[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Schematic diagram of a single trial in a visual backward masking task. On each trial, participants identified the location of a gap in a target (up, bottom, or left) by pressing a corresponding button with their dominant hand. Stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was varied to manipulate the strength of the masking effect. The beginning of each visual backward masking trial was signaled by two 100-millisecond flashes of a fixation point, followed by a 600-millisecond blank period. A target was briefly presented for 26.6 milliseconds, followed by a 53.3-millisecond mask at one of the specified SOAs (26.6, 40, 80, and 200 milliseconds) and then an intertrial interval (ITI).

Schematic diagram of a single trial in a visual backward masking task. On each trial, participants identified the location of a gap in a target (up, bottom, or left) by pressing a corresponding button with their dominant hand. Stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was varied to manipulate the strength of the masking effect. The beginning of each visual backward masking trial was signaled by two 100-millisecond flashes of a fixation point, followed by a 600-millisecond blank period. A target was briefly presented for 26.6 milliseconds, followed by a 53.3-millisecond mask at one of the specified SOAs (26.6, 40, 80, and 200 milliseconds) and then an intertrial interval (ITI).

Figure 2. 
Behavioral performance on backward masking task. The mean (SE) performance of patients and controls is shown for the 4 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) (ie, decreased masking) corresponding to increased accuracy. Both groups showed clearer upward slopes with increasing SOAs (ie, decreased masking) and the groups did not differ significantly at any SOA. Chance performance is 33%.

Behavioral performance on backward masking task. The mean (SE) performance of patients and controls is shown for the 4 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) (ie, decreased masking) corresponding to increased accuracy. Both groups showed clearer upward slopes with increasing SOAs (ie, decreased masking) and the groups did not differ significantly at any SOA. Chance performance is 33%.

Figure 3. 
Visual processing localizers. The 3 visual processing regions of interest were functionally defined for each subject, and this figure shows the activation for each area for a representative subject at 3 different planes. Areas in blue are retinotopic areas; areas in green indicate the human motion selective cortex (hMT+); and areas in red are within the lateral occipital complex.

Visual processing localizers. The 3 visual processing regions of interest were functionally defined for each subject, and this figure shows the activation for each area for a representative subject at 3 different planes. Areas in blue are retinotopic areas; areas in green indicate the human motion selective cortex (hMT+); and areas in red are within the lateral occipital complex.

Figure 4. 
Time series of signal change for each region of interest in patients and controls. A and B, Retinotopic areas. C and D, The human motion selective cortex (hMT+). E and F, Lateral occipital complex. SOA indicates stimulus onset asynchrony. Values are presented as mean (SE).

Time series of signal change for each region of interest in patients and controls. A and B, Retinotopic areas. C and D, The human motion selective cortex (hMT+). E and F, Lateral occipital complex. SOA indicates stimulus onset asynchrony. Values are presented as mean (SE).

Figure 5. 
Comparison of group on peak signal change for regions of interest. This figure shows the peak signal change in hemodynamic response for both patient (SZ) and control (CO) groups separated by stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Each panel reflects 1 region of interest. A, Retinotopic areas. B, The human motion selective cortex (hMT+). C, Lateral occipital complex (LO). Values are presented as mean (SE).

Comparison of group on peak signal change for regions of interest. This figure shows the peak signal change in hemodynamic response for both patient (SZ) and control (CO) groups separated by stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Each panel reflects 1 region of interest. A, Retinotopic areas. B, The human motion selective cortex (hMT+). C, Lateral occipital complex (LO). Values are presented as mean (SE).

