Context
High neuroticism is a personality risk factor that reflects much of the genetic vulnerability to major depressive disorder (MDD), and low extraversion may increase risk as well. Both have been linked to the serotonin system.
Objectives
To test whether patients with MDD taking selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) report greater changes in neuroticism and extraversion than patients receiving inert placebo, and to examine the state effect hypothesis that self-reported personality change during SSRI treatment is merely a change of depression-related measurement bias.
Design
A placebo-controlled trial.
Setting
Research clinics.
Patients
Adult patients with moderate to severe MDD randomized to receive paroxetine (n = 120), placebo (n = 60), or cognitive therapy (n = 60).
Outcome Measures
NEO Five-Factor Inventory and Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression.
Results
Patients who took paroxetine reported greater personality change than placebo patients, even after controlling for depression improvement (neuroticism, P < .001; extraversion, P = .002). The advantage of paroxetine over placebo in antidepressant efficacy was no longer significant after controlling for change in neuroticism (P = .46) or extraversion (P = .14). Patients taking paroxetine reported 6.8 times as much change on neuroticism and 3.5 times as much change on extraversion as placebo patients matched for depression improvement. Although placebo patients exhibited substantial depression improvement (Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression score, −1.2 SD, P < .001), they reported little change on neuroticism (−0.18 SD, P = .08) or extraversion (0.08 SD, P = .50). Cognitive therapy produced greater personality change than placebo (P ≤ .01); but its advantage on neuroticism was no longer significant after controlling for depression (P = .14). Neuroticism reduction during treatment predicted lower relapse rates among paroxetine responders (P = .003) but not among cognitive therapy responders (P = .86).
Conclusions
Paroxetine appears to have a specific pharmacological effect on personality that is distinct from its effect on depression. If replicated, this pattern would disconfirm the state effect hypothesis and instead support the notion that SSRIs' effects on personality go beyond and perhaps contribute to their antidepressant effects.
Neuroticism and extraversion are 2 of the 5 primary personality dimensions in the Five-Factor Model of Personality.1-3 Neuroticism refers to a tendency to experience negative emotions and emotional instability; extraversion encompasses social extraversion, dominance, and a tendency to experience positive emotions.1-3 Neuroticism and extraversion are largely independent constructs, as evidenced by correlations between them of −0.28 and −0.38 in 2 normative samples.4
Results from longitudinal studies have consistently shown that neuroticism predicts both the onset and the chronicity of major depressive disorder (MDD).5-11 For example, in a study of more than 800 children, precursors of neuroticism measured at age 3 years predicted whether these children would develop MDD at age 21 years.11 In some longitudinal studies, lower levels of extraversion have also predicted the onset of MDD,9,12 but this pattern has not been replicated in other studies.8,10
Neuroticism and extraversion are both moderately heritable, with genetic factors determining 50% to 60% of their variance.2,13 Twin studies have found substantial overlap in the genetic factors that are associated with high neuroticism and those that predispose persons to MDD.10,14-17 Neuroticism, therefore, appears to reflect much of the genetic vulnerability to MDD.10,16,17
Research on medical disorders with known causes often focuses on the impact of treatments on these causal factors. Despite the accumulating evidence associating neuroticism and extraversion with a causal path to MDD, little research exists on how selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) treatment affects these personality risk factors. Patients have reported that SSRIs make them less reactive to stress, less sensitive to rejection, and more outgoing and vivacious.18-23 These descriptions are consistent with decreases in neuroticism and increases in extraversion, but these reports have not come from patients in double-blind, placebo-controlled studies.18-22 In the one published placebo-controlled study of change in neuroticism during SSRI treatment of MDD, Agosti and McGrath24 did not find that patients taking fluoxetine reported significantly greater neuroticism reduction than placebo patients. However, fewer than half of the patients in that study (42%) completed both treatment and assessment, making these findings difficult to interpret. Thus, it remains unclear whether neuroticism and extraversion change in response to SSRI treatment of MDD above and beyond their natural history and the placebo effect.
