psychotic disorders or subjects who had first-degree relatives with psychotic disorders, several screenings were conducted before the volunteers were enrolled in the study. First, subjects who responded were initially screened by telephone for the absence of neuropsychiatric disorders. Second, detailed medical and psychiatric histories were assessed for both themselves and their first-degree relatives by using a semistructured interview. Third, if no exclusion criteria were fulfilled, the subjects were invited to a comprehensive interview including the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–Patient Edition44 and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders48 to validate the absence of any lifetime psychotic disorder. Additionally, the Family History Assessment Module50 was conducted to exclude psychotic disorders among first-degree relatives. A neurological examination was also conducted to exclude subjects with current central nervous system impairment. In volunteers older than 60 years, the Mini-Mental Status Test51 was performed to exclude subjects with possible cognitive impairment. All of the 1344 control participants completed the full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised assessment, and 364 completed tests from a further extensive neuropsychological battery.

Cognitive assessment

This study was designed so that identical or near identical tests of the phenotypes of general cognitive function (IQ), episodic memory, working memory, and attention were used for both the Irish discovery samples and the German replication samples. Only 1 verbal and 1 visuospatial measure for each cognitive function were tested in the Irish samples. Replication in the German samples was then sought for the cognitive functions showing significant association in the discovery sample.

General Cognitive Functioning (IQ)

General cognitive functioning (IQ) was measured in the Irish sample using selected subtests (vocabulary, similarities, block design, and matrix reasoning) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third edition,52 yielding full-scale, verbal, and performance IQs. For the German samples, IQ was indexed by the German version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised49 using all 11 verbal or performance subtests (vocabulary, comprehension, information, digit span, arithmetic, similarities, block design, picture completion, picture arrangement, object assembly, and digit symbol coding).

Episodic Memory

Verbal episodic memory and visual episodic memory were assessed in the Irish samples using the logical memory and faces subtests, respectively, from the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition.53 In the German samples, these were assessed using the logical memory and visual memory scores from the German version of the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised.54,55

Working Memory

Verbal working memory and spatial working memory were assessed in the Irish samples using the letter-number sequencing task from the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition, and the spatial working memory task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, Expedio Version.56 In the German samples, working memory was measured using the composite digit span and spatial span score from the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised and the N-back task (National Institute of Mental Health version).57

Attentional Control

Attentional control was assessed in the Irish samples using the continuous performance test, identical pairs version.58 In the German sample, it was assessed using the 3-7 Continuous Performance Test.59 Full descriptions of all memory and attention tasks are provided in the eText.

Genotyping of samples

The SNP rs6490121 was genotyped in the German samples and a proportion of the Irish samples using the iPLEX Gold system (Sequenom, Inc, San Diego, California) (further details are provided in the article by O’Donovan et al4). The call rate for the iPLEX genotyping was greater than 99%, and all Irish and German case and control samples were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P > .05). The remainder of the Irish samples (n = 529) was genotyped using a Taqman SNP Genotyping Assay on a 7900HT Sequence Detection System (Applied Biosystems Inc, Foster City, California). The call rate for the Taqman genotyping was greater than 95%, and both case and control samples were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P > .05). Along with the Irish samples, a number of HapMap CEU DNA samples (http://www.hapmap.org) were genotyped for rs6490121 for quality control purposes and were all found to be concordant with available online HapMap data for this SNP.

Statistical analyses

Association between NOS1 rs6490121 and the cognitive intermediate phenotypes of general cognitive function, episodic memory, working memory, and attentional control was tested using a general factorial design in SPSS statistical software version 14.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois). In the original article identifying this variant,4 there was no difference in the genotypic vs allelic model; as there was no evidence on which to test a specific dominant or recessive model and as sample sizes allowed, our analysis was based on a comparison of all 3 genotype groups. The NOS1 genotype (GG vs AG vs AA) and diagnosis (cases vs controls) were entered as fixed effects. Scores for each neuropsychological subtest were entered as the dependent variables, with age and sex included as covariates as appropriate.

Results

Clinical and demographic characteristics by genotype for each sample appear in Table 1. For the discovery control sample, NOS1 genotypes were not associated with age, sex, or years of education. For the patient group, AA genotype carriers were observed to be older than the AG carriers by an average of 4 years (t = −2.94; uncorrected P = .03); the GG group did not differ significantly in age from the other genotype groups. To investigate whether possible differences in symptom severity rating between genotype groups might influence cognitive performance in cases, we conducted a principal components analysis with varimax rotation of all scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, yielding 3 factors of positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms (Table 1, eTable 1, and eTable 2). We did not observe any differences on any of the symptom factors associated with genotype. We also did not observe differences in medication dosage associated with NOS1 genotype (all P > .05). A similar analysis of Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale60 scores in the German sample also failed to identify any differences in symptom severity associated with NOS1; medication dosage was similarly nonsignificant (all P > .05).

NOS1 AND GENERAL COGNITIVE ABILITY

Analysis of verbal IQ in the Irish sample revealed a significant overall effect of genotype (Table 2). Tukey post hoc analysis revealed that this effect was driven by differences between the GG and AA groups (t = −6.36; P = .04). An effect size estimate (partial η2) suggests that genotype explains approximately 2% in cases and approximately 3.5% in controls. In clinical terms, carriers of the risk GG allele performed on average 5 or 6 standard score verbal IQ points below carriers of the AG and AA genotypes (Figure 1). No genotype × diagnostic group interaction was observed (even though, as expected, patients performed significantly lower than control subjects on this and all other cognitive tests [Table 2]). Although the same general pattern of results was observed for performance and full-scale IQ scores, these differences were not statistically significant (P > .05).