Table. 
Activated Brain Regions
Activated Brain Regions
1.
Breitmeyer  BG Visual Masking: An Integrative Approach.  New York, NY Oxford University Press1984;
2.
Breitmeyer  BGOgmen  H Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: a comparison, review, and update.  Percept Psychophys 2000;62 (8) 1572- 1595PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Cadenhead  KSSerper  YBraff  DL Transient versus sustained visual channels in the visual backward masking deficits of schizophrenia patients.  Biol Psychiatry 1998;43 (2) 132- 138PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania: specifying the visual channels.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51 (12) 945- 951PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania: specifying a mechanism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51 (12) 939- 944PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Rund  BR Backward-masking performance in chronic and nonchronic schizophrenics, affectively disturbed patients, and normal control subjects.  J Abnorm Psychol 1993;102 (1) 74- 81PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Saccuzzo  DPSchubert  DL Backward masking as a measure of slow processing in schizophrenia spectrum disorders.  J Abnorm Psychol 1981;90 (4) 305- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Saccuzzo  DPBraff  DL Information-processing abnormalities: trait- and state-dependent components.  Schizophr Bull 1986;12 (3) 447- 459PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Saccuzzo  DPBraff  DL Early information processing deficit in schizophrenia: new findings using schizophrenic subgroups and manic control subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981;38 (2) 175- 179PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Sergi  MJRassovsky  YNuechterlein  KHGreen  MF Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 2006;163 (3) 448- 454PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Miller  SSaccuzzo  DBraff  D Information processing deficits in remitted schizophrenics.  J Abnorm Psychol 1979;88 (4) 446- 449PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Backward masking in unmedicated schizophrenic patients in psychotic remission: possible reflections of aberrant cortical oscillations.  Am J Psychiatry 1999;156 (9) 1367- 1373PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Bedwell  JSBrown  JMMiller  S The magnocellular visual system and schizophrenia: what can the color red tell us?  Schizophr Res 2003;63 (3) 273- 284PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  B Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients: evidence for a vulnerability indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54 (5) 465- 472PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Forward and backward visual masking in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (5) 446- 451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Kéri  SKelemen  OBenedek  GJanka  Z Different trait markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a neurocognitive approach.  Psychol Med 2001;31 (5) 915- 922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Lieb  KDenz  EHess  RSchuttler  RKornhuber  HHSchreiber  H Preattentive information processing as measured by backward masking and texton detection tasks in adolescents at high genetic risk for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 1996;21 (3) 171- 182PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Rassovsky  YGreen  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Modulation of attention during visual masking in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 2005;162 (8) 1533- 1535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BTsuang  JMintz  J Forward and backward visual masking in schizophrenia: influence of age.  Psychol Med 2003;33 (5) 887- 895PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  MWalker  E Symptom correlates of vulnerability to backward masking in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 1986;143 (2) 181- 186PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Slaghuis  WLCurran  CE Spatial frequency masking in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia.  J Abnorm Psychol 1999;108 (1) 42- 50PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Butler  PDZemon  VSchechter  ISaperstein  AMHoptman  MJLim  KORevheim  NSilipo  GJavitt  DC Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (5) 495- 504PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Van Essen  DCAnderson  CHFelleman  DJ Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective.  Science 1992;255 (5043) 419- 423PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Breitmeyer  BGGanz  L Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing.  Psychol Rev 1976;83 (1) 1- 36PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Schechter  IButler  PDSilipo  GZemon  VJavitt  DC Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to backward masking dysfunction in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2003;64 (2-3) 91- 101PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Schuck  JRLee  RG Backward masking, information processing, and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull 1989;15 (3) 491- 500PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Green  MFMintz  JSalveson  DNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BLight  GABraff  DL Visual masking as a probe for abnormal gamma range activity in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (12) 1113- 1119PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Wynn  JKLight  GABreitmeyer  BNuechterlein  KHGreen  MF Event-related gamma activity in schizophrenia patients during a visual backward masking task.  Am J Psychiatry 2005;162 (12) 2330- 2336PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Purushothaman  GOgmen  HBedell  HE Gamma-range oscillations in backward masking functions and their putative neural correlates.  Psychol Rev 2000;107 (3) 556- 577PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bar  MTootell  RBSchacter  DLGreve  DNFischl  BMendola  JDRosen  BRDale  AM Cortical mechanisms specific to explicit visual object recognition.  Neuron 2001;29 (2) 529- 535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Grill-Spector  KKushnir  THendler  TMalach  R The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans.  Nat Neurosci 2000;3 (8) 837- 843PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Carlson  TARauschenberger  RVerstraten  FA No representation without awareness in the lateral occipital cortex.  Psychol Sci 2007;18 (4) 298- 302PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Haynes  JDDriver  JRees  G Visibility reflects dynamic changes of effective connectivity between V1 and fusiform cortex.  Neuron 2005;46 (5) 811- 821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Green  MFGlahn  DEngel  SANuechterlein  KHSabb  FStrojwas  MCohen  MS Regional brain activity associated with visual backward masking.  J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17 (1) 13- 23PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition.  New York Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute1997;
36.
First  MBGibbon  MSpitzer  RLWilliams  JBWBenjamin  L Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders.  New York Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute1996;
37.
Buxton  R-BLiu  TTMartinez  AFrank  LRLuh  WMWong  E-C Sorting out event-related paradigms in fMRI: the distinction between detecting an activation and estimating the hemodynamic response.  Neuroimage 2000;11S457Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Liu  TT Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related fMRI with multiple trial types, II: design of experiments.  Neuroimage 2004;21 (1) 401- 413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Liu  TTFrank  LR Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related FMRI with multiple trial types, part I: theory.  Neuroimage 2004;21 (1) 387- 400PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Dale  AM Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI.  Hum Brain Mapp 1999;8 (2-3) 109- 114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Wynn  JKGreen  MFEngel  SKorb  ALee  JGlahn  DNuechterlein  KHCohen  MS Increased extent of object-sensitivity cortex in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2008;16497- 105Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Malach  RReppas  JBBenson  RRKwong  KKJiang  HKennedy  WALedden  PJBrady  TJRosen  BRTootell  RB Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1995;92 (18) 8135- 8139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Grill-Spector  KMalach  R The human visual cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci 2004;27649- 677PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Malach  RLevy  IHasson  U The topography of high-order human object areas.  Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6 (4) 176- 184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Engel  SAGlover  GHWandell  BA Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI.  Cereb Cortex 1997;7 (2) 181- 192PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hasson  ULevy  IBehrmann  MHendler  TMalach  R Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas.  Neuron 2002;34 (3) 479- 490PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Butler  PDSilverstein  SMDakin  SC Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;64 (1) 40- 47PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Doniger  GMFoxe  JJMurray  MMHiggins  BAJavitt  DC Impaired visual object recognition and dorsal/ventral stream interaction in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (11) 1011- 1020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Foxe  JJMurray  MMJavitt  DC Filling-in in schizophrenia: a high-density electrical mapping and source-analysis investigation of illusory contour processing.  Cereb Cortex 2005;15 (12) 1914- 1927PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Vinberg  JGrill-Spector  K Representation of shapes, edges, and surfaces across multiple cues in the human visual cortex.  J Neurophysiol 2008;99 (3) 1380- 1393PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Enns  JTDi Lollo  VDi Lollo V, What's new in visual masking?  Trends Cogn Sci 2000;4 (9) 345- 352PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Enns  JT Object substitution and its relation to other forms of visual masking.  Vision Res 2004;44 (12) 1321- 1331PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Merigan  WHMaunsell  JH How parallel are the primate visual pathways?  Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16369- 402PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
December 2009

Functional Neuroanatomy of Visual Masking Deficits in Schizophrenia

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior (Drs Green, Lee, Cohen, Nuechterlein, and Wynn and Mr Korb) and Department of Psychology (Dr Nuechterlein), University of California, Los Angeles, and Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System (Drs Green, Lee, and Wynn); Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis (Dr Engel); and Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Institute of Living, and Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut (Dr Glahn).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1295-1303. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.132
Abstract

Context  Visual masking procedures assess the earliest stages of visual processing. Patients with schizophrenia reliably show deficits on visual masking, and these procedures have been used to explore vulnerability to schizophrenia, probe underlying neural circuits, and help explain functional outcome.