When change in personality is reported during SSRI treatment, it is usually assumed to reflect nothing more than the state effect of depression on personality measurement.21,25,26 When in depressive episodes, patients often describe themselves as more neurotic and less extroverted than they are in their intermorbid or premorbid states. This is consistent with the idea that there is a state effect of depression on personality measurement.5,6,8 The state effect hypothesis holds that reported personality change during SSRI treatment is just change in state effect: as depression improves, the state effect of depression will decline, and personality measurement will change as a consequence.21,25,26 This hypothesis thus regards reported personality change as an inconsequential byproduct of depression improvement. The main evidence for this hypothesis is the finding that personality change correlates with depression improvement during SSRI treatment.18,21
Two studies of healthy subjects without current MDD reported findings that are inconsistent with the state effect hypothesis. In both studies, SSRIs were found to influence behaviors and affects that are closely associated with neuroticism and extraversion.27,28 Recent findings from neuroscience investigations also support associations between these personality dimensions and the serotonin system. For example, molecular genetic and positron emission tomography studies have associated neuroticism with serotonin receptor polymorphisms and binding.29-31 Individual studies of neuroticism and the functional polymorphic variants of the serotonin transporter gene have yielded mixed results, but 2 meta-analyses of these studies have converged on the conclusion that the link is statistically significant.32,33 Moreover, the serotonin system has been implicated as a substrate for behaviors related to extraversion in numerous animal and human studies.27,28,34-36 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment, which targets the serotonin system, might thus affect neuroticism and extraversion directly, in a manner that does not depend on its antidepressant effects.
Cognitive therapy (CT), a leading psychosocial treatment of MDD, has been shown to reduce depression symptom severity to a degree similar to that produced by SSRIs.37-39 While some studies of CT have examined pretreatment personality as predictors of outcome,40,41 very little theoretical or empirical work exists on how CT might impact neuroticism and extraversion. Although the state effect hypothesis might also apply to personality changes reported in CT, it is also plausible that CT changes personality through cognitive or behavioral pathways. For example, CT therapists routinely encourage socially withdrawn patients to seek out social interactions and to test whether these interactions are as unpleasant as they predict. Cognitive therapy also teaches patients to challenge internal and stable negative attributions, such as “I am an inferior person.” Such interventions, if successful, might result in changes in neuroticism and extraversion.
This project examined self-reported personality changes in a randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trial of MDD. In the trial, 240 patients were randomized such that 60 patients received placebo for 8 weeks, 120 patients received paroxetine for 16 weeks, and 60 patients received CT for 16 weeks.37 Our first goal was to test whether patients treated with either paroxetine or CT reported greater changes in neuroticism and extraversion at week 8 relative to patients taking placebo.
If paroxetine or CT was observed to produce significantly greater personality change than placebo, our second goal was to test whether such effects could be attributed to between-group differences in depression improvement. We examined this in 2 ways: (1) a linear regression that accounted for depression improvement statistically, and (2) a matching analysis in which patients taking placebo were compared with patients taking paroxetine matched on amount of depression improvement. The state effect hypothesis would predict that the between-group differences in personality change would be largely eliminated in both of these analyses.
Our third goal was to examine the state-effect hypothesis by analyzing data obtained from the patients assigned to 8 weeks of treatment with placebo. In studies of MDD treatments, placebo patients tend to experience substantial improvement in depression.42-46 Thus, their personality data provide a direct test of the state effect hypothesis, which predicts that these placebo patients should also report substantial change in personality. Because this test is not confounded by the effects of active treatments, it might be the purest method to test this hypothesis. Moreover, during the 8 weeks that followed the placebo phase, 31 patients treated with placebo accepted the offer of free subsequent treatment with an SSRI. This set up a within-subject comparison between their placebo phase and SSRI phase. The state effect hypothesis predicts that the magnitude of reported personality change during each phase should be roughly proportional to the magnitude of depression improvement during that same phase.
Our final goal was to examine whether reported neuroticism reduction during SSRI (or CT) treatment predicted subsequent relapse, since neuroticism is a key risk factor that reflects much of the genetic cause of MDD. If reported neuroticism reduction during active treatment was merely a change in the state effect of depression, then it should have no long-term clinical consequences.
Institutional review boards approved the clinical trial protocol, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were 240 moderate to severely depressed adult outpatients; patient characteristics, as well as the procedures of the trial, have been detailed elsewhere.37,47 All patients met criteria for MDD and scored 20 or higher at both screening and intake evaluations on the Hamilton Scale for Depression (HAM-D).42 Inclusion criteria were (1) DSM-IV MDD diagnosis48; (2) age 18 to 70 years; (3) fluency in English; and (4) willingness and ability to give informed consent. Exclusion criteria were (1) history of bipolar I disorder; (2) substance abuse or dependence judged to require treatment; (3) current or past psychosis; (4) another DSM-IV Axis I disorder judged to require priority treatment; (5) antisocial, borderline, and/or schizotypal disorders (all other Axis II disorders were permitted); (6) suicide risk requiring immediate hospitalization; (7) a medical condition that contraindicated study medications; or (8) nonresponse to an adequate trial of paroxetine in the preceding year.