Analysis of verbal IQ in the German sample also revealed a significant overall effect of genotype (Table 2), with carriers of the risk GG allele performing significantly below AA and AG carriers. Based on post hoc analysis, the largest difference was between the GG and AG genotype groups (t = 2.87; P = .01), with the difference between the GG and AA groups just failing to achieve statistical significance (t = 2.34; P = .051). In this sample, an interaction effect with affected status was also observed (F = 11.51; P < .001); as illustrated in Figure 1, the association with genotype appears to be strongest in cases. An effect size estimate (partial η2) suggests that genotype explains approximately 5% of variance in verbal IQ in cases and 0.2% in controls.

Following this replication of association between NOS1 genotype and verbal IQ, we investigated post hoc whether NOS1 was also associated with the 2 other indices of IQ in the German sample—performance and full-scale IQ. A similar pattern of differences associated with genotype was observed (Table 2). Genotype × diagnosis interactions were observed for both scores (performance IQ: F = 11.52; P < .001; full-scale IQ: F = 8.95; P < .001) such that the effect of genotype was apparent in cases only (Figure 1). For cases, the estimate of effect size was 6.9% for performance IQ and 6.4% for full-scale IQ.

As an additional secondary analysis, we further sought to determine whether the overall effect of genotype was comparable when cases and controls were considered separately. For the Irish samples, verbal IQ was not significantly associated with NOS1 when cases and controls were considered individually (P > .05), suggesting insufficient power to analyze these samples separately. In the German samples, the GG genotype was associated with significantly poorer verbal IQ in patients (F = 5.54; P = .005) but not control subjects; this pattern of association in patients but not control subjects was also observed for performance IQ in patients (F = 7.84; P = .001) and full-scale IQ in patients (F = 7.26; P = .001).

NOS1 AND WORKING MEMORY PERFORMANCE

For working memory, we observed a significant difference associated with genotype in verbal working memory as measured in the Irish samples by scores on the letter-number sequencing task of the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition (Table 2). While the association was statistically significant as a main effect (F = 4.02; P = .02), as Figure 2 illustrates and a statistical interaction of genotype × diagnosis confirms, this association is clearly driven by differences in the control group. For the control group, the risk GG allele carriers performed 3 scaled score points (1 SD based on norms for the test) lower than the AA carriers on this measure of verbal working memory, with the between-group difference significant between GG carriers and both other genotype groups (GG < AG: t = −2.21; P = .01; GG < AA: t = −3.05; P < .001). A significant main effect of genotype was also observed with the spatial working memory task used (Table 2), in the absence of an interaction with diagnosis. Post hoc analysis reveals the difference as largest between the AG and AA groups (t = 0.31; P = .02).

Using 2 different measures of working memory in the German samples, the risk GG allele was again associated with poorer performance on both measures (Table 2). While association with genotype was observed as a main effect, in the absence of a statistical interaction with diagnosis, the effect size appeared slightly larger in cases than in controls (N-back partial η2 = 2.2% vs 1.1%, respectively).

We again sought to determine whether the overall effect of genotype was comparable when cases and controls were considered separately. For the Irish samples, differences on both working memory tasks were associated with NOS1 genotype in controls (letter-number sequencing task: F2,163 = 9.02; P < .001; spatial working memory task: F2,163 = 4.99; P = .008) but not patients (P > .05). When the German case and control samples were considered separately, the association was seen in the patient group but not the control group on the working memory score of the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition (F = 5.49; P = .005); for the N-back task, the effects of genotype were no longer apparent when cases and controls were considered separately.

NOS1 GENOTYPE, MEMORY, AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

In the Irish samples, no differences associated with NOS1 genotype were observed on either the verbal or nonverbal memory tests used (the logical memory and Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery paired associates learning tests, respectively). Similarly, when the effects of NOS1 on attention were investigated using the intradimensional-extradimensional task (scores for block 8) and the continuous performance test, identical pairs version task (using d′ values for each of the 3 conditions), no significant associations were observed. As no association with NOS1 genotype was observed in the Irish samples (all P > .05), we did not investigate these aspects of cognition in the German samples.

Comment

Variants at NOS1 have previously been associated with both increased SZ risk and variation in cognitive performance. The involvement of NOS1 in cognition is well established from animal models, with the N-methyl-D-aspartate–NO–cyclic guanine monophosphate signaling cascade being linked to neurotransmitter release and synaptic plasticity in multiple genetic and pharmacological studies.61 From the known biology of SZ, NOS-driven negative feedback on N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor function and inhibition of synaptic reuptake of dopamine positions it at the crossroads between 2 messenger systems whose mutual regulation is a leading hypothesis for SZ.23

This study shows that a NOS1 SNP, identified as a putative risk variant by a recent SZ genome-wide association study, is associated with variation in verbal intelligence and working memory in 2 large independent samples. Significant issues for studies addressing the cognitive effects of previously identified risk variants in SZ have included small sample size, lack of a suitable replication sample within the same study design, and a heavy multiple testing burden. One approach to the issue of multiple testing is statistical (eg, Bonferroni) correction; however, the expected effect size based on previous molecular studies of cognition and the nonindependence of measures of cognition serve to limit the value of these statistical approaches. Instead, our approach was to seek independent replication of significant results using 2 large independent case and control samples, both of which contributed to the original identification of the NOS1 rs6490121 variant studied here.4 Second, only a tightly selected group of cognitive functions (IQ, memory, and attention), each measuring a core cognitive deficit in SZ, were selected for analysis on the basis of their availability in both samples. Finally, this study was based on a single associated allele and results obtained were in the same expected direction in both samples, with homozygous G carriers—the associated allele in the original genome-wide association study—performing more poorly.