Objective  To identify and compare regional brain activity associated with one form of visual masking (ie, backward masking) in schizophrenic patients and healthy controls.

Design  Subjects received functional magnetic resonance imaging scans. While in the scanner, subjects performed a backward masking task and were given 3 functional localizer activation scans to identify early visual processing regions of interest (ROIs).

Setting  University of California, Los Angeles, and the Department of Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System.

Participants  Nineteen patients with schizophrenia and 19 healthy control subjects.

Main Outcome Measure  The magnitude of the functional magnetic resonance imaging signal during backward masking.

Results  Two ROIs (lateral occipital complex [LO] and the human motion selective cortex [hMT+]) showed sensitivity to the effects of masking, meaning that signal in these areas increased as the target became more visible. Patients had lower activation than controls in LO across all levels of visibility but did not differ in other visual processing ROIs. Using whole-brain analyses, we also identified areas outside the ROIs that were sensitive to masking effects (including bilateral inferior parietal lobe and thalamus), but groups did not differ in signal magnitude in these areas.

Conclusions  The study results support a key role in LO for visual masking, consistent with previous studies in healthy controls. The current results indicate that patients fail to activate LO to the same extent as controls during visual processing regardless of stimulus visibility, suggesting a neural basis for the visual masking deficit, and possibly other visual integration deficits, in schizophrenia.

Much can be learned about schizophrenia as early as the first tenth of a second of visual processing. In a visual masking paradigm, the visibility of a visual target is disrupted by the presence of a mask that occurs briefly before or after the target.1,2 Many studies over the years have demonstrated that schizophrenic patients perform more poorly than comparison subjects in identifying the target in the presence of a mask.3-9 Visual masking in schizophrenia may provide a window into neural substrates, clinical symptoms, and vulnerability. Also, the degree of impairment may be associated with community functioning.10

The deficits in backward masking (ie, masking in which the mask appears after the target) in schizophrenia have generated interest for several reasons. First, performance on visual masking procedures may be a trait marker that reflects vulnerability to schizophrenia. Masking deficits have been reported in patients who are in clinical remission11,12 and, to a reduced degree, in first-degree relatives of patients,13-17 suggesting that visual masking deficits are related to genetic vulnerability to illness. Second, they represent a narrowly defined early perceptual process for which effortful attentional modulation has minimal effects.18 We have found visual masking impairment even after individuals are equated for sensory input of the target, and the groups did not differ in the contrast levels needed for target detection.19 Third, backward masking deficits have been related to clinical aspects of schizophrenia, including negative symptoms.20,21 Fourth, initial evidence suggests that deficits on backward masking tasks are related to functional outcome, probably through key mediating variables. For example, visual masking has been found to be associated with performance on social cognitive tasks, which in turn are related to community functioning.10 Similar findings have been reported for other early visual processing tasks in schizophrenia.22

A fifth reason for interest in backward masking paradigms is their detailed linkages to underlying neural systems.1,2 The visual masking deficits have been viewed in terms of the interactions of 2 key visual channels that form the basis for complex visual processing and are labeled in functional (transient and sustained) or anatomical (magnocellular and parvocellular) terms.23 The magnocellular pathway is characterized by fast phasic (transient) responses, while the parvocellular pathway has slower tonic (sustained) responses. Thus, magnocellular channels respond rapidly and briefly to stimulus onset, offset, and location, whereas parvocellular channels have a longer response related to stimulus identification. According to this model of masking, parvocellular activity elicited by the target conveys information needed to identify targets and masking results from disruption of this target information by either the magnocellular or parvocellular pathways of the mask, depending on the particular paradigm.1,24 Deficits in masking in schizophrenia have been attributed to abnormalities in the magnocellular pathway.3,4,25,26 However, there is also interest in possible schizophrenia-related abnormalities in high-frequency brain electrical activity (ie, gamma range, >30 Hz) that is associated with formation of visual representations.27,28 In backward masking models, this gamma activity is presumed to occur in the parvocellular system.29

Although the very brief temporal demands of backward masking present challenges for its application in the scanner, several recent studies have successfully examined masking paradigms with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in healthy samples.30-33 While fMRI does not have the temporal resolution to examine the blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) response to the target and mask separately, it can yield an informative BOLD response to the target-mask sequence. There have been no studies, to our knowledge, on the neural basis of visual masking in schizophrenia using any functional neuroimaging procedure. Hence, the regional brain activity associated with these key deficits is not known. We previously examined visual masking in a group of healthy subjects to determine which areas are sensitive to masking effects (ie, show more activation with increasing duration between target and mask).34 Among the early visual processing areas identified a priori through localizer scans, the lateral occipital complex (LO) showed strong sensitivity to masking effects. Hence, LO was of particular interest as a possible substrate of the masking effect. Other areas showed similar effects on post hoc analyses, including inferior parietal lobe, anterior cingulate cortex, and thalamus.