The trial randomized 60 patients into the CT group, 120 patients into the paroxetine group, and 60 patients into the placebo group (Figure 1). The patients who dropped out or who did not complete the personality assessments did not differ significantly from the other patients at intake on depression severity, neuroticism, or extraversion.
For patients in the paroxetine or CT conditions, response was defined in terms of HAM-D scores using the following criteria: (1) week 16 HAM-D score of 12 or less, and either week 14 HAM-D score of 14 or less or week 10 and week 12 HAM-D scores of 12 or less; or (2) week 12, week 14, and week 18 HAM-D scores of 12 or less.37 These criteria prevented a transient exacerbation of depressive symptoms at week 14 or 16 from precluding a patient as being recognized as a responder.
After acute treatment, responders to paroxetine or CT entered the 12-month continuation phase (Figure 1). Paroxetine responders were randomly assigned to 2 subgroups: 34 continued taking medication at the same dosage, and 35 were withdrawn to placebo pills. The 35 CT responders were allowed 3 booster sessions, scheduled at least 1 month apart. Responders were asked not to pursue depression treatment outside the research protocol during this continuation phase.
The patients who had initially been assigned to placebo completed participation in the study at week 8. Thirty-one of these placebo patients then opted for subsequent SSRI treatment (Figure 1).
Personality variables were assessed with the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI), a widely used self-report measure based on the Five-Factor Model of Personality,3 the dominant paradigm in current personality research.1-3 The NEO-FFI has been validated by spouse and peer ratings,3 and 3-month test-retest reliability coefficients of 0.79 have been reported for both neuroticism and extraversion.3 The NEO-FFI scales have well-established normative sample mean scores (neuroticism: mean, 19.1; standard deviation [SD], 7.7; extraversion: mean, 27.7; SD, 5.8).3 In this article, SD for neuroticism and extraversion refers to the SD estimates obtained in the normative sample.
Depression was measured with the 17-item version of the HAM-D, modified to assess atypical symptoms.42 Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation II was the primary means of evaluation during follow-up.49 Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation II tracks whether patients have reentered treatment and whether they had depressive episodes between evaluations.49 Patients met criteria for relapse if (1) they had a HAM-D score of 14 or more for 2 consecutive weeks (3 weeks during the period of medication withdrawal) or (2) if Longitudinal Interval Follow-Up Evaluation II interviews yielded a diagnosis of MDD.
The main effects were tested by regressing week 8 scores against treatment assignment in a standard linear regression, with intake scores as covariates. All main-effect analyses used last available observations as week 8 scores for patients who dropped out or missed assessments. We also conducted analyses restricted to only patients with both intake and week 8 scores, and no notable differences emerged. Effect sizes for main effects were estimated by dividing the difference in the respective least squares mean at week 8 with the pooled SD of the respective mean. They are basically Cohen d for least squares means, and effect sizes above 0.8 can be considered large; between 0.5 and 0.8, medium; and between 0.2 and 0.5, small.50 Unless otherwise specified, multiple regressions results reported reflect the incremental changes attributed to the predictor variable in question and not to the full model. Within-subject comparisons between 2 times were examined with paired t tests. Repeated-measure analysis of variance was used to evaluate whether the placebo patients showed greater changes during their placebo phase than their SSRI phase. Survival curves and relapse rates were estimated using the Cox proportional hazards model,51 and the observations that reflected patients who dropped out, deviated from medication regimen, or sought outside treatment prior to relapse were censored.
In an exploratory analysis, we identified placebo patients for whom we could find a paroxetine patient with matching amounts of depression improvement from intake to week 8. When such a match could not be found for a placebo patient, that patient was dropped. When a placebo patient could be matched with more than 1 paroxetine patient, the paroxetine patient whose intake HAM-D score was closest to the placebo patient's was chosen. If more than 1 match remained, the paroxetine patient with the closest study identification number to that of the placebo patient was chosen. This matching algorithm eliminated subjective choices, and it was unaffected by personality scores. It successfully matched 44 of the 52 placebo patients (85%) with intake and week 8 personality data with 44 paroxetine patients. Rather than conducting paired comparisons of these matched groups, we analyzed for the differences in group means. (While we attempted the same analysis with placebo and CT, we will not detail the results in this article because matching CT patients were found for only 28 of the 52 placebo patients [54%], and results from these 28 pairs were perfectly consistent with the results of regression analyses.)