The utility of the cognitive intermediate phenotypes approach in SZ has been well articulated as potentially representing less complex genetic architecture than the broader disease phenotype, often shared by unaffected relatives, and thus capable of increasing power to detect genetic effects.62 While verbal IQ and working memory are themselves complex processes likely to be influenced by many genetic variants (eg, DTNBP1),63-65 the observed effect of the NOS1 genotype on cognition is not simply statistically significant. In both samples, the size of the association for verbal IQ in clinical terms is between 0.3 and 0.5 SD. For working memory, the differences between genotype groups represent a difference of approximately 1 SD. By comparison with findings of association between cognitive performance and other gene variants, the observed effect size is considerably higher than that reported in the recent meta-analysis of catechol O-methyltransferase and IQ66 but comparable to reports of the association between dysbindin and general cognitive ability in patients and control subjects63 and spatial working memory in patients.64 While the overall pattern was the same across the Irish and German samples, the effect size varied between patients and healthy control subjects. Such differences may reflect differences related to sample size or ascertainment strategy (eg, the somewhat better-than-expected overall performance of German patients and Irish control subjects, respectively).

That NOS1 genotype was associated with both verbal IQ and working memory but not attentional control is noteworthy. We have previously argued that deficits in working memory, unlike attentional control, correlate with the general cognitive decline apparent in SZ.67 Working memory has been characterized as the common factor in Spearman g, a measure of general cognitive function derived from the statistical correlation between cognitive tasks. Alternatively, working memory may simply be another name for g given the overlap between working memory and psychometric tests of g.68 This is supported by the evidence that various measures of working memory correlate with g at a level corresponding to the reliability of the measure used.69-71 Our association with indices of both (verbal) intelligence and working memory is consistent with this evidence and suggests that for NOS1, verbal IQ and working memory may represent overlapping or at least psychometrically similar constructs. Providing empirical support for this point, covarying for the influence of either cognitive variable resulted in the association of the other with NOS1 becoming nonsignificant (eTable 3 and eTable 4). These findings are consistent with cognitive studies in both animal and human models of NOS1 where a general rather than specific effect on cognition is suggested.13,31,32,38-41

NOS1 AND COGNITION: MOLECULAR MECHANISM

The implicated SNP (rs6490121), located in intron 10 of NOS1, has no obvious functional effect and may reflect a proxy association with 1 or more other causal variants. Based on HapMap CEU data, rs6490121 is not in high linkage disequilibrium (r2 > 0.80) with any other common SNP at this locus. NOS1 is characterized by complex transcriptional regulation. Twelve alternative first exons have been identified (exon 1a-1l).72 Exons 1d and 1f, along with the exon 1g transcript, are the most commonly expressed NOS1 transcripts in the brain.73 A dinucleotide variable-number tandem repeat is located in the core promoter region of exon 1f.74 The short alleles of this variant, termed NOS1 Ex1f variable-number tandem repeat, are associated with decreased transcriptional activity of the NOS1 exon 1f promoter, alterations in the neuronal transcriptome affecting other putative SZ susceptibility genes (RGS4 and GRIN1), and effects in electrocortical measures of prefrontal functions involved in cognitive control.75 Interestingly, data from our laboratory indicate that the risk G allele of rs6490121 is in linkage disequilibrium (D′ = 0.70, r2 = 0.26, HapMap CEU) with the short alleles of the NOS1 Ex1f variable-number tandem repeat, highlighting a putative functional mechanism of cognitive performance dysregulation at NOS1 for future study.

To explore potential mechanisms by which NO could exert an effect on cognitive processes, we screened experimentally validated protein-protein interactions of NOS1 using the protein-protein interaction databases (STRING [Search Tool for the Retrieval of Interacting Genes/Proteins],76 DIP [Database of Interacting Proteins],77 IntAct,78 and BIND [Biomolecular Interaction Network Database]79) and identified 19 confirmed human binary interactions. With potential relevance to SZ susceptibility are genes involved in presynaptic synaptogenesis (NOS1AP and syntrophin [SNTA1] [OMIM 601017]) and postsynaptically through the postsynaptic density 95–NOS1 receptor complex including direct NOS1 interaction with GRIN1 and GRIN2b. Elements of PSD95 signaling cascades have been targeted in SZ genetic association studies including erbB4/neuregulin signaling and the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor complex, which is involved in long-term potentiation, memory, and learning. There is some support from these studies for involvement of GRIN2B, GRM3, erbB4, and NRG1 in SZ susceptibility. Negative studies have been reported for PSD95 (studies by Kawashima et al80 and Tsai et al81) and NOS1AP (studies by Zheng et al,82 Puri et al,83 and Fang et al84). de Quervain and Papassotiropoulos85 identified variation at GRIN2B and GRM3 as genes contributing to the memory formation signaling cascade involved in episodic memory performance. Additionally, NOS1 activators calmodulin and protein kinase C, alpha, although not directly associated with SZ risk, have also been associated with memory in humans.85,86 More systematic studies of genes involved in NOS1 regulation and adaptor proteins linked to synaptic function and plasticity are warranted, and we plan to screen functional variants at genes from these pathways for additive and epistatic effects on cognition and SZ susceptibility.