For the current study, we used an approach similar to our previous study34 in which we first examined well-defined early visual processing areas for which there are established localizer tasks. We refer to these regions as a priori regions of interest (ROIs). Because areas outside of these early visual regions may be involved with masking, and may explain group differences, we also conducted exploratory whole-brain analyses (ie, post hoc analyses). We included samples of patients with schizophrenia (n = 19) and healthy controls (n = 19) to address several questions: (1) Among the visual processing areas identified a priori as ROIs, which show sensitivity to the effects of visual masking? 2) Of these ROIs, which show differences between patients and controls in level of activation? (3) Do other areas (identified post hoc) show sensitivity to masking effects? (4) Do patients and controls differ in activation on any of these areas?

Methods
Participants

Twenty-four patients with schizophrenia (5 female) and 19 healthy controls (5 female) were recruited for this study. Participants were recruited from a larger National Institute of Mental Health study of early visual processing in schizophrenia.4,5,15 Schizophrenic patients were recruited from outpatient treatment clinics at the Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System and from local board and care facilities. Schizophrenic patients met diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Axis I Disorders.35 Exclusion criteria for patients included (1) substance abuse or dependence in the last 6 months, (2) mental retardation based on review of medical records, (3) history of loss of consciousness for more than 1 hour, (4) an identifiable neurological disorder, and (5) not sufficiently fluent in English. We did not exclude patients for a history of substance dependence.

Normal control participants were recruited through flyers posted in the local community, newspaper advertisements in local newspapers, and Web site postings. Exclusion criteria for control participants included (1) history of schizophrenia or other psychotic disorder, bipolar disorder, recurrent depression, history of substance dependence, or any substance abuse in the last 6 months based on the SCID,35 (2) any of the following Axis II disorders: avoidant, paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal, or borderline, based on the SCID for Axis II disorders,36 (3) schizophrenia or other psychotic disorder in a first-degree relative, (4) any significant neurological disorder or head injury, and (5) not sufficiently fluent in English.

Schizophrenic patients and normal controls were comparable in terms of age and education (mean [SD], age, 38.2 [11.8] years and 42.7 [9.0] years, respectively; t36 = −1.33; P = .19; education, 13.6 [1.6] years and 13.2 [1.3] years, respectively; t36 = 0.95; P = .34). All patients were clinically stable at the time of testing and exhibited low levels of symptoms (for a more complete characterization of the patient sample for the larger project, see Sergi et al10). All of the patients were medicated (conventional antipsychotic, n = 1; single atypical antipsychotic, n = 16; 2 atypical antipsychotics, n = 2). Subjects did not take any sedatives during the 12 hours prior to assessment. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision, and we did not select subjects on handedness.

All SCID interviewers were trained to a minimum κ of 0.75 for key psychotic and mood items through the Treatment Unit of the Department of Veterans Affairs VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center. None of the research staff were blind to group membership. All participants were evaluated for the capacity to give informed consent and provided written informed consent after all procedures were fully explained, according to procedures approved by the institutional review boards at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), and the Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System.

Design and procedure

All participants completed 6 runs of the visual backward masking task followed by 3 localizer tasks (LO, retinotopic areas, and the human motion selective cortex [hMT+]) in the scanner. The entire scanning session lasted 60 minutes. The visual backward masking task was presented using E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) and the localizer tasks were presented using MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, Massachusetts). All tasks were presented with magnetic resonance–compatible LCD goggles (Resonance Technology, Northridge, California).

For the visual backward masking task, the target was a square with a gap on 1 of 3 sides (up, down, or left) that appeared at the center of the screen. The mask was a composite square made up of 4 smaller squares, overlapping the area occupied by the target (Figure 1). A mask was presented at 1 of 4 possible stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs). These SOAs and stimuli durations were decided on after piloting with the magnetic resonance–compatible goggles. The beginning of each visual backward masking trial was signaled by two 100-millisecond flashes of a fixation point, followed by a 600-millisecond blank period. The target duration was held constant at 26.6 milliseconds and followed by a 53.3-millisecond mask at 1 of 4 possible SOAs: 26.6, 40, 80, and 200 milliseconds. The target subtended 5.7° and the mask 10.2° of visual angle. Participants were instructed to identify the location of a gap in the target (up, bottom, or left) by pressing a corresponding button with their dominant hand.

The visual backward masking tasks consisted of 6 runs, each with thirty 5-second trials (ie, 6 trials for each of the 4 SOAs and 6 null trials that included fixation but no stimuli). We used a rapid event-related design and the trials were presented in a permuted block design to maximize both hemodynamic response function estimation and signal detection power.37-39 To create trial sequences for each run, we first determined a random ordering of blocks, in which blocks contained 3 trials from a single condition (ie, 1 of the 4 SOAs or null). Then, we randomly swapped trials in the sequence until the first-order entropy of the sequence increased by a factor of 1.33. This procedure made the trial sequence appear random, and it was not easy for subjects to form expectations about the upcoming trial type. To generate the final sequence, this process was repeated 500 times and the trial sequence with the highest statistical efficiencies was selected.40

After the visual backward masking task, participants performed 3 functional localization tasks: LO, retinotopic areas, and hMT+. Full descriptions of the 3 functional localizer tasks are provided elsewhere41 and are summarized briefly herein. The LO localizer task consisted of alternating blocked presentations of pictures of abstract objects (ie, sculptures) and scrambled pictures of objects, with each block containing 10 images presented for a total of 12.5 seconds.42,43 The scrambled pictures were created by dividing each picture into 64 squares and randomly scrambling their positions. There were 6 blocks each of abstract objects and scrambled objects. Our definition of LO follows that of Malach and colleagues44 and includes lateral cortex between V3 and the medial temporal lobe (MT+) but also continues into adjacent ventral cortex. To identify retinotopic areas, participants viewed slowly rotating wedges of a contrast-reversing checkerboard.45 The wedge made 5 rotations, with 1 rotation every 30 seconds. Retinotopic areas that respond well to the standard fMRI techniques for measuring retinotopic organization include V1, V2, ventral V3 (or VP), V3a, and V4. The motion-sensitive hMT+ localizer task consisted of alternating blocked presentations of moving rings and stationary rings, with each block presented for 15 seconds. There were 5 blocks each of moving and stationary rings.