At intake, the mean neuroticism score was 34 (1.9 SD above the normative sample mean) and the mean extraversion score was 20 (1.3 SD below the normative sample mean). The treatment groups did not differ significantly on neuroticism (P = .27) or extraversion (P = .84) at intake. At week 8, treatment assignment (placebo, paroxetine, and CT) significantly predicted depression severity, neuroticism, and extraversion. The main-effect statistics are presented in Table 1; although no significant differences emerged between the 2 active treatment groups, paroxetine and CT each outperformed placebo in changing depression, neuroticism, and extraversion. Interestingly, in the comparisons with placebo, the effect sizes on depression improvement were smaller than those on neuroticism and extraversion for both paroxetine and CT (Table 1).
Personality change from intake to week 8
Table 2 presents the magnitude of change (in SD) among patients who completed both intake and week 8 personality measurements, and Table 3 compares reported personality change across the 3 groups after accounting for change in depression. Changes reported in all groups are in the direction of normalization: HAM-D scores and neuroticism decreased and extraversion increased. Consistent with prior studies,43-46 placebo patients reported substantial depression improvement (1.2 SD), equal to 75% of the depression improvement shown by patients receiving either CT or paroxetine (1.6 SD each). Contrary to the prediction of the state effect hypothesis, placebo patients reported little change in neuroticism (0.18 SD, P = .08) or extraversion (0.08 SD, P = .50) despite considerable depression improvement.
From intake to week 8, CT patients reported substantial changes on neuroticism and extraversion (Table 2). After accounting for depression improvement, CT patients still reported significantly greater improvement than placebo on extraversion, but the 2 groups' difference on neuroticism was no longer significant (Table 3). Furthermore, after accounting for depression improvement, CT patients reported less change on neuroticism than paroxetine patients, at the level of a nonsignificant trend (Table 3).
Paroxetine patients reported changes in neuroticism and extraversion that were 4 to 8 times as large as the changes reported by placebo patients (Table 2). Regression analyses suggested that such large differences were unlikely to be explained by the modest between-group difference on depression improvement (1.2 SD vs 1.6 SD): after accounting for depression improvement, paroxetine still significantly outperformed placebo in changing both neuroticism and extraversion (Table 3). Furthermore, the difference between paroxetine and placebo in depression reduction was no longer significant after controlling for neuroticism reduction (F1,145 = 0.74, P = .46, effect size = 0.19) or change in extraversion (F1,145 = 1.5, P = .14, effect size = 0.23).
As linear regression approaches might model the nonlinear relationships between depression and personality poorly, we reexamined these variables in the paroxetine and placebo patients in an exploratory matching analysis. For each placebo patient, we attempted to identify a paroxetine patient with the same amount of depression improvement. We found matching paroxetine patients for 44 of the 52 placebo patients with intake and week 8 personality data.
Personality change in these patients from intake to week 8 are presented in Table 4. Although matched on depression improvement (and thus the state effect of depression), these paroxetine patients still reported far greater personality change than placebo patients. On neuroticism, the matched paroxetine patients showed 3.5 times as much reduction as the placebo patients; on extraversion, the matched paroxetine patients showed 6.8 times as much improvement as the placebo patients.
Placebo patients’ within-subject comparison
Figure 2 shows the pattern of change for the 31 placebo patients who opted for subsequent SSRI treatment after week 8. Their HAM-D scores decreased by an average of 6.4 points during the placebo phase (from intake to week 8) compared with 2.4 points during the subsequent SSRI phase (from week 8 to week 16) (Figure 2A). A repeated-measure analysis of variance confirmed that HAM-D scores changed significantly more during the placebo phase than during the subsequent SSRI phase: F2,28 = 9.0, P = .005.
Based on this pattern of depression improvement, the state effect hypothesis predicts that personality measurements should change considerably more during the placebo phase than during the SSRI phase. However, the mean neuroticism score of these patients did not decrease at all during the placebo phase (it increased slightly instead), but it decreased 0.66 SD during the subsequent SSRI phase (Figure 2B). Similarly, their mean extraversion score increased only 0.12 SD during the placebo phase, but it increased 0.34 SD during the subsequent SSRI phase (Figure 2C). Repeated-measures analysis of variance confirmed that neuroticism and extraversion changed less during the placebo phase than during the subsequent SSRI phase (neuroticism, F2,28 = 11.5, P = .002; extraversion, F2,28 = 4.2, P = .03).