NOS1 AND SZ: FUNCTIONAL IMPLICATIONS

The findings reported here raise interesting questions. Is NOS1 an SZ susceptibility gene? Or, is NOS1 perhaps a modifier gene that influences cognitive deficits without altering disease liability?6,87 The present data do not provide evidence one way or another for association between NOS1 and SZ. Including the additional Irish samples in the meta-analysis of NOS1 by O’Donovan et al4 does not change the significance of the findings of that study (ie, NOS1 remains moderately associated with SZ in the European-only sample [P = .005, 2-tailed], but not in the total European and Asian sample combined). One interpretation of these findings is in terms of cognitive reserve—the idea that individuals with lower IQ are more vulnerable to or more likely to express the features of neurodevelopmental, neurodegenerative, or acquired brain injury.88 In SZ, evidence of low intelligence as a risk factor is long established, with both prospective birth cohort studies89-91 and conscription studies92-96 consistently reporting lower intelligence in children and adolescents who will later develop SZ. However, differences in education (often used as a proxy measure of premorbid IQ) were observed as a trend in only the Irish samples and were absent in the German samples, thus only weakly supporting lower premorbid IQ as the mechanism of risk associated with NOS1. Instead, given the association between NOS1 and cognition in both patients and control subjects shown here, a more parsimonious explanation is that NOS1 influences cognition generally, an effect that is observed to some extent in both cases and controls. In terms of the intermediate phenotype approach in psychiatric genetics, these findings highlight the benefits of cognitive studies of putative SZ variants for understanding the genetics of fundamental cognitive and neurodevelopmental processes. It remains an open question as to the degree to which these genes are coding for disease-specific processes, either as mediated through or independent of the effects on cognition reported here.

Correspondence: Gary Donohoe, DClinPsych, PhD, Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Trinity Health Sciences Bldg, St James's Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland (gary.donohoe@tcd.ie).

Submitted for Publication: September 30, 2008; final revision received January 14, 2009; accepted February 23, 2009.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: Recruitment of the patients from Munich, Germany, was partially supported by GlaxoSmithKline. Genotyping of the German samples was funded by grants from the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust. Recruitment and genotyping of the Irish samples were supported by the Wellcome Trust and Science Foundation Ireland. Dr Donohoe was supported by a research frontiers grant from Science Foundation Ireland and a Young Investigator Award from NARSAD. Dr Walters was supported by a clinical training fellowship from the Medical Research Council.

Additional Contributions: We sincerely thank all patients who contributed to this study and all staff who facilitated their involvement. We are grateful to the Genetics Research Centre GmbH, an initiative by GlaxoSmithKline and Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität.