fMRI DATA ACQUISITION

All scanning was conducted on a 3-T scanner (Siemens Allegra, Erlangen, Germany) located in the UCLA Ahmanson-Lovelace Brain Mapping Center. For anatomical reference, a high-resolution, echo planar, axial, T2-weighted series was obtained for each subject prior to functional scanning (repetition time = 6000 milliseconds, echo time = 54 milliseconds, flip angle = 90°, 30 axial slices, field of view = 20 cm). A T2*-weighted gradient-echo sequence was used to detect BOLD signal (repetition time = 2000 milliseconds, echo time = 42 milliseconds, flip angle = 80°, voxel size of 3.125 × 3.125 × 4.00 mm with a 1-mm gap), acquiring 24 slices parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure plane.

fMRI DATA ANALYSIS

A detailed description of the fMRI data analysis is shown in the eText. In brief, we approached the fMRI data analyses in 2 complementary ways. First, we used an ROI-based approach in which we (1) identified 3 key visual processing areas separately for each subject using the functional localizer tasks, (2) examined the effect of SOA (that is, masking effect) for each ROI, and (3) examined between-group differences at each ROI. Second, we conducted a whole-brain analysis in which we (1) identified clusters of voxels that were sensitive to SOA (masking) effects outside the 3 ROIs and (2) compared the 2 groups on these areas. Analyses were performed using automated procedures that did not involve user-adjusted parameters; thus, the experimenter was not blinded to the subjects' group assignments.

Results

Five patients were excluded from analyses: 1 subject had excessive movement artifact due to tardive dyskinesia, 2 had technical problems during scanning, and 2 showed chance-level performance (defined as ≤38% accuracy) at the longest SOA (200 milliseconds). Therefore, 19 schizophrenic patients (4 female) and 19 normal controls (5 female) were included in the following analyses.

Performance data

Behavioral data across SOAs are shown in Figure 2. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) yielded a significant effect of SOA (F1,36 = 350.9; P < .001). Neither the main effect of group nor the group × SOA interaction was significant. Performance was comparable at all points except SOA3. Controls performed 0.50 effect size better than patients at this SOA but the difference was not significant at these sample sizes. As expected, both schizophrenic patients and normal controls showed improved performance as SOAs increased (ie, the masking became weaker). Because the performance data for SOA1 and SOA2 were close to chance performance for both groups, we combined the responses for these SOAs in all subsequent analyses.

Roi-based analysis

There were no significant differences (all P values >.20) between groups in the number of voxels for any of the ROIs (Figure 3) (mean [SD], retinotopic areas: controls, 360 [46]; patients, 380 [50]; hMT+: controls 57 [11]; patients, 59 [12]; LO: controls, 34 [12]; patients, 56 [11]).

The time series of signal change for each ROI are presented for patients and controls in Figure 4. Our initial question was whether any of the ROIs showed sensitivity to masking (ie, an SOA effect at the middle time points) across groups. We conducted a 3 (SOA) × 7 (time points) × 2 (group) ANOVA separately for each ROI. Our interest was whether there was (1) an SOA × time point interaction or (2) an SOA × group interaction. Each ROI had a significant SOA × time point interaction (F12,396 = 2.54; P < .05; F12,384 = 1.92; P < .05; and F12,384 = 2.68; P < .05 for retinotopic areas, hMT+, and LO, respectively). None of the ROIs had a significant SOA × group interaction, so we combined groups and conducted post hoc ANOVAs at each time for each ROI to better understand the SOA × time point interaction. For retinotopic areas (Figure 4A and B), there was no significant effect of SOA at any time point. For hMT+ (Figure 4C and D), significant SOA effects were present at time points 6 and 8 (F2,66 = 3.23; P < .05 and F2,66 = 4.04; P < .05, respectively). For LO (Figure 4E and F), we also found significant SOA effects at time points 6 and 8 (F2,66 = 6.49; P < .01 and F2,66 = 5.28; P < .01, respectively).

While the SOA effect for LO was predicted, the one for hMT+ was not. To determine whether the SOA effect for LO was greater than that of hMT+, we conducted a 3 (SOAs) × 2 (LO and hMT+) repeated-measures ANOVA for time points 6 and 8 separately. For time point 6, we found significant main effects of ROI (F1,29 = 10.40; P < .01) and SOA (F2,58 = 5.49; P < .01). Notably, the ROI × SOA interaction was significant (F2,58 = 3.27; P < .05). Hence, LO showed more activation overall and a clearer SOA effect (more activation with increasing visibility of the target) compared with hMT+ (Figure 4C-F). For time point 8, we found a significant effect of ROI (larger for LO, F1,29 = 7.77; P < .01) and a significant effect of SOA (F2,58 = 4.81; P < .05), but not a significant ROI × SOA interaction.

Next, we considered any differences between groups by comparing their peak amplitude of percent signal change (ie, peak signal change) for each ROI (Figure 5). For retinotopic areas and hMT+, none of the main effects or interactions were significant. The groups were generally comparable in peak signal change for these ROIs (P values = .59 and .37 for retinotopic areas and hMT+, respectively). For LO, we found a significant group effect (F1,32 = 11.74; P < .01) and effect of SOA (F2,64 = 3.42; P < .05); the SOA × group interaction was not significant. Hence, patients showed lower peak signal change on LO across SOAs, but a similar SOA effect, compared with controls.