Long-term clinical consequence
Among the 69 paroxetine responders, extraversion improvement during acute treatment did not predict relapse rates (χ21 = 0.27, P = .60). However, greater neuroticism reduction predicted a significantly lower likelihood of relapse (χ21 = 8.6, P = .003). This relationship remained statistically significant even after accounting for pretreatment and end-of-treatment HAM-D scores, pretreatment neuroticism, and whether the responder was assigned to active medication or placebo during the continuation phase (χ21 = 12, P < .001). (While medication continuation vs withdrawal to placebo predicted lower relapse rates,47 it did not interact with neuroticism improvement in predicting relapse: χ21 = 0.025, P = .87.) To better understand the strength of this relationship between neuroticism improvement and relapse, we divided the paroxetine responders into 3 subgroups based on the magnitude of their neuroticism reduction: top third, neuroticism reduction by 15 or more points; middle third, reduction of more than 9 points and less than 15 points; and bottom third, reduction of 9 points or less. The relapse rate was 35% in the top third (n = 23), 54% in the middle third (n = 23), and 84% in the bottom third (n = 23). Among the 35 CT responders, personality change during treatment did not predict relapse rates (neuroticism, χ21 = 0.028, P = .86; extraversion, χ21 = 0.12, P = .73).
To the best of our knowledge, these are the first findings from a randomized placebo-controlled trial to suggest that an SSRI treatment of MDD produces greater changes in neuroticism and extraversion than an inert placebo. In other words, paroxetine demonstrated a true drug effect on neuroticism and extraversion scores, reflecting pharmacological specificity.
The state effect hypothesis predicts that differences between medications and placebo in reported personality change should largely disappear after controlling for depression improvement. However, after accounting for depression improvement in regression analyses and in a procedure that matched paroxetine and placebo patients on depression improvement, paroxetine patients still reported far greater personality change than did the placebo patients. This pattern of findings should not occur if personality change conformed to the state effect hypothesis.
The observed course of change in patients who were given placebo for 8 weeks followed by 8 weeks of SSRIs would likewise not be predicted by the state effect hypothesis. Depression reduction was substantial during the placebo phase and was much less so during the paroxetine phase. Personality change showed the opposite pattern. Perhaps the most surprising pattern we observed was the relation between neuroticism reduction during acute SSRI treatment and subsequent resistance to relapse. This finding is not in line with the state effect hypothesis either.
The cause-correction model
If the state effect hypothesis (Figure 3A) proves incorrect in future research, then what alternatives should be considered? One possibility is that the biochemical properties of SSRIs directly produce real personality change. Furthermore, because neuroticism is an important risk factor that captures much of the genetic vulnerability for MDD, change in neuroticism (and in neurobiological factors underlying neuroticism) might have contributed to depression improvement. Indeed, our regression analyses suggest that personality change can explain the advantage of paroxetine over placebo in antidepressant efficacy, rather than vice versa.
These possibilities can be integrated into the cause-correction model (Figure 3B): SSRI treatment produces changes in neuroticism/extraversion and the neurobiological factors underlying them; these changes then contribute to depression improvement. This model could be directly tested by tracking a large sample of premorbid subjects, waiting for them to develop MDD, and then treating them in an SSRI clinical trial. How much SSRI treatment changes personality could then be measured by comparing the premorbid and posttreatment personality scores of responders in full remission. It would be challenging to obtain an adequate sample for such a study: a recent study found that only 262 subjects developed their first episode of MDD after tracking 4263 subjects for 2 years.6
Among responders to paroxetine, those for whom neuroticism changed the most during treatment were also those least likely to relapse. The predictive power of change in neuroticism was not accounted for by pretreatment levels of neuroticism or by posttreatment levels of depressive symptoms. It would appear, then, that change in neuroticism—or in a phenomenon related to self-reported neuroticism—served to reduce vulnerability to relapse. This explanation of the relapse prediction finding is consistent with the cause-correction model. In this model, greater neuroticism reduction reflects more correction of the personality risk factors of depression, which then reduces the risk for relapse. From this perspective, the paroxetine responders who recovered without experiencing much neuroticism reduction resembled the placebo patients in that they might not have benefited much from the biochemical properties of SSRIs. The alternative explanation of the relapse prediction finding is that confounding variables might have influenced both neuroticism and relapse rates; as a result, patients with lower relapse risk also happened to show greater neuroticism improvement during treatment.
The state effect hypothesis exemplifies one causal interpretation of the correlation between depression and personality: depression improvement directly causes personality change. The cause-correction model integrates the 2 other possible causal interpretations, which in the context of our findings would be (1) reverse causation (personality change caused depression improvement) and (2) causation by a third variable (changes in underlying neurobiological factors caused both depression improvement and personality change). However, statistical associations that do not reflect causal relations might also exist among these variables. Depression, neuroticism, and extraversion probably overlap conceptually, which could lead to apparent correlations. For example, if SSRIs primarily change depression, but the definitions of depression and neuroticism overlap extensively, then neuroticism would also appear to change (Figure 3C). Nevertheless, such a conceptual overlap hypothesis cannot explain the dissociations we observed between depression change and neuroticism change with respect to paroxetine and placebo.