References
1.
Cardno  AGGottesman  II Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics.  Am J Med Genet 2000;97 (1) 12- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Sullivan  PF Schizophrenia genetics: the search for a hard lead.  Curr Opin Psychiatry 2008;21 (2) 157- 160PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
International Schizophrenia Consortium, Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia.  Nature 2008;455 (7210) 237- 241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
O'Donovan  MCCraddock  NNorton  NWilliams  HPeirce  TMoskvina  VNikolov  IHamshere  MCarroll  LGeorgieva  LDwyer  SHolmans  PMarchini  JLSpencer  CCHowie  BLeung  HTHartmann  AMMöller  HJMorris  DWShi  YFeng  GHoffmann  PPropping  PVasilescu  CMaier  WRietschel  MZammit  SSchumacher  JQuinn  EMSchulze  TGWilliams  NMGiegling  IIwata  NIkeda  MDarvasi  AShifman  SHe  LDuan  JSanders  ARLevinson  DFGejman  PVCichon  SNöthen  MMGill  MCorvin  ARujescu  DKirov  GOwen  MJBuccola  NGMowry  BJFreedman  RAmin  FBlack  DWSilverman  JMByerley  WFCloninger  CRMolecular Genetics of Schizophrenia Collaboration, Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up.  Nat Genet 2008;40 (9) 1053- 1055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Stefansson  HRujescu  DCichon  SPietiläinen  OPIngason  ASteinberg  SFossdal  RSigurdsson  ESigmundsson  TBuizer-Voskamp  JEHansen  TJakobsen  KDMuglia  PFrancks  CMatthews  PMGylfason  AHalldorsson  BVGudbjartsson  DThorgeirsson  TESigurdsson  AJonasdottir  AJonasdottir  ABjornsson  AMattiasdottir  SBlondal  THaraldsson  MMagnusdottir  BBGiegling  IMöller  HJHartmann  AShianna  KVGe  DNeed  ACCrombie  CFraser  GWalker  NLonnqvist  JSuvisaari  JTuulio-Henriksson  APaunio  TToulopoulou  TBramon  EDi Forti  MMurray  RRuggeri  MVassos  ETosato  SWalshe  MLi  TVasilescu  CMühleisen  TWWang  AGUllum  HDjurovic  SMelle  IOlesen  JKiemeney  LAFranke  BSabatti  CFreimer  NBGulcher  JRThorsteinsdottir  UKong  AAndreassen  OAOphoff  RAGeorgi  ARietschel  MWerge  TPetursson  HGoldstein  DBNöthen  MMPeltonen  LCollier  DASt Clair  DStefansson  KGROUP, Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia.  Nature 2008;455 (7210) 232- 236PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Fanous  AHKendler  KS Genetics of clinical features and subtypes of schizophrenia: a review of the recent literature.  Curr Psychiatry Rep 2008;10 (2) 164- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Mefford  HCSharp  AJBaker  CItsara  AJiang  ZBuysse  KHuang  SMaloney  VKCrolla  JABaralle  DCollins  AMercer  CNorga  Kde Ravel  TDevriendt  KBongers  EMde Leeuw  NReardon  WGimelli  SBena  FHennekam  RCMale  AGaunt  LClayton-Smith  JSimonic  IPark  SMMehta  SGNik-Zainal  SWoods  CGFirth  HVParkin  GFichera  MReitano  SGiudice  MLLi  KECasuga  IBroomer  AConrad  BSchwerzmann  MRäber  LGallati  SStriano  PCoppola  ATolmie  JLTobias  ESLilley  CArmengol  LSpysschaert  YVerloo  PDe Coene  AGoossens  LMortier  GSpeleman  Fvan Binsbergen  ENelen  MRHochstenbach  RPoot  MGallagher  LGill  M McClellan  JKing  MCRegan  RSkinner  CStevenson  REAntonarakis  SEChen  CEstivill  XMenten  BGimelli  GGribble  SSchwartz  SSutcliffe  JSWalsh  TKnight  SJSebat  JRomano  CSchwartz  CEVeltman  JAde Vries  BBVermeesch  JRBarber  JCWillatt  LTassabehji  MEichler  EE Recurrent rearrangements of chromosome 1q21.1 and variable pediatric phenotypes.  N Engl J Med 2008;359 (16) 1685- 1699PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Abkevich  VCamp  NJHensel  CHNeff  CDRussell  DLHughes  DCPlenk  AMLowry  MRRichards  RLCarter  CFrech  GCStone  SRowe  KChau  CACortado  KHunt  ALuce  KO'Neil  GPoarch  JPotter  JPoulsen  GHSaxton  HBernat-Sestak  MThompson  VGutin  ASkolnick  MHShattuck  DCannon-Albright  L Predisposition locus for major depression at chromosome 12q22-12q23.2.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;73 (6) 1271- 1281PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Bailer  ULeisch  FMeszaros  KLenzinger  EWillinger  UStrobl  RGebhardt  CGerhard  EFuchs  KSieghart  WKasper  SHornik  KAschauer  HN Genome scan for susceptibility loci for schizophrenia.  Neuropsychobiology 2000;42 (4) 175- 182PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Bailer  ULeisch  FMeszaros  KLenzinger  EWillinger  UStrobl  RHeiden  A Gebhardt  CDöge  EFuchs  KSieghart  WKasper  SHornik  KAschauer  HN Genome scan for susceptibility loci for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.   Biol Psychiatry 2002;52 (1) 40- 52PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
DeLisi  LEShaw  SHCrow  TJShields  GSmith  ABLarach  VWWellman  NLoftus  JNanthakumar  BRazi  KStewart  JComazzi  MVita  AHeffner  TSherrington  R A genome-wide scan for linkage to chromosomal regions in 382 sibling pairs with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder.  Am J Psychiatry 2002;159 (5) 803- 812PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Shinkai  TOhmori  OHori  HNakamura  J Allelic association of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS1) gene with schizophrenia.  Mol Psychiatry 2002;7 (6) 560- 563PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Reif  AHerterich  SStrobel  AEhlis  ACSaur  DJacob  CPWienker  TTöpner  TFritzen  SWalter  USchmitt  AFallgatter  AJLesch  KP A neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) haplotype associated with schizophrenia modifies prefrontal cortex function.  Mol Psychiatry 2006;11 (3) 286- 300PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Fallin  MDLasseter  VKAvramopoulos  DNicodemus  KKWolyniec  PS McGrath  JASteel  GNestadt  GLiang  KYHuganir  RLValle  DPulver  AE Bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia: a 440-single-nucleotide polymorphism screen of 64 candidate genes among Ashkenazi Jewish case-parent trios.  Am J Hum Genet 2005;77 (6) 918- 936PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Tang  WHuang  KTang  RZhou  GFang  CZhang  JDu  LFeng  GHe  LShi  Y Evidence for association between the 5′ flank of the NOS1 gene and schizophrenia in the Chinese population.  Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;11 (8) 1063- 1071PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Liou  YJTsai  SJHong  CJLiao  DL Association analysis for the CA repeat polymorphism of the neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS1) gene and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2003;65 (1) 57- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Akbarian  SViñuela  AKim  JJPotkin  SGBunney  WE  JrJones  EG Distorted distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase neurons in temporal lobe of schizophrenics implies anomalous cortical development.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50 (3) 178- 187PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Akbarian  SBunney  WE  JrPotkin  SGWigal  SBHagman  JOSandman  CAJones  EG Altered distribution of nicotinamide-adenine dinucleotide phosphate-diaphorase cells in frontal lobe of schizophrenics implies disturbances of cortical development.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50 (3) 169- 177PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Taneli  FPirildar  SAkdeniz  FUyanik  BSAri  Z Serum nitric oxide metabolite levels and the effect of antipsychotic therapy in schizophrenia.  Arch Med Res 2004;35 (5) 401- 405PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Das  IKhan  NSPuri  BKSooranna  SRde Belleroche  JHirsch  SR Elevated platelet calcium mobilization and nitric oxide synthase activity may reflect abnormalities in schizophrenic brain.  Biochem Biophys Res Commun 1995;212 (2) 375- 380PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Yilmaz  NHerken  HCicek  HKCelik  AYürekli  MAkyol  O Increased levels of nitric oxide, cortisol and adrenomedullin in patients with chronic schizophrenia.  Med Princ Pract 2007;16 (2) 137- 141PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Baba  HSuzuki  TArai  HEmson  PC Expression of nNOS and soluble guanylate cyclase in schizophrenic brain.  Neuroreport 2004;15 (4) 677- 680PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Harrison  PJWeinberger  DR Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence.  Mol Psychiatry 2005;10 (1) 40- 68, 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Ross  CAMargolis  RLReading  SAPletnikov  MCoyle  JT Neurobiology of schizophrenia.  Neuron 2006;52 (1) 139- 153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Brzustowicz  LMSimone  JMohseni  PHayter  JEHodgkinson  KAChow  EWBassett  AS Linkage disequilibrium mapping of schizophrenia susceptibility to the CAPON region of chromosome 1q22.  Am J Hum Genet 2004;74 (5) 1057- 1063PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Xu  SGuo  SJiang  XUmezawa  THisamitsu  T The role of norepinephrine and nitric oxide in activities of rat arginine vasopressin neurons in response to immune challenge.  Neurosci Lett 2005;383 (3) 231- 235PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Lencz  TLambert  CDeRosse  PBurdick  KEMorgan  TVKane  JMKucherlapati  RMalhotra  AK Runs of homozygosity reveal highly penetrant recessive loci in schizophrenia.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007;104 (50) 19942- 19947PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Lee  HJSong  JYKim  JWJin  SYHong  MSPark  JKChung  JHShibata  HFukumaki  Y Association study of polymorphisms in synaptic vesicle-associated genes, SYN2 and CPLX2, with schizophrenia.  Behav Brain Funct 2005;1- 15PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Saviouk  VMoreau  MPTereshchenko  IVBrzustowicz  LM Association of synapsin 2 with schizophrenia in families of Northern European ancestry.  Schizophr Res 2007;96 (1-3) 100- 111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Walsh  T McClellan  JM McCarthy  SEAddington  AMPierce  SBCooper  GMNord  ASKusenda  MMalhotra  DBhandari  AStray  SMRippey  CFRoccanova  PMakarov  VLakshmi  BFindling  RLSikich  LStromberg  TMerriman  BGogtay  NButler  PEckstrand  KNoory  LGochman  PLong  RChen  ZDavis  SBaker  CEichler  EEMeltzer  PSNelson  SFSingleton  ABLee  MKRapoport  JLKing  MCSebat  J Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia.  Science 2008;320 (5875) 539- 543PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Kirchner  LWeitzdoerfer  RHoeger  HUrl  ASchmidt  PEngelmann  MVillar  SRFountoulakis  MLubec  GLubec  B Impaired cognitive performance in neuronal nitric oxide synthase knockout mice is associated with hippocampal protein derangements.  Nitric Oxide 2004;11 (4) 316- 330PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Weitzdoerfer  RHoeger  HEngidawork  EEngelmann  MSingewald  NLubec  GLubec  B Neuronal nitric oxide synthase knock-out mice show impaired cognitive performance.  Nitric Oxide 2004;10 (3) 130- 140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Johansson  CJackson  DMSvensson  L Nitric oxide synthase inhibition blocks phencyclidine-induced behavioural effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotor activity in the rat.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;131 (2) 167- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Johansson  CMagnusson  ODeveney  AMJackson  DMZhang  JEngel  JASvensson  L The nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, L-NAME, blocks certain phencyclidine-induced but not amphetamine-induced effects on behaviour and brain biochemistry in the rat.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1998;22 (8) 1341- 1360Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L The nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, L-NAME, block phencyclidine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition in mice.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;156 (2-3) 182- 186PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Phencyclidine-induced behaviour in mice prevented by methylene blue.  Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2004;94 (2) 65- 72PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Klamer  DPålsson  ERevesz  AEngel  JASvensson  L Habituation of acoustic startle is disrupted by psychotomimetic drugs: differential dependence on dopaminergic and nitric oxide modulatory mechanisms.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176 (3-4) 440- 450PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L The neuronal selective nitric oxide synthase inhibitor, Nomega-propyl-L-arginine, blocks the effects of phencyclidine on prepulse inhibition and locomotor activity in mice.  Eur J Pharmacol 2004;503 (1-3) 103- 107PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Klamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Effects of phencyclidine on acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in neuronal nitric oxide synthase deficient mice.  Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2005;15 (5) 587- 590PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Wass  CArcher  TPålsson  EFejgin  KAlexandersson  AKlamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Phencyclidine affects memory in a nitric oxide-dependent manner: working and reference memory.  Behav Brain Res 2006;174 (1) 49- 55PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Wass  CArcher  TPålsson  EFejgin  KKlamer  DEngel  JASvensson  L Effects of phencyclidine on spatial learning and memory: nitric oxide-dependent mechanisms.  Behav Brain Res 2006;171 (1) 147- 153PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Pålsson  EFejgin  KWass  CEngel  JASvensson  LKlamer  D The amino acid L-lysine blocks the disruptive effect of phencyclidine on prepulse inhibition in mice.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;192 (1) 9- 15PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Heinrichs  RWZakzanis  KK Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence.  Neuropsychology 1998;12 (3) 426- 445PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders–Patient Edition.  New York Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute1995;
45.
Andreasen  NC Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (Manual).  Iowa City University of Iowa Press1984;
46.
Andreasen  NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS).  Iowa City University of Iowa Press1984;
47.
Corvin  ADonohoe  GNangle  JMSchwaiger  SMorris  DGill  M A dysbindin risk haplotype associated with less severe manic-type symptoms in psychosis.  Neurosci Lett 2008;431 (2) 146- 149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  BWBenjamin  L Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II).  New York Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute1990;
49.
Tewes  U Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (HAWIE-R).  Göttingen, Germany Hogrefe1991;
50.
Rice  JPReich  TBucholz  KKNeuman  RJFishman  RRochberg  NHesselbrock  VMNurnberger  JI  JrSchuckit  MABegleiter  H Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19 (4) 1018- 1023PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Kessler  JFolstein  SEDenzler  P Mini-Mental Status Test, German Version.  Weinheim, Germany Beltz1990;
52.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III).  New York, NY Psychological Corp1997;
53.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition (WMS-III).  New York, NY Psychological Corp1998;
54.
Wechsler  D Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised.  San Antonio, TX Psychological Corp1987;
55.
Härting  CMarkowitsch  HJNeufeld  HCalabrese  PDeisinger  KKessler  J Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised, German Version: Wechsler Gedächtnistest; Revidierte Fassung.  Bern, Switzerland Huber2000;
56.
Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, Expedio Version (CANTABexpedio).  Cambridge, England Cambridge Cognition Ltd2003;
57.
Egan  MFGoldberg  TEKolachana  BSCallicott  JHMazzanti  CMStraub  REGoldman  DWeinberger  DR Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;98 (12) 6917- 6922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Cornblatt  BARisch  NJFaris  GFriedman  DErlenmeyer-Kimling  L The continuous performance test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): new findings about sustained attention in normal families.  Psychiatry Res 1988;26 (2) 223- 238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Nuechterlein  KAsarnow  R 3-7 Continuous Performance Test.  Los Angeles University of California2004;
60.
Kay  SRFiszbein  AOpler  LA The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull 1987;13 (2) 261- 276PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Feil  RKleppisch  T NO/cGMP-dependent modulation of synaptic transmission.  Handb Exp Pharmacol 2008; (184) 529- 560PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Gottesman  IIGould  TD The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;160 (4) 636- 645PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Burdick  KELencz  TFunke  BFinn  CTSzeszko  PRKane  JMKucherlapati  RMalhotra  AK Genetic variation in DTNBP1 influences general cognitive ability.  Hum Mol Genet 2006;15 (10) 1563- 1568PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Donohoe  GMorris  DWClarke  S McGhee  KASchwaiger  SNangle  JMGaravan  HRobertson  IHGill  MCorvin  A Variance in neurocognitive performance is associated with dysbindin-1 in schizophrenia: a preliminary study.  Neuropsychologia 2007;45 (2) 454- 458PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Plomin  R Genetics, genes, genomics and g.  Mol Psychiatry 2003;8 (1) 1- 5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Barnett  JHScoriels  LMunafò  MR Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;64 (2) 137- 144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Donohoe  GClarke  SMorris  DNangle  JMSchwaiger  SGill  MCorvin  ARobertson  IH Are deficits in executive sub-processes simply reflecting more general cognitive decline in schizophrenia?  Schizophr Res 2006;85 (1-3) 168- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Plomin  RSpinath  FM Genetics and general cognitive ability (g).  Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6 (4) 169- 176PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Engle  RWTuholski  SWLaughlin  JEConway  AR Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach.  J Exp Psychol Gen 1999;128 (3) 309- 331PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Kyllonen  PC Is working memory capacity Spearman's g? Dennis  ITapsfield  P Human Abilities Their Nature and Measurement. Philadelphia, PA Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1996;Google Scholar
71.
Stauffer  JM Cognitive-components tests are not much more than g: an extension of Kyllonen's analyses.  J Gen Psychol 1996;123193- 205Google ScholarCrossref
72.