Some authors have separated out the dorsal and ventral portions of LO, with a particular interest in ventral LO for object representation.34,46 As an additional check on the findings, we examined the ventral portion of LO only. The results were essentially the same as for the entire LO: this region showed a clear effect of SOA and the patients showed lower amplitude responses (eFigure 1).

Whole-brain analysis

For the whole-brain analyses, we first identified voxels that showed a parametric increase of SOA1 + 2 less than SOA3 less than SOA4. With both patients and controls combined, the activated brain regions included posterior cingulate cortex, right postcentral gyrus, bilateral inferior parietal lobe, bilateral superior temporal gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus, left inferior temporal gyrus, bilateral fusiform gyrus, and thalamus (Table) (eFigure 2). A second analysis compared activity levels in patients and controls but found no reliable regions of differential activation.

Comment

The current study used 2 different approaches (an a priori ROI approach with localizer scans and whole-brain analyses) to examine the neural basis of visual masking in schizophrenia. Of the visual processing areas that were defined a priori through localizer scans, both LO and hMT+ showed sensitivity to the effects of masking, meaning that the activation increased as the SOAs became longer and the target more visible. The masking effect was significantly more pronounced in LO than in hMT+ at the 6-second time point. Compared with controls, patients showed lower peak signal change in LO, but not in the other visual processing areas. We identified areas outside of the visual processing areas that were sensitive to the effects of masking, including some areas that were implicated in our previous study (bilateral inferior parietal lobe and thalamus), but the groups did not differ in their activation of these areas.

The results of this study provide additional support for a key role in LO for visual masking, which is consistent with our previous study34 and with other fMRI studies of visual masking in controls.31,32 The current results also indicate that patients fail to activate LO to the same extent as controls across SOAs. The groups showed a nonsignificant 0.50–effect size difference on SOA3, so we computed the correlation between accuracy and peak signal change on LO at this SOA in each group. The correlation was moderate but nonsignificant in normal controls (r = 0.34) and small in schizophrenic patients (r = 0.09).

The blunted LO response was a general effect, observed for both short SOAs, where masking rendered the stimulus almost invisible, and the longest SOA, where masking had little effect. This pattern suggests that patients have generally weaker stimulus representations. Such a response from LO could not be inferred from behavioral masking studies of schizophrenia, in which it is common for patients to differ from controls at midrange SOAs (ie, those without floor and ceiling effects). However, with fMRI it is possible to see a deficit across a range of visibility. Because of its generality, the blunted LO response could reveal itself as reduced performance in other tasks that depend heavily on precise readout of LO activity. Schizophrenia is associated with impairment on a range of visual integration tasks, including object recognition, grouping, perceptual closure, face processing, and reading,47 and deficits on any of these could be associated with a blunted LO response.

Other studies have used electrophysiology to suggest LO abnormalities in schizophrenia.48,49 For example, some have examined an event-related potential, closure negativity (Ncl), that is associated with object recognition. This wave has peak amplitude at approximately 290 milliseconds and is distributed over the lateral occipital scalp regions. Hence, its distribution would include region LO assessed in the current study. Reports have found that this wave differs in magnitude48 or pattern49 in patients with schizophrenia, thereby providing indirect, but consistent, support for LO abnormalities in schizophrenia.

What processes might be occurring in LO? One probable function of LO is to integrate different types of information from a variety of sources to provide a unified representation of visual shape.50 The reduced LO activity we observed in patients may indicate that patients have a weaker unified representation, at least under conditions of masking. Because LO dysfunction appears to be a key neural source of masking effects,30-33 we propose that patients' weakened LO representations underlie the masking impairment observed in laboratory studies. It is possible that other brain areas supply a compensatory response for the weakened LO representation; however, the whole-brain analyses did not reveal group differences in regions outside LO that would suggest such compensation.

Patients' increased susceptibility in masking could originate from either feed-forward or reentrant processing in LO. Visual masking has typically been interpreted in terms of feed-forward models of visual processing (ie, involving the flow of information from retina to cortex). In contrast to this model, more recent advances in cognitive neuroscience argue for the importance of reentrant (top-down or attentional) processes in early components of visual processing.51,52 Reentrant models of visual processing emphasize the iterative processing of visual stimuli within the cortex. One recent study used fMRI to examine a type of backward masking (4-dot masking) that is thought to involve primarily masking by reentrant processes.32 The results supported the idea that reentrant processes disrupted the neural representation of the target in LO. Hence, LO appears to be a critical site for neural representation of the target. Although our masking methods were not designed to separate feed-forward from reentrant processes, it is entirely possible that group differences in LO could be attributable to disruption in reentrant processes.

In the present study, we intentionally defined each ROI in a relatively restricted way, based on the largest single cluster for each activation task for each individual. This is a conservative approach that yields smaller, but hopefully more functionally homogeneous, regions. A previous publication from our laboratory41 used a more liberal approach to define ROIs to examine the extent of activation with the localizer tasks in the absence of any visual masking procedures. In that study, patients differed from controls in having a significantly broader area of activation with the LO localizer stimuli, but not with retinotopic or hMT+ localizers, again suggesting that LO is a critical area to consider regarding group differences in visual perception. To ensure that the present findings were not a result of using a more restricted approach for defining ROIs, we reanalyzed the fMRI data using ROIs defined as in our previous study and found the same pattern of results with visual masking (ie, reduced LO activity in schizophrenic patients).