This study is also the first clinical trial to show that CT can produce significantly greater change in neuroticism and extraversion than placebo. However, it remains unclear whether this effect reflects personality change, change in state effect, or measurement artifact related to cognitive therapists' explicit efforts to address thoughts and behaviors related to high neuroticism and low extraversion.
Cognitive therapy did not produce greater change in neuroticism than placebo after controlling for change in depression, and neuroticism improvement during CT did not predict subsequent relapse as it did among paroxetine responders. These findings suggest that the nature of reported neuroticism reduction in CT might differ from that in SSRI treatment and that it might be more closely associated with depression improvement. The fact that rates of subsequent relapse were relatively low in CT regardless of the amount of change in neuroticism suggests that it works through mechanisms other than neuroticism to produce its enduring effect.47
On extraversion, CT outperformed placebo even after controlling for depression improvement. This suggests that extraversion improvement in CT may be independent of depression improvement, which contradicts the state effect hypothesis (and the conceptual overlap hypothesis). Cognitive therapy showed no significant differences with paroxetine on extraversion, even after controlling for depression. However, the lack of difference between 2 active treatments on a process variable is often uninformative in testing mechanism models.52 The lack of difference between CT and paroxetine on extraversion can be consistent with the state effect hypothesis, the conceptual overlap hypothesis, and even the cause-correction hypothesis. For example, if both CT and paroxetine first change extraversion, which then cause depression to improve (ie, the cause-correction model), then the 2 treatments may still appear similar on extraversion improvement. (Presumably, paroxetine and CT would change extraversion via distinct pathways.53)
The present findings need to be replicated with paroxetine, other SSRIs, and other antidepressant medications to determine whether the effects we observed are reliable and whether they are limited to the medication or medication class that we tested.18 In previous research on the effects of antidepressant medications, other temperament variables such as harm avoidance, novelty seeking, and reward dependence54 have been used. It remains unclear how these variables relate to the personality dimensions of the Five-Factor Model of Personality. In addition, because this clinical trial measured personality with the more abbreviated NEO-FFI, rather than the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, we could not distinguish the different facets of neuroticism and extraversion.3 We also did not investigate which underlying neurobiological system is involved in the cause-correction model. While the serotonin system is a natural candidate, other possibilities should be carefully considered in future research as well. Furthermore, although our main-effects analyses were obtained in the context of a placebo-controlled randomized clinical trial, our secondary analyses were nonexperimental and may therefore be open to alternative interpretations. Finally, there were a number of dropouts in this trial, and the sample sizes for some analyses were modest.
In this study, patients with MDD reported substantial personality change during SSRI treatment; such personality change was not dependent upon depression improvement; and it might have contributed to acute and long-term treatment outcomes. Although SSRIs are the most widely used treatment of MDD, our understanding of their mechanisms remains limited.55 Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors are also effective in treating several anxiety disorders and eating disorders,56,57 and recent studies have suggested that high neuroticism and low extraversion may be general risk factors for these disorders as well.5,58,59 Investigating how SSRIs affect neuroticism and extraversion may thus lead toward a more parsimonious understanding of the mechanisms of SSRIs.
Correspondence: Tony Z. Tang, PhD, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheridan Rd, Evanston, IL 60208 (ttang@northwestern.edu).
Submitted for Publication: August 4, 2008; final revision received April 23, 2009; accepted April 23, 2009.
Author Contributions: Dr Tang, who is independent of any commercial funder, performed the statistical analysis for this study; he had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: The data set of this study came from a clinical trial supported by grants MH50129 R10, MH55875 R10, and MH01697 K02 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland. GlaxoSmithKline of Brentford, England, provided medications and placebo pills.
1.Digman
JM Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor model.
Annu Rev Psychol 1990;41417- 440
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Costa
PTMcCrae
RR Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five-Factor Inventory: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc1992;
4.McCrae
RRCosta
PT A contemplated revision of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory.
Pers Individ Dif 2004;36587- 596
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Clark
LAWatson
DMineka
S Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders.
J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103
(1)
103- 116
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Ormel
JOldehinkel
AJVollebergh
W Vulnerability before, during, and after a major depressive episode: a 3-wave population-based study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61
(10)
990- 996
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Kendler
KSKuhn
JPrescott
CA The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of major depression.