Boissel  JPZelenka  MGödtel-Armbrust  UFeuerstein  TJFörstermann  U Transcription of different exons 1 of the human neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene is dynamically regulated in a cell- and stimulus-specific manner.  Biol Chem 2003;384 (3) 351- 362PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Bros  MBoissel  JPGödtel-Armbrust  UFörstermann  U Transcription of human neuronal nitric oxide synthase mRNAs derived from different first exons is partly controlled by exon 1-specific promoter sequences.  Genomics 2006;87 (4) 463- 473PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Hall  AVAntoniou  HWang  YCheung  AHArbus  AMOlson  SLLu  WCKau  CLMarsden  PA Structural organization of the human neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene (NOS1).  J Biol Chem 1994;269 (52) 33082- 33090PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Reif  AJacob  CPRujescu  DHerterich  SLang  SGutknecht  LBaehne  CGStrobel  AFreitag  CMGiegling  IRomanos  MHartmann  ARösler  MRenner  TJFallgatter  AJRetz  WEhlis  ACLesch  KP Influence of functional variant of neuronal nitric oxide synthase on impulsive behaviors in humans.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66 (1) 41- 50PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Jensen  LJKuhn  MStark  MChaffron  SCreevey  CMuller  JDoerks  TJulien  PRoth  ASimonovic  MBork  Pvon Mering  C STRING 8: a global view on proteins and their functional interactions in 630 organisms.  Nucleic Acids Res 2009;37 (database issue) D412- D416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Salwinski  LMiller  CSSmith  AJPettit  FKBowie  JUEisenberg  D The Database of Interacting Proteins: 2004 update.  Nucleic Acids Res 2004;32 (database issue) D449- D451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Hermjakob  HMontecchi-Palazzi  LLewington  CMudali  SKerrien  SOrchard  SVingron  MRoechert  BRoepstorff  PValencia  AMargalit  HArmstrong  JBairoch  ACesareni  GSherman  DApweiler  R IntAct: an open source molecular interaction database.  Nucleic Acids Res 2004;32 (database issue) D452- D455PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Bader  GDDonaldson  IWolting  COuellette  BFPawson  THogue  CW BIND: the Biomolecular Interaction Network Database.  Nucleic Acids Res 2001;29 (1) 242- 245PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Kawashima  ROhnuma  TShibata  NArai  H No genetic association between postsynaptic density-95 gene polymorphisms and schizophrenia.  Neurosci Lett 2006;400 (1-2) 168- 171PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Tsai  SJHong  CJCheng  CYLiao  DLLiou  YJ Association study of polymorphisms in post-synaptic density protein 95 (PSD-95) with schizophrenia.  J Neural Transm 2007;114 (4) 423- 426PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Zheng  YLi  HQin  WChen  WDuan  YXiao  YLi  CZhang  JLi  XFeng  GHe  L Association of the carboxyl-terminal PDZ ligand of neuronal nitric oxide synthase gene with schizophrenia in the Chinese Han population.  Biochem Biophys Res Commun 2005;328 (4) 809- 815PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
Puri  V McQuillin  AThirumalai  SLawrence  JKrasucki  RChoudhury  KDatta  SKerwin  SQuested  DBass  NPimm  JLamb  GMoorey  HKandasami  GBadacsonyi  AKelly  KMorgan  JPunukollu  BNadeem  HCurtis  DGurling  HM Failure to confirm allelic association between markers at the CAPON gene locus and schizophrenia in a British sample.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (2) 195- 197PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
Fang  CTang  WTang  RQWang  LZhou  GQHuang  KLi  XWFeng  GYHe  MDu  LZZhu  SMXing  YLSang  HHe  LShi  YY Family-based association studies of CAPON and schizophrenia in the Chinese Han population.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008;32 (5) 1210- 1213PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
de Quervain  DJPapassotiropoulos  A Identification of a genetic cluster influencing memory performance and hippocampal activity in humans.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103 (11) 4270- 4274PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
86.
Huentelman  MJPapassotiropoulos  ACraig  DWHoerndli  FJPearson  JVHuynh  KDCorneveaux  JHänggi  JMondadori  CRBuchmann  AReiman  EMHenke  Kde Quervain  DJStephan  DA Calmodulin-binding transcription activator 1 (CAMTA1) alleles predispose human episodic memory performance.  Hum Mol Genet 2007;16 (12) 1469- 1477PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Meyer-Lindenberg  AWeinberger  DR Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7 (10) 818- 827PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Barnett  JHSalmond  CHJones  PBSahakian  BJ Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (8) 1053- 1064PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Crow  TJDone  DJSacker  A Childhood precursors of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins.  Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;245 (2) 61- 69PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Kremen  WSBuka  SLSeidman  LJGoldstein  JMKoren  DTsuang  MT IQ decline during childhood and adult psychotic symptoms in a community sample: a 19-year longitudinal study.  Am J Psychiatry 1998;155 (5) 672- 677PubMedGoogle Scholar
91.
Cannon  MCaspi  AMoffitt  TEHarrington  HTaylor  AMurray  RMPoulton  R Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (5) 449- 456PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
David  ASMalmberg  ABrandt  LAllebeck  PLewis  G IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study.  Psychol Med 1997;27 (6) 1311- 1323PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Davidson  MReichenberg  ARabinowitz  JWeiser  MKaplan  ZMark  M Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents.  Am J Psychiatry 1999;156 (9) 1328- 1335PubMedGoogle Scholar
94.
Gunnell  DHarrison  GRasmussen  FFouskakis  DTynelius  P Associations between premorbid intellectual performance, early-life exposures and early-onset schizophrenia: cohort study.  Br J Psychiatry 2002;181298- 305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Reichenberg  AWeiser  MRabinowitz  JCaspi  ASchmeidler  JMark  MKaplan  ZDavidson  M A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder.  Am J Psychiatry 2002;159 (12) 2027- 2035PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Reichenberg  AWeiser  MRapp  MARabinowitz  JCaspi  ASchmeidler  JKnobler  HYLubin  GNahon  DHarvey  PDDavidson  M Elaboration on premorbid intellectual performance in schizophrenia: premorbid intellectual decline and risk for schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (12) 1297- 1304PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×