Patients and controls did not differ in performance in the scanner. On the surface, this appears to be inconsistent with performance data from many previous studies. The methods we have used in our published performance studies were optimized to show group differences. The target stimuli are small (0.24° of visual angle) and are shown at visual threshold so that all of the subjects are equated for initial sensory input. The backward masking methods in the present report were optimized to show a BOLD response. The stimuli were large (target = 5.7°, mask = 10.2°), dark with high contrast, and well above threshold for identification. Hence, the target in this fMRI study was roughly 24 times as large as in our performance laboratory studies. In addition, we used fewer trials and assessed fewer SOAs than in our typical behavioral paradigms. Although the absence of large performance differences is unusual in the context of the masking literature, it provides an interpretive advantage in that performance level is not a confound in considering group differences in regional brain activity.

It remains possible that the lack of significant performance difference indicates that the patients in this study are atypical. To examine this possibility, we evaluated data from a subset of patients and controls who were also examined on visual masking performance out of the scanner as part of the larger masking protocol. Although the small number of subjects (10 patients, 17 controls) precludes formal statistical analysis, the between-group effect sizes for a similar masking task (ie, high-energy identification) of these subjects were 0.05, 0.31, 0.61, and 0.73 for SOAs of 100, 125, 150, and 225 milliseconds, respectively. These effect sizes between groups were highly consistent with what is found in other studies when patients and controls are matched for sensory input values. Hence, we do not consider the patient sample to be unusual in this regard.

Although fMRI does not lend itself well to separate examination of the parvocellular and magnocellular visual pathways, limited inferences can be made based on the input of these pathways to visual processing regions.23 Area MT is considered part of the dorsal pathway that receives predominant input from the magnocellular system, whereas LO is considered part of the ventral pathway and receives input from both the magnocellular and parvocellular systems.53 Hence, the current findings of abnormalities in LO are not inconsistent with previous evidence for a magnocellular abnormality in schizophrenia.3,4,25,26

Assuming that LO is a critical site for neural representation of a visual target, and that it underlies visual masking as well as other visual processing deficits in schizophrenia, our data raise questions about other consequences of a dysfunctional LO. For example, could abnormalities in this region contribute in part to the functional disability in schizophrenia? This view, while clearly speculative, would fit with behavioral performance findings that early visual processing tasks are associated with performance on social cognitive tasks, which are related to community functioning in schizophrenia.10,22 Hence, with sufficient samples it will be possible to test these speculations by using brain activation in selected ROIs as key variables in proposed pathways that run from brain to perception to basic and social cognition and eventually to functioning in schizophrenia.

Correspondence: Michael F. Green, PhD, 300 Medical Plaza, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095-6968 (mgreen@ucla.edu).

Submitted for Publication: December 1, 2008; final revision received February 27, 2009; accepted March 2, 2009.

Author Contributions: Dr Green takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analyses. All authors had full access to all of the data in the study.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: Support for this study came from National Institute of Mental Health grants MH43292 and MH065707 (principal investigator, Dr Green).

Additional Contributions: Poorang Nori and Alisa Malin assisted in data collection. For generous support of the UCLA Brain Mapping Center, we also thank the Brain Mapping Medical Research Organization, Brain Mapping Support Foundation, Pierson-Lovelace Foundation, The Ahmanson Foundation, William M. and Linda R. Dietel Philanthropic Fund at the Northern Piedmont Community Foundation, Tamkin Foundation, Jennifer Jones-Simon Foundation, Capital Group Companies Charitable Foundation, Robson Family, and Northstar Fund.

References
1.
Breitmeyer  BG Visual Masking: An Integrative Approach.  New York, NY Oxford University Press1984;
2.
Breitmeyer  BGOgmen  H Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: a comparison, review, and update.  Percept Psychophys 2000;62 (8) 1572- 1595PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Cadenhead  KSSerper  YBraff  DL Transient versus sustained visual channels in the visual backward masking deficits of schizophrenia patients.  Biol Psychiatry 1998;43 (2) 132- 138PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania: specifying the visual channels.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51 (12) 945- 951PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania: specifying a mechanism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51 (12) 939- 944PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Rund  BR Backward-masking performance in chronic and nonchronic schizophrenics, affectively disturbed patients, and normal control subjects.  J Abnorm Psychol 1993;102 (1) 74- 81PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Saccuzzo  DPSchubert  DL Backward masking as a measure of slow processing in schizophrenia spectrum disorders.  J Abnorm Psychol 1981;90 (4) 305- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Saccuzzo  DPBraff  DL Information-processing abnormalities: trait- and state-dependent components.  Schizophr Bull 1986;12 (3) 447- 459PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Saccuzzo  DPBraff  DL Early information processing deficit in schizophrenia: new findings using schizophrenic subgroups and manic control subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981;38 (2) 175- 179PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Sergi  MJRassovsky  YNuechterlein  KHGreen  MF Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 2006;163 (3) 448- 454PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Miller  SSaccuzzo  DBraff  D Information processing deficits in remitted schizophrenics.  J Abnorm Psychol 1979;88 (4) 446- 449PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Backward masking in unmedicated schizophrenic patients in psychotic remission: possible reflections of aberrant cortical oscillations.  Am J Psychiatry 1999;156 (9) 1367- 1373PubMedGoogle Scholar
13.
Bedwell  JSBrown  JMMiller  S The magnocellular visual system and schizophrenia: what can the color red tell us?  Schizophr Res 2003;63 (3) 273- 284PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  B Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients: evidence for a vulnerability indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54 (5) 465- 472PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Forward and backward visual masking in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (5) 446- 451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Kéri  SKelemen  OBenedek  GJanka  Z Different trait markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a neurocognitive approach.  Psychol Med 2001;31 (5) 915- 922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Lieb  KDenz  EHess  RSchuttler  RKornhuber  HHSchreiber  H Preattentive information processing as measured by backward masking and texton detection tasks in adolescents at high genetic risk for schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 1996;21 (3) 171- 182PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Rassovsky  YGreen  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BMintz  J Modulation of attention during visual masking in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 2005;162 (8) 1533- 1535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BTsuang  JMintz  J Forward and backward visual masking in schizophrenia: influence of age.  Psychol Med 2003;33 (5) 887- 895PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  MWalker  E Symptom correlates of vulnerability to backward masking in schizophrenia.  Am J Psychiatry 1986;143 (2) 181- 186PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Slaghuis  WLCurran  CE Spatial frequency masking in positive- and negative-symptom schizophrenia.  J Abnorm Psychol 1999;108 (1) 42- 50PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Butler  PDZemon  VSchechter  ISaperstein  AMHoptman  MJLim  KORevheim  NSilipo  GJavitt  DC Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (5) 495- 504PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Van Essen  DCAnderson  CHFelleman  DJ Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective.  Science 1992;255 (5043) 419- 423PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Breitmeyer  BGGanz  L Implications of sustained and transient channels for theories of visual pattern masking, saccadic suppression, and information processing.  Psychol Rev 1976;83 (1) 1- 36PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Schechter  IButler  PDSilipo  GZemon  VJavitt  DC Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to backward masking dysfunction in schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2003;64 (2-3) 91- 101PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Schuck  JRLee  RG Backward masking, information processing, and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Bull 1989;15 (3) 491- 500PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Green  MFMintz  JSalveson  DNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  BLight  GABraff  DL Visual masking as a probe for abnormal gamma range activity in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (12) 1113- 1119PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Wynn  JKLight  GABreitmeyer  BNuechterlein  KHGreen  MF Event-related gamma activity in schizophrenia patients during a visual backward masking task.  Am J Psychiatry 2005;162 (12) 2330- 2336PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Purushothaman  GOgmen  HBedell  HE Gamma-range oscillations in backward masking functions and their putative neural correlates.  Psychol Rev 2000;107 (3) 556- 577PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bar  MTootell  RBSchacter  DLGreve  DNFischl  BMendola  JDRosen  BRDale  AM Cortical mechanisms specific to explicit visual object recognition.  Neuron 2001;29 (2) 529- 535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Grill-Spector  KKushnir  THendler  TMalach  R The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans.  Nat Neurosci 2000;3 (8) 837- 843PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Carlson  TARauschenberger  RVerstraten  FA No representation without awareness in the lateral occipital cortex.  Psychol Sci 2007;18 (4) 298- 302PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Haynes  JDDriver  JRees  G Visibility reflects dynamic changes of effective connectivity between V1 and fusiform cortex.  Neuron 2005;46 (5) 811- 821PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Green  MFGlahn  DEngel  SANuechterlein  KHSabb  FStrojwas  MCohen  MS Regional brain activity associated with visual backward masking.  J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17 (1) 13- 23PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders—Patient Edition.  New York Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute1997;
36.
First  MBGibbon  MSpitzer  RLWilliams  JBWBenjamin  L Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders.  New York Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute1996;
37.
Buxton  R-BLiu  TTMartinez  AFrank  LRLuh  WMWong  E-C Sorting out event-related paradigms in fMRI: the distinction between detecting an activation and estimating the hemodynamic response.  Neuroimage 2000;11S457Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Liu  TT Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related fMRI with multiple trial types, II: design of experiments.  Neuroimage 2004;21 (1) 401- 413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Liu  TTFrank  LR Efficiency, power, and entropy in event-related FMRI with multiple trial types, part I: theory.  Neuroimage 2004;21 (1) 387- 400PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Dale  AM Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI.  Hum Brain Mapp 1999;8 (2-3) 109- 114PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Wynn  JKGreen  MFEngel  SKorb  ALee  JGlahn  DNuechterlein  KHCohen  MS Increased extent of object-sensitivity cortex in schizophrenia.  Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2008;16497- 105Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Malach  RReppas  JBBenson  RRKwong  KKJiang  HKennedy  WALedden  PJBrady  TJRosen  BRTootell  RB Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1995;92 (18) 8135- 8139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Grill-Spector  KMalach  R The human visual cortex.  Annu Rev Neurosci 2004;27649- 677PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Malach  RLevy  IHasson  U The topography of high-order human object areas.  Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6 (4) 176- 184PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Engel  SAGlover  GHWandell  BA Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI.  Cereb Cortex 1997;7 (2) 181- 192PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hasson  ULevy  IBehrmann  MHendler  TMalach  R Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas.  Neuron 2002;34 (3) 479- 490PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Butler  PDSilverstein  SMDakin  SC Visual perception and its impairment in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;64 (1) 40- 47PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Doniger  GMFoxe  JJMurray  MMHiggins  BAJavitt  DC Impaired visual object recognition and dorsal/ventral stream interaction in schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (11) 1011- 1020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Foxe  JJMurray  MMJavitt  DC Filling-in in schizophrenia: a high-density electrical mapping and source-analysis investigation of illusory contour processing.  Cereb Cortex 2005;15 (12) 1914- 1927PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Vinberg  JGrill-Spector  K Representation of shapes, edges, and surfaces across multiple cues in the human visual cortex.  J Neurophysiol 2008;99 (3) 1380- 1393PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Enns  JTDi Lollo  VDi Lollo V, What's new in visual masking?  Trends Cogn Sci 2000;4 (9) 345- 352PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Enns  JT Object substitution and its relation to other forms of visual masking.  Vision Res 2004;44 (12) 1321- 1331PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Merigan  WHMaunsell  JH How parallel are the primate visual pathways?  Annu Rev Neurosci 1993;16369- 402PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×