Am J Psychiatry 2004;161
(4)
631- 636
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Kendler
KSNeale
MCKessler
RCHeath
ACEaves
LJ A longitudinal twin study of personality and major depression in women.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50
(11)
853- 862
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Krueger
RFCaspi
AMoffitt
TESilva
PAMcGee
R Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: a multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort.
J Abnorm Psychol 1996;105
(3)
299- 312
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Kendler
KSGatz
MGardner
COPedersen
NL Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63
(10)
1113- 1120
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Caspi
AMoffitt
TENewman
DLSilva
PA Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53
(11)
1033- 1039
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Hirschfeld
RMKlerman
GLLavori
PKeller
MBGriffith
PCoryell
W Premorbid personality assessments of first onset of major depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46
(4)
345- 350
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Tellegen
ALykken
DTBouchard
TJ
JrWilcox
KJSegal
NLRich
S Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;54
(6)
1031- 1039
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Jardine
RMartin
NGHenderson
AS Genetic covariation between neuroticism and the symptoms of anxiety and depression.
Genet Epidemiol 1984;1
(2)
89- 107
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Kendler
KSWalters
EENeale
MCKessler
RCHeath
ACEaves
LJ The structure of the genetic and environmental risk factors for six major psychiatric disorders in women: phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, bulimia, major depression, and alcoholism.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995;52
(5)
374- 383
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Kendler
KSGardner
COGatz
MPedersen
NL The sources of co-morbidity between major depression and generalized anxiety disorder in a Swedish national twin sample.
Psychol Med 2007;37
(3)
453- 462
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Hettema
JMNeale
MCMyers
JMPrescott
CAKendler
KS A population-based twin study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders.
Am J Psychiatry 2006;163
(5)
857- 864
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Bagby
RMLevitan
RDKennedy
SHLevitt
AJJoffe
RT Selective alteration of personality in response to noradrenergic and serotonergic antidepressant medication in depressed sample: evidence of non-specificity.
Psychiatry Res 1999;86
(3)
211- 216
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Ekselius
Lvon Knorring
L Changes in personality traits during treatment with sertraline or citalopram.
Br J Psychiatry 1999;174444- 448
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Brody
ALSaxena
SFairbanks
LAAlborzian
SDemaree
HAMaidment
KMBaxter
LR
Jr Personality changes in adult subjects with major depressive disorder or obsessive-compulsive disorder treated with paroxetine.
J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61
(5)
349- 355
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Du
LBakish
DRavindran
AVHrdina
PD Does fluoxetine influence major depression by modifying five-factor personality traits?
J Affect Disord 2002;71
(1-3)
235- 241
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Santor
DABagby
RMJoffe
RT Evaluating stability and change in personality and depression.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;73
(6)
1354- 1362
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Kramer
PD Listening to Prozac. New York, NY Viking1993;
24.Agosti
VMcGrath
PJ Comparison of the effects of fluoxetine, imipramine and placebo on personality in atypical depression.
J Affect Disord 2002;71
(1-3)
113- 120
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Knutson
BWolkowitz
OMCole
SWChan
TMoore
EAJohnson
RCTerpstra
JTurner
RAReus
VI Selective alteration of personality and social behavior by serotonergic intervention.
Am J Psychiatry 1998;155
(3)
373- 379
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Melke
JWestberg
LNilsson
SLanden
MSoderstrom
HBaghaei
FRosmond
RHolm
GBjörntorp
PNilsson
LGAdolfsson
REriksson
E A polymorphism in the serotonin receptor 3A (HTR3A) gene and its association with harm avoidance in women.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60
(10)
1017- 1023
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Strobel
AGutknecht
LRothe
CReif
AMössner
RZeng
YBrocke
BLesch
KP Allelic variation in 5-HT1A receptor expression is associated with anxiety- and depression-related personality traits.
J Neural Transm 2003;110
(12)
1445- 1453
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Tauscher
JBagby
RMJavanmard
MChristensen
BKKasper
SKapur
S Inverse relationship between serotonin 5-HT(1A) receptor binding and anxiety: a [(11)C]WAY-100635 PET investigation in healthy volunteers.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158
(8)
1326- 1328
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Sen
SBurmeister
MGhosh
D Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2004;127B
(1)
85- 89
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Schinka
JABusch
RMRobichaux-Keene
N A meta-analysis of the association between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and trait anxiety.
Mol Psychiatry 2004;9
(2)
197- 202
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Young
SNLeyton
M The role of serotonin in human mood and social interaction: insight from altered tryptophan levels.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2002;71
(4)
857- 865
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Huber
RPanksepp
JBYue
ZDelago
AMoore
P Dynamic interactions of behavior and amine neurochemistry in acquisition and maintenance of social rank in crayfish.
Brain Behav Evol 2001;57
(5)
271- 282
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.DeRubeis
RJHollon
SDAmsterdam
JDShelton
RCYoung
PRSalomon
RMO’Reardon
JPLovett
MLGladis
MMBrown
LLGallop
R Cognitive therapy vs medications in the treatment of moderate to severe depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(4)
409- 416
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Jarrett
RBSchaffer
MMcIntire
DWitt-Browder
AKraft
DRisser
RC Treatment of atypical depression with cognitive therapy or phenelzine: a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999;56
(5)
431- 437
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.DeRubeis
RJGelfand
LATang
TZSimons
AD Medications versus cognitive behavior therapy for severely depressed outpatients: mega-analysis of four randomized comparisons.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;156
(7)
1007- 1013
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Quilty
LCDe Fruyt
FRolland
JPKennedy
SHRouillon
PFBagby
RM Dimensional personality traits and treatment outcome in patients with major depressive disorder.
J Affect Disord 2008;108
(3)
241- 250
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Bagby
RMQuilty
LCSegal
ZVMcBride
CCKennedy
SHCosta
PT Personality and differential treatment response in major depression: a randomized controlled trial comparing cognitive-behavioural therapy and pharmacotherapy.
Can J Psychiatry 2008;53
(6)
361- 370
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Khan
AWarner
HABrown
WA Symptom reduction and suicide risk in patients treated with placebo in antidepressant clinical trials: an analysis of the Food and Drug Administration database.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57
(4)
311- 317
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Greenberg
RPBornstein
RZborowski
MJFisher
SGreenberg
ME A meta-analysis of fluoxetine outcome in the treatment of depression.
J Nerv Ment Dis 1994;182
(10)
547- 551
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Kirsch
IMoore
TJScoboria
ANicholls
SS The emperor's new drugs: an analysis of antidepressant medication data submitted to the U.S. Food and Drug Administration.
Prev Treat 2002;5
Google Scholar 46.Kirsch
ISapirstein
G Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: a meta-analysis of antidepressant medication.
Prev Treat 1998;1
Google Scholar 47.Hollon
SDDeRubeis
RJShelton
RCAmsterdam
JDSalomon
RMO’Reardon
JPLovett
MLYoung
PRHaman
KLFreeman
BBGallop
R Prevention of relapse following cognitive therapy vs medications in moderate to severe depression.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(4)
417- 422
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.American Psychological Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC American Psychological Association1994;
49.Keller
MBLavori
PWFriedman
BNielsen
EEndicott
JMcDonald-Scott
PAndreasen
NC The longitudinal interval follow-up evaluation: a comprehensive method for assessing outcome in prospective longitudinal studies.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44
(6)
540- 548
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Cohen
J Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1988;
51.Cox
DROakes
D Analysis of Survival Data. London, England Chapman & Hall1984;
52.Hollon
SDDeRubeis
RJEvans
MD Causal mediation of change in treatment for depression: discriminating between nonspecificity and noncausality.
Psychol Bull 1987;102
(1)
139- 149
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Goldapple
KSegal
ZGarson
CLau
MBieling
PKennedy
SMayberg
H Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61
(1)
34- 41
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Hellerstein
DJKocsis
JHChapman
DStewart
JWHarrison
W Double-blind comparison of sertraline, imipramine, and placebo in the treatment of dysthymia: effects on personality.
Am J Psychiatry 2000;1571436- 1444
Google ScholarCrossref 55. Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with major depressive disorder (revision): American Psychiatric Association.
Am J Psychiatry 2000;157
(4)
((suppl))
1- 45
PubMedGoogle Scholar 56.White
KSBarlow
DH Panic disorder and agoraphobia. Barlow
DH
Anxiety and Its Disorders. 2nd ed. New York, NY Guilford2002;328- 379
Google Scholar 57.Walsh
BT Pharmacological treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Fairburn
CGBrownell
KD
Eating Disorders and Obesity: A Comprehensive Handbook. 2nd ed. New York, NY Guilford2002;325- 329
Google Scholar 58.Jorm
AFChristensen
HHenderson
ASJacomb
PAKorten
AERodgers
B Predicting anxiety and depression from personality: is there a synergistic effect of neuroticism and extraversion?
J Abnorm Psychol 2000;109
(1)
145- 149
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Wade
TDBulik
CMPrescott
CAKendler
KS Sex influences on shared risk factors for bulimia nervosa and other psychiatric disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61
(3)
251- 256
